INTRODUCTION

‘Anticholinergic’ muscarinic antagonists such as scopolamine or atropine may evoke a psychotic state termed ‘anticholinergic syndrome’ or ‘antimuscarinic psychosis’. Noteworthy, in comparison to the dopamine (DA)-releasing psychotomimetic amphetamine, which induces psychosis characterized by hallucinations and delusions (Snyder, 1973), antimuscarinic-induced psychosis includes in addition disorganized thinking, attentional impairments, and delirium, characteristics of endogenous schizophrenia (Clarke et al, 2004; Fisher, 1991; Holland, 1992; Marchlewski, 1994; Mego et al, 1988; Minzenberg et al, 2004; Perry and Perry, 1995; Perry et al, 1978; Wilkinson, 1987; Yeomans, 1995). Antimuscarinic psychosis can be alleviated by antipsychotic drugs (APDs) (Gopel et al, 2002; Perry et al, 1978) as well as by acetylcholinesterase (AChE) inhibitors (Brown et al, 2004; Gopel et al, 2002; Granacher and Baldessarini, 1975; Nogue et al, 1991; Perry et al, 1978). Conversely, muscarinic antagonists used to reduce extrapyramidal side effects associated with APDs (Tandon, 1999) have been reported to exacerbate schizophrenia symptoms and to interfere with the therapeutic effects of APDs (Johnstone et al, 1983; Lo and Tsai, 1996; Singh and Kay, 1975, 1979; Tandon et al, 1990). These findings, taken together with postmortem and neuroimaging findings of cholinergic alterations in the brains of schizophrenia patients (eg Crook et al, 2001; Dean et al, 1996; Garcia-Rill et al, 1995; Karson et al, 1991; Raedler et al, 2003; Zavitsanou et al, 2005), have led to a growing interest in the involvement of the cholinergic system in this disorder (eg Hyde and Crook, 2001; Sarter et al, 2005; Tandon et al, 1992; Weiner et al, 2004; Yeomans, 1995). The focus on the cholinergic system has been reinforced by the increasingly acknowledged need for improved treatments of cognitive deficits in schizophrenia (eg Bymaster et al, 2002; Friedman, 2004).

Consistent with findings in humans, systemic administration of nonspecific muscarinic antagonists, such as atropine or scopolamine, has been shown to induce psychotic-like effects in several animal models of schizophrenia, including locomotor hyperactivity (Mathur et al, 1997; Shannon and Peters, 1990; Sipos et al, 1999), stereotypy (Mathur et al, 1997), and disruption of prepulse inhibition (PPI) (Jones et al, 2005; Jones and Shannon, 2000a, 2000b; Ukai et al, 2004; Wu et al, 1993), and these effects were reversed by cholinomimetic drugs, such as the AChE inhibitor physostigmine (Jones and Shannon, 2000b; Shannon and Peters, 1990), as well as APDs (Jones et al, 2005; Shannon and Peters, 1990).

To date, there has not been a systematic investigation of the effects of muscarinic antagonists on latent inhibition (LI), a well-established model of schizophrenia. LI is a cross-species phenomenon manifested as a poorer conditioning to a stimulus that is seen when the stage of conditioning is preceded by a stage of repeated nonreinforced pre-exposure to that stimulus. LI is commonly considered to index the ability to ignore stimuli that predicted no significant consequences in the past and has been used extensively to model cognitive impairments in schizophrenia (Weiner, 1990, 2000, 2003). It has been suggested that LI stems from the reduced attention to, or the associability of, the pre-exposed (PE) stimulus, which reduces the effectiveness with which it enters into an association with reinforcement (Lubow et al, 1981; Lubow, 1989). An alternative explanation posits that the acquisition of an association between the PE CS and the absence of a significant consequence during pre-exposure interferes with the subsequent expression (Gray et al, 1995a; Weiner, 1990, 2003) or retrieval (Bouton, 1993) of the CS-reinforcement association. LI is disrupted in rats and mice by amphetamine (eg Killcross and Robbins, 1993; Meyer et al, 2004; Moran et al, 1996; Weiner et al, 1984, 1988) and this is paralleled by disrupted LI in amphetamine-treated normal humans (Gray et al, 1992b; Salgado et al, 2000; Swerdlow et al, 2003; Thornton et al, 1996) and in acute schizophrenia patients (Baruch et al, 1988; Gray et al, 1992a, 1995b; Rascle et al, 2001; but also see Swerdlow et al, 2005). The LI model is further validated by its sensitivity to APDs, which reverse amphetamine-induced disruption of LI and potentiate the phenomenon under conditions that do not suffice to yield it in no-drug controls, such as low number of pre-exposures (Gosselin et al, 1996; Shadach et al, 1999; Warburton et al, 1994; Weiner et al, 1996).

Pharmacological studies of LI using cholinergic compounds in rats and humans used primarily nicotinic agents and have yielded inconsistent findings (Della Casa et al, 1999; Gould et al, 2001; Gray et al, 1997; Joseph et al, 1993; Rochford et al, 1996; Thornton et al, 1996). To the best of our knowledge, few studies testing the effects of muscarinic manipulations on LI have been published to date. Moore et al (1976) found that scopolamine injected in both pre-exposure and conditioning did not affect LI in rabbits. However, both systemic (Carlton and Vogel, 1965) and intra-insular cortex (Naor and Dudai, 1996) injection of scopolamine confined to the pre-exposure stage disrupted LI in rats. Finally, LI was shown to be enhanced in M5 mutant mice (Wang et al, 2004).

The present study tested whether scopolamine would disrupt LI and whether such disruption would be reversed by APDs. As scopolamine is known to induce memory and learning deficits, which could mask its effect on LI, we used low doses of scopolamine previously shown to spare associative learning (Anagnostaras et al, 1999). In addition, because the effects of propsychotic and antipsychotic compounds on LI depend on whether they are administered in pre-exposure or conditioning (Weiner, 2003), we sought to determine the stage at which scopolamine and APDs act to produce LI disruption and restoration, respectively. Specifically, Experiment 1 tested the effects of 0.15, 0.5, and 1 mg/kg scopolamine given in both stages on LI, and Experiment 2 tested at what stage of the LI procedure scopolamine acts to produce LI disruption. Experiments 3 and 4 tested the effects of the typical APD haloperidol and the atypical APD clozapine, respectively, on scopolamine-induced LI disruption and at which stage these drugs acted. In addition to APDs, we tested in Experiment 5 the effects of physostigmine on scopolamine-induced LI disruption. Finally, Experiment 6 tested the capacity of the low doses of scopolamine found to disrupt LI to produce locomotor hyperactivity. For purposes of comparison, amphetamine was included in Experiments 1, 5, and 6.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Subjects

Male Wistar rats (Tel Aviv University Medical School, Tel Aviv), 3–4 months old and weighing 280–460 g, were housed four to a cage under reversed cycle lighting (lights on: 1900–0700 h) with ad lib food and water except for the duration of the LI experiments (see below). All experimental protocols conformed to the guidelines of the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of Tel Aviv University, Israel, and to the guidelines of the NIH (animal welfare assurance number A5010–01, expires on November 30, 2006). All efforts were made to minimize the number of animals used and their suffering.

Apparatus and Procedure

LI

LI was measured in a thirst-motivated, conditioned emotional response (CER) procedure by comparing the suppression of drinking to a tone previously paired with a foot shock in rats that received nonreinforced exposure to the tone prior to conditioning (PE) and in rats for whom the tone was novel (non-pre-exposed (NPE)).

Rats were tested in Campden Instruments rodent test chambers (model 410) with a retractable bottle. When the bottle was not present, the hole was covered by a metal lid. Licks were detected by a Campden Instruments drinkometer (model 435). The PE to-be-conditioned stimulus was a 10 s, 80 dB, 2.8 kHz tone produced by a Sonalert module (model SC 628). Shock was supplied through the floor by a Campden Instruments shock generator (model 521/C) and shock scrambler (model 521/S) set at 0.5 mA and 1 s duration. Equipment programming and data recording were computer controlled.

Prior to the beginning of each LI experiment, rats were handled for about 2 min daily for 5 days. A 23 h water restriction schedule was initiated simultaneously with handling and continued throughout the experiment. On the next 5 days, rats were trained to drink in the experimental chamber for 20 min during the 1st day and for 15 min/day during each of the next 4 days. Water in the test apparatus was given in addition to the daily ration of 1 h given in the home cages. The LI procedure was conducted on days 11–14 and consisted of the following stages:

Pre-exposure (day 11) With the bottle removed, the PE rats received 40 tone presentations with an inter-stimulus interval of 40 s. The NPE rats were confined to the chamber for an identical period of time without receiving the tone.

Conditioning (day 12) With the bottle removed, each rat received two tone-shock pairings given 5 min apart. Shock immediately followed tone termination. The first tone-shock pairing was given 5 min after the start of the session. After the last pairing, rats were left in the experimental chamber for an additional 5 min.

Rebaseline (day 13) Rats were given a 15 min drinking session as in initial training. Data of rats that failed to complete 600 licks were dropped from the analysis.

Test (day 14) Each rat was placed in the chamber and allowed to drink from the bottle. When the rat completed 75 licks, the tone was presented for 5 min. The following times were recorded: time to first lick, time to complete licks 1–50, time to complete licks 51–75 (before tone onset), and time to complete licks 76–100 (after tone onset). Times to complete licks 76–100 were submitted to logarithmic transformation to allow parametric analysis of variance. Longer log times indicate stronger suppression of drinking. LI is defined as significantly shorter log times to complete licks 76–100 of the PE compared to NPE rats.

Spontaneous and drug-induced activity

Activity was measured in plastic chambers (46 × 57 × 37 cm), covered by 50 × 50 cm clear Perspex lids, located in a darkened room. A Coulbourn Instruments infrared sensor unit was installed in the center of a front wall 22 cm from the side walls, and 12 cm above the grid floor. The sensor was protected by a wire fence measuring 10 × 13 × 6 cm to prevent animals’ access. Blind areas of the sensor (the two corners of the triangles adjacent to the sensor, measuring 17 × 17 × 25) were blocked by two clear Perspex walls with dimensions of 25 × 57 cm. The movements detected by the sensor were transmitted through a Coulbourn Instruments eight-channel infrared motion interface to a Coulbourn Instruments infrared motion activity monitor controller/analyzer. Rats were individually placed in the activity chambers and allowed 30 min of free exploration (spontaneous activity), after which they were returned to their home cage, injected with the appropriate drug and replaced into the chamber for an additional 60 min (drug-induced activity). The pre- and post-drug duration of movements for each animal were recorded in 6 min blocks. Data recording was computer controlled.

Drugs

All drugs were administered intraperitoneally in a volume of 1 ml/kg. Scopolamine HBr (Sigma, Israel) was diluted in saline and administered at doses of 0.15, 0.5, or 1 mg/kg. Haloperidol, prepared from an ampoule containing 5 mg haloperidol in 1 ml solvent containing 6 mg lactic acid (Johnson & Johnson, Belgium) and diluted with saline, was administered at a dose of 0.1 mg/kg. Clozapine (Novartis, Switzerland), dissolved in 1 N acetic acid (1.5 ml/10 mg) and diluted with saline, was administered at a dose of 10 mg/kg. Physostigmine (eserine) hemisulfate (Sigma, Israel) was diluted in saline and administered at doses of 0.05 or 0.15 mg/kg. D-amphetamine (Sigma, Israel) was diluted in saline and administered at a dose of 1 mg/kg. The doses of scopolamine were chosen based on studies showing that they did not disrupt tone-shock conditioning (Anagnostaras et al, 1999). The doses of haloperidol, clozapine, and amphetamine were chosen based on our previous LI studies using these drugs (Weiner et al, 1987, 1997, 1996). The doses of physostigmine were chosen based on previous behavioral experiments with this drug (Jones and Shannon, 2000b; Shannon and Peters, 1990). In LI experiments (1–5), all drugs were administered 30 min prior to pre-exposure and/or conditioning, except for haloperidol, which was administered 60 min prior to pre-exposure and/or conditioning. No drug controls received the corresponding vehicle.

Data Analysis

In LI experiments, times to complete licks 50–75 and mean log times to complete licks 76–100 were analyzed in experiments 1–4 using two-way ANOVA with main factors of pre-exposure (0, 40) and treatment (five levels in experiments 1, 3, and 4, and three levels in experiment 2), and in experiment 5, using a three-way ANOVA with main factors of pre-exposure (0, 40), treatment (vehicle, scopolamine, amphetamine), and pretreatment (0, 0.05, and 0.15 mg/kg physostigmine). LSD post hoc comparisons were used to assess the difference between the PE and NPE groups within each drug condition. In locomotor activity experiment (experiment 6), duration of movements was analyzed using a 4 × (3) × (5) ANOVA with main factor of drug and repeated measurements factors of three 30-min periods (1–30 min before injection, 31–60 min after injection, 61–90 min after injection) and five 6-min blocks within each 30-min period.

Experiment 1: Effects of 0.15, 0.5, or 1 mg/kg Scopolamine and 1 mg/kg Amphetamine on LI

The experiment included 10 experimental groups (n per group=11–13) in a 2 × 5 design with main factors of pre-exposure (0, 40) and treatment (vehicle, 0.15 mg/kg scopolamine, 0.5 mg/kg scopolamine, 1 mg/kg scopolamine, 1 mg/kg amphetamine). Both drugs were administered prior to the pre-exposure and the conditioning stages.

Results

The 10 experimental groups did not differ in their times to complete licks 51–75 before tone onset (all p's>0.05; overall mean A period=6.97 s). Figure 1 presents the mean log times to complete licks 76–100 (after tone onset) of the PE and NPE rats injected with vehicle, 0.15 mg/kg scopolamine, 0.5 mg/kg scopolamine, 1 mg/kg scopolamine, or 1 mg/kg amphetamine. As can be seen, LI was present in vehicle-treated rats, but was disrupted by amphetamine as well as by the two lower doses of scopolamine, whereas the highest dose of scopolamine (1 mg/kg) spared LI. ANOVA yielded significant main effects of pre-exposure (F(1,107)=14.01, p<0.0005) and treatment (F(4,107)=5.17, p<0.001), and a significant interaction of pre-exposure × treatment (F(4,107)=2.98, p<0.05). Post hoc comparisons revealed a significant difference between the PE and NPE groups, that is, LI, in rats injected with vehicle (p<0.0001), and 1 mg/kg scopolamine (p<0.05) but not in the other three conditions.

Figure 1
figure 1

Effects of scopolamine on LI. Means and standard errors of the log times to complete licks 76–100 (after tone onset) of the pre-exposed (PE) and non-pre-exposed (NPE) rats injected with vehicle, scopolamine (0.15, 0.5, 1 mg/kg), or amphetamine (amph; 1 mg/kg). Asterisk indicates a significant difference between the PE and NPE groups, namely, presence of LI.

Experiment 2: Effects of 0.15 mg/kg Scopolamine Injected in PE or Conditioning on LI

This experiment sought to determine at which stage of the LI procedure scopolamine acted to produce LI disruption. This was of particular interest because previous studies had shown that amphetamine did not disrupt LI when given in the pre-exposure stage alone (Weiner, 2003; Weiner and Feldon, 1997; Weiner et al, 1984, 1988), but disrupted LI if administered in conditioning (Gray et al, 1997; Joseph et al, 2000). The experiment included six experimental groups (n per group=7–8) in a 2 × 3 design with main factors of pre-exposure (0, 40) and treatment (vehicle, scopolamine in pre-exposure, scopolamine in conditioning).

Results

The six experimental groups did not differ in their times to complete licks 51–75 before tone onset (all p's>0.05; overall mean A period=6.76 s). Figure 2 presents the mean log times to complete licks 76–100 (after tone onset) of the PE and NPE rats injected with vehicle, scopolamine in pre-exposure, or scopolamine in conditioning. As can be seen, LI was present in vehicle-treated rats as well as in rats injected with scopolamine in the conditioning stage alone. In contrast, LI was disrupted following scopolamine administration in the pre-exposure stage alone. ANOVA yielded significant main effects of pre-exposure (F(1,40)=20.187, p<0.0001) and treatment (F(2,40)=5.46, p<0.05), and a nearly significant interaction of treatment × pre-exposure (F(2,40)=2.704, p=0.079). Post hoc comparisons revealed a significant difference between the PE and NPE groups injected with vehicle (p<0.001) and scopolamine in conditioning (p<0.005), but not between those injected with scopolamine in pre-exposure.

Figure 2
figure 2

Effects of scopolamine on LI as a function of stage of administration. Means and standard errors of the log times to complete licks 76–100 (after tone onset) of the pre-exposed (PE) and non-pre-exposed (NPE) vehicle- or scopolamine (0.15 mg/kg)-injected rats. Scopolamine was injected either in the pre-exposure stage or in the conditioning stage. Asterisk indicates a significant difference between the PE and NPE groups, namely, presence of LI.

Experiments 3 and 4: Effects of Haloperidol and Clozapine on Pre-Exposure-Based Scopolamine-Induced LI Disruption

As APDs-induced reversal of disrupted LI is owing to their effects in the conditioning stage (Weiner, 2003), experiments 3 and 4 tested whether the same pattern would be seen with scopolamine-induced LI disruption. This was of particular interest given that scopolamine disrupted LI via effects at the pre-exposure stage, thus raising the possibility that APDs would reverse scopolamine-induced LI disruption not at the stage of the LI procedure at which disruption was induced. Scopolamine was injected in the pre-exposure stage. Haloperidol and clozapine (Experiments 3 and 4, respectively) were injected to scopolamine-treated rats in either the pre-exposure stage, the conditioning stage, or in both stages. We did not administer haloperidol and clozapine on their own, because we had extensively characterized the effects of both drugs on LI in the present procedure in our previous studies. Specifically, we have shown that both drugs have no effect on LI when given in conditioning or in both stages, whereas clozapine, but not haloperidol, can disrupt LI when administered in pre-exposure (Shadach et al, 2000; Weiner, 2003; Weiner et al, 1987, 1997).

Experiment 3: Effects of 0.1 mg/kg Haloperidol Injected in Pre-Exposure and/or Conditioning on Pre-Exposure-Based Scopolamine-Induced LI Disruption

The experiment included 10 experimental groups (n per group=7–10) in a 2 × 5 design with main factors of pre-exposure (0, 40) and treatment (vehicle, scopolamine, scopolamine+haloperidol in pre-exposure, scopolamine+haloperidol in conditioning, scopolamine+haloperidol in both stages). Data of four rats were dropped from the analysis.

Results

The 10 experimental groups did not differ in their times to complete licks 51–75 before tone onset (all p's>0.05; overall mean A period=7.65 s). Figure 3 presents the mean log times to complete licks 76–100 (after tone onset) of the PE and NPE rats injected with vehicle, scopolamine, scopolamine+haloperidol in pre-exposure, scopolamine+haloperidol in conditioning, or scopolamine+haloperidol in both stages. As can be seen, LI was present in vehicle-treated rats and absent in rats that were treated with scopolamine. Haloperidol restored LI in scopolamine-treated rats when given in both pre-exposure and conditioning as well as if given only in conditioning, but failed to restore LI if given in pre-exposure only. ANOVA yielded a significant main effect of pre-exposure (F(1,76)=17.29, p<0.0001), and a significant interaction of treatment × pre-exposure (F(4,76)=2.69, p<0.05). Post hoc comparisons revealed a significant difference between the PE and NPE groups injected with vehicle (p<0.001), scopolamine+haloperidol in conditioning (p<0.001), and scopolamine+haloperidol in both stages (p<0.05), but not between PE and NPE groups that received only scopolamine, or scopolamine+haloperidol in pre-exposure.

Figure 3
figure 3

Effects of haloperidol on scopolamine-induced LI disruption as a function of stage of administration. Means and standard errors of the log times to complete licks 76–100 (after tone onset) of the pre-exposed (PE) and non-pre-exposed (NPE) rats in four drug conditions: vehicle, scopolamine (0.15 mg/kg), scopolamine+haloperidol (hal; 0.1 mg/kg) in pre-exposure, scopolamine+haloperidol in conditioning, and scopolamine+haloperidol in both stages. Scopolamine was injected in the pre-exposure stage. Asterisk indicates a significant difference between the PE and NPE groups, namely, presence of LI.

Experiment 4: Effects of 10 mg/kg Clozapine Injected in Pre-Exposure and/or Conditioning on Scopolamine-Induced LI Disruption

The experiment included 10 experimental groups (n per group=7–10) in a 2 × 5 design with main factors of pre-exposure (0, 40) and treatment (vehicle, scopolamine, scopolamine+clozapine in pre-exposure, scopolamine+clozapine in conditioning, scopolamine+clozapine in both stages). Data of three rats were dropped from the analysis.

Results

The 10 experimental groups did not differ in their times to complete licks 51–75 before tone onset (all p's>0.05; overall mean A period=7.16 s). Figure 4 presents the mean log times to complete licks 76–100 (after tone onset) of the PE and NPE rats injected with vehicle, scopolamine, scopolamine+clozapine in pre-exposure, scopolamine+clozapine in conditioning, or scopolamine+clozapine in both stages. As can be seen, LI was present in vehicle-treated rats and absent in rats treated with scopolamine. Clozapine restored LI in scopolamine-treated rats if given in both pre-exposure and conditioning as well as if given in conditioning only, but failed to restore LI if given in pre-exposure only. ANOVA yielded significant main effects of pre-exposure (F(1,74)=15.97, p<0.0005) and treatment (F(4,74)=5.94, p<0.0005), and an almost significant interaction of treatment × pre-exposure (F(4,74)=2.43, p=0.055). Post hoc comparisons revealed a significant difference between the PE and NPE groups in rats injected with vehicle (p<0.005), scopolamine+clozapine in conditioning (p<0.01), and scopolamine+clozapine in both stages (p<0.05), but not in rats injected with scopolamine or scopolamine+clozapine in pre-exposure.

Figure 4
figure 4

Effects of clozapine on scopolamine-induced LI disruption as a function of stage of administration. Means and standard errors of the log times to complete licks 76–100 (after tone onset) of the pre-exposed (PE) and non-pre-exposed (NPE) rats in four drug conditions: vehicle, scopolamine (0.15 mg/kg), scopolamine+clozapine (cloz; 10 mg/kg) in pre-exposure, scopolamine+clozapine in conditioning, and scopolamine+clozapine in both stages. Scopolamine was injected in the pre-exposure stage. Asterisk indicates a significant difference between the PE and NPE groups, namely, presence of LI.

Experiment 5: Effects of Physostigmine on Scopolamine- and Amphetamine-Induced LI Disruption

Because physostigmine increases ACh levels in the synaptic cleft, we expected that it would reverse the effect of scopolamine-induced muscarinic blockade on LI, as has been found for scopolamine-induced PPI disruption and hyperactivity. In addition, because it was reported that physostigmine might act similarly to ‘dopaminergic’ APDs (Karan et al, 2000), our interest was to test whether this drug would also reverse amphetamine-induced LI disruption. The experiment included 18 experimental groups (n per group=7–9) in a 2 × 3 × 3 design with main factors of pre-exposure (0, 40), treatment (vehicle, scopolamine, amphetamine), and pretreatment (0, 0.05, 0.15 mg/kg physostigmine). Data of three rats were dropped from the analysis.

Results

The 18 experimental groups did not differ in their times to complete licks 51–75 before tone onset (all p's>0.05; overall mean A period=6.76 s). Figure 5 presents the mean log times to complete licks 76–100 (after tone onset) of the PE and NPE rats in the different experimental groups. As can be seen, LI was present in vehicle-treated rats and in rats injected with physostigmine alone, but was absent in rats that were treated with scopolamine or amphetamine. Physostigmine, at both doses, restored LI in scopolamine-treated rats, but failed to restore LI in amphetamine-treated rats. ANOVA yielded significant main effects of pre-exposure (F(1,119)=62.85, p<0.0001), treatment (F(2,119)=13.55, p<0.0001), and pretreatment (F(2,119)=3.71, p<0.05), and significant interactions of pre-exposure × treatment (F(2,119)=9.4, p<0.0005) and pre-exposure × treatment × pretreatment (F(4,119)=2.52, p<0.05). Post hoc comparisons revealed a significant difference between the PE and NPE groups in the vehicle, 0.05 mg/kg physostigmine, scopolamine+0.05 mg/kg physostigmine (p's<0.0001), 0.15 mg/kg physostigmine, and scopolamine+0.15 mg/kg physostigmine (p's<0.005) conditions, but not in the scopolamine alone, amphetamine alone, or the two amphetamine+physostigmine conditions.

Figure 5
figure 5

Effects of physostigmine on scopolamine- and amphetamine-induced LI disruption. Means and standard errors of the log times to complete licks 76–100 (after tone onset) of the pre-exposed (PE) and non-pre-exposed (NPE) vehicle-, scopolamine (0.15 mg/kg)-, or amphetamine (1 mg/kg)-treated rats, pretreated with physostigmine (0.05, 0.15 mg/kg). Scopolamine was injected in the pre-exposure stage. Amphetamine and physostigmine were injected in both stages. Asterisk indicates a significant difference between the PE and NPE groups, namely, presence of LI.

Experiment 6: Effects of 0.15 and 0.5 mg/kg Scopolamine and 1 mg/kg Amphetamine on Locomotor Activity

Drug-induced locomotor hyperactivity is the most widely used animal model of psychosis; accordingly, scopolamine has been shown to increase locomotor activity, but only at doses higher than 0.3 mg/kg (Mathur et al, 1997; Shannon and Peters, 1990; Sipos et al, 1999). While scopolamine at low doses used here (0.15 and 0.5 mg/kg) disrupted LI like amphetamine (1 mg/kg; Experiment 1), Experiment 5 indicated that these two propsychotic drugs may act via distinct mechanisms. We, therefore, compared the effects of the low doses of scopolamine and amphetamine on locomotor activity. The experiment included four experimental groups (n per group=6–7).

Results

Figure 6 presents the means and standard errors of duration of movements, in 6 min blocks, before and after vehicle, scopolamine, or amphetamine injection. As can be seen, spontaneous activity levels (first 30 min period) did not differ among the groups. Following amphetamine injection, there was a dramatic rise in activity level. No such increase was seen in scopolamine-injected rats although the higher dose produced a mild increase in activity. ANOVA yielded significant main effects of drug (F(3,21)=9.97, p<0.0005), periods (F(3,42)=9.32, p<0.0005), and blocks (F(4,84)=5.43, p<0.001) as well as significant interactions of drug × periods (F(6,42)=8.6, p<0.0001), periods × blocks (F(8,168)=2.21, p<0.05), and drug × period × blocks (F(24,168)=3.38, p<0.0001). Post hoc comparisons indicated that only amphetamine significantly increased duration of movements compared to the vehicle group (p<0.0001).

Figure 6
figure 6

Effects of scopolamine and amphetamine on locomotor activity. Means and standard errors of duration of movements, in 6 min blocks, before (blocks 1–5) and after (blocks 6–15) injection of vehicle, 1 mg/kg amphetamine, 0.15 mg/kg scopolamine, or 0.5 mg/kg scopolamine.

DISCUSSION

The present experiments demonstrated that LI can be abolished by scopolamine, and that this abolition can be reversed by APDs, as well as by the AChE inhibitor, physostigmine. Experiment 1 showed that scopolamine, administered in both pre-exposure and conditioning at doses of 0.15 and 0.5 mg/kg, abolished LI, while sparing LI at a higher dose of 1 mg/kg. These results are consistent with other findings suggesting that lower doses of muscarinic antagonists might be more effective than higher doses in some behavioral procedures. For example, Carnicella et al (2005) showed that the muscarinic antagonist atropine disrupted the degraded contingency effect (retarded conditioning following high probability of US-alone presentations compared to high probability of CS-US presentations) at 5 mg/kg, but not at higher doses. Likewise, Ukai et al (2004) showed that scopolamine attenuated PPI in mice at a dose of 0.3 mg/kg, but not at higher doses. It is not clear why scopolamine loses its efficacy in disrupting LI at higher doses. One possibility is that low and high doses of scopolamine bind to different types of muscarinic receptors, for example, excitatory vs inhibitory. The dose-dependent effects of scopolamine on DA release within the nucleus accumbens (NAC) may be particularly relevant in this respect. Thus, low dose of scopolamine increased NAC DA release, presumably via blockade of M2 inhibitory autoreceptors, whereas at higher doses this effect diminished, presumably due to increased blockade of M1 receptors (Ichikawa et al, 2002b). As LI disruption requires DA release within the NAC (Joseph et al, 2000; Weiner, 2003), this would be expected to occur with low but not high scopolamine doses.

In line with previous results (eg Anagnostaras et al, 1999), scopolamine at doses used here did not impair tone-shock conditioning, as reflected in the fact that suppression levels of scopolamine-treated NPE rats did not differ from those of vehicle-treated NPE rats. In fact, scopolamine-induced LI disruption stemmed exclusively from improved performance of the scopolamine-treated PE groups, which showed levels of suppression similar to those of their NPE counterparts. In other words, scopolamine-treated PE rats behaved as if they were not PE. The latter could stem from at least two sources: (1) scopolamine enhanced fear conditioning selectively in PE rats, or attenuated the retarding effect of stimulus pre-exposure on its subsequent conditioning; in this case, the site of scopolamine action would be the conditioning stage; (2) scopolamine impaired the capacity to learn to ignore the PE stimulus in the pre-exposure stage. The results of Experiment 2 supported the latter possibility. In this experiment, 0.15 mg/kg scopolamine disrupted LI if given only before pre-exposure, but not if given only before conditioning. Although this pattern could reflect state dependency, this possibility was ruled out by our finding that the same dose of scopolamine disrupted LI also when given before both stages. Pre-exposure-based LI disruption by scopolamine is consistent with the results of Carlton and Vogel (1965), but contradict those of Moore et al (1976), who found LI to be unaffected by scopolamine. The latter could be due to the higher dose used by Moore et al as also in the present study, the highest dose spared LI, or could reflect species differences (Moore et al used rabbits). The fact that scopolamine acts selectively in the PE groups, and that this action is exerted in the pre-exposure stage, implies that muscarinic blockade attenuates the normal loss of attention to the stimulus occurring during nonreinforced pre-exposure (Lubow et al, 1981), in line with extensive evidence implicating the cholinergic system in attentional processes (see Blokland, 1995; Hasselmo and McGaughy, 2004; Mirza and Stolerman, 2000; Sarter et al, 2003, 1999, 2005).

Experiments 3 and 4 showed that scopolamine-induced LI disruption was reversed by the typical APD haloperidol and the atypical APD clozapine, respectively. Moreover, APDs restored disrupted LI if injected in both the pre-exposure and conditioning stages, or in the conditioning stage alone, but not in the pre-exposure stage alone. Precisely the same stage-dependent pattern of APD action is obtained for the most widely documented effect of APDs on LI, namely, LI potentiation following low number of pre-exposures that do not suffice to yield LI in no-drug controls (eg in the procedure used here, no LI is seen with 10 pre-exposures, but LI emerges under this condition following APD treatment; Weiner et al, 1996; Warburton et al, 1994). As functionally, pre-exposure to 40 tones under scopolamine may be equivalent to reducing the number of pre-exposures, restoration of LI in scopolamine-treated rats by APDs may represent an instance of APD-induced LI potentiation seen with low number of pre-exposures.

While reversal of scopolamine-induced behavioral deficits by typical and atypical APDs had been shown previously (Jones et al, 2005; Mathur et al, 1997; Shannon and Peters, 1990), the unique aspect of the present results is that scopolamine-induced LI deficit and its reversal by APDs were generated in different stages of the LI procedure taking place 24 h apart. Therefore, while reversal of scopolamine-induced behavioral deficits by APDs has been attributed to a direct interaction between the dopaminergic and the muscarinic cholinergic systems (eg Jones et al, 2005; Mathur et al, 1997), such an interaction cannot explain the present results. Rather, the reversal of scopolamine-induced LI disruption by APDs is likely to reflect complex interactions within the brain circuitry that modulates the expression of LI (Weiner, 2003), whereby scopolamine exerts its effects on brain substrates mediating the processing of the PE stimulus in pre-exposure that differ from but interact with brain substrates at which APDs act to potentiate LI in conditioning. Studies of the neural substrates of LI have shown that the APD-induced LI potentiation is mediated via the NAC (Joseph et al, 2000; Weiner, 2003), whereas the information on the PE stimulus is fed to the NAC from the entorhinal cortex (Jeanblanc et al, 2004; Weiner, 2003), raising the possibility that the latter is the region where muscarinic blockade acts to impair LI. While the neural substrates involved in APD-induced reversal of scopolamine-induced LI disruption remain to be investigated, the fact that scopolamine and APDs act in different stages suggests that scopolamine-induced disrupted LI may allow the detection of antipsychotic action that is independent of the mechanism of action of the propsychotic drug, opening a unique avenue for identifying agents acting through novel mechanisms.

Experiment 5 showed that scopolamine-induced LI disruption was reversed by physostigmine. This drug did not affect LI when given alone. These findings are in line with previous results using other animal models of schizophrenia. For example, physostigmine alone had no effect on PPI (Jones and Shannon, 2000b; Mach et al, 2004) or locomotor activity (Shannon and Peters, 1990; but see Mach et al, 2004), but reversed scopolamine-induced PPI disruption (Jones and Shannon, 2000b) and hyperactivity (Shannon and Peters, 1990). In contrast to its success in reversing scopolamine-induced LI disruption, physostigmine failed to restore amphetamine-induced LI disruption. This suggests that physostigmine acted to restore LI by restoring the ability to learn the irrelevance of the PE stimulus, which was impaired by scopolamine; in amphetamine-treated rats, this ability is intact (Weiner, 2003), and therefore physostigmine was inactive. While these suggestions remain to be investigated, the fact that physostigmine reversed scopolamine- but not amphetamine-induced LI disruption sets this compound apart from APDs, which reverse both deficits. The latter is inconsistent with the report that physostigmine acted like ‘dopaminergic’ APDs, and in particular, blocked amphetamine-induced stereotypy (Karan et al, 2000), but is in line with Stone et al's (1990) finding of low susceptibility of amphetamine-induced hyperactivity to physostigmine, because here we used a low, activity-producing dose of amphetamine. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first demonstration of distinct effects of physostigmine on scopolamine- and amphetamine-induced behavioral deficits produced in the same behavioral phenomenon.

The findings of the present study join those of other studies showing that scopolamine and other muscarinic antagonists produce psychotic-like effects in animals (eg Jones et al, 2005; Jones and Shannon, 2000a, 2000b; Mathur et al, 1997; Shannon and Peters, 1990; Sipos et al, 1999; Ukai et al, 2004; Wu et al, 1993) and that these effects are reversed by APDs (Jones et al, 2005; Shannon and Peters, 1990) and physostigmine (Jones and Shannon, 2000b; Shannon and Peters, 1990). Disruption of LI by amphetamine and its reversal by APDs is a well-established model of positive symptoms, fortified by findings of disrupted LI in amphetamine-treated normal humans, high schizotypal individuals, and acute schizophrenia patients (see Lubow, 2005; Weiner, 2003). Therefore, disruption of LI by scopolamine and its reversal by APDs and physostigmine may provide a model of the ‘positive’ symptom spectrum of the antimuscarinic psychosis (anticholinergic syndrome), and by corollary, of the cholinergic aspects of positive symptoms in endogenous schizophrenia.

However, it is important to underscore in this context that in spite of their identical behavioral manifestations (disrupted LI), the ‘antimuscarinic LI model’ and the ‘dopamine agonist LI model’ are clearly distinct in several respects. First, scopolamine disrupts LI via effects exerted at the pre-exposure stage and spares LI when given in conditioning only, whereas amphetamine disrupts LI via effects exerted at the conditioning stage and spares LI if given before pre-exposure only (Gray et al, 1997; Joseph et al, 2000; McAllister, 1997; Weiner, 2003; Weiner et al, 1984, 1988). In addition to indicating that the neural substrates underlying LI disruption induced by amphetamine and scopolamine are different, the stage-based dissociation implies that the disruptions of LI induced by these two pharmacologically distinct classes of drugs cannot be attributed to a disturbance to a common psychological function. The dissociation between scopolamine- and amphetamine-induced disruption of LI is further supported by the manner in which the two abnormalities are reversed by APDs. Thus, although APDs reverse both abnormalities, in the case of amphetamine-induced LI disruption, the propsychotic and the antipsychotic actions are exerted at the same stage of the procedure and thus likely reflect a direct interaction, whereas in the case of scopolamine-induced LI disruption, the propsychotic and antipsychotic actions are generated in different stages of the procedure and thus mediated by distinct mechanisms. Third, scopolamine-induced, but not amphetamine-induced, LI disruption was reversed by physostigmine. Thus, while scopolamine-induced LI disruption can be reversed by both APDs and an AChE inhibitor, amphetamine-induced LI disruption can be reversed only by the former.

Taken together, these findings indicate that scopolamine- and amphetamine-induced LI disruption represents different phenomena, and therefore might model different aspects of schizophrenic psychoses. Specifically, scopolamine-induced LI disruption may model muscarinic-related attentional deficits, which may be linked to cognitive impairments seen in this disorder.

The dissociation between scopolamine and amphetamine was also evident in the activity model. While amphetamine (1 mg/kg) markedly increased locomotor activity, scopolamine (0.15 and 0.5 mg/kg) did not alter locomotor activity, supporting the notion that distinct mechanisms underlie the effects of the two psychotomimetics. Although it was suggested that scopolamine affects behavior like amphetamine by increasing striatal/accumbal DA transmission (Ichikawa et al, 2002a; Yeomans, 1995), our results in LI and locomotor activity imply that this is not the case with low doses of scopolamine.

In sum, the present study showed that low doses of scopolamine impair rats’ capacity to ignore stimuli that are repeatedly presented without consequences while leaving their capacity for associative learning intact. This pattern implies that the cholinergic muscarinic system plays a role in attentional processes underlying the acquisition of LI. The latter, in turn, suggests that scopolamine-induced LI disruption may model attentional abnormalities associated with cholinergic dysfunction. In addition, the fact that scopolamine-induced LI disruption is in several respects distinct from amphetamine-induced LI disruption underscores the utility of the two deficits for modeling antimuscarinic and dopaminergic psychoses, respectively, and by extension, cholinergic and dopaminergic aspects of schizophrenic psychoses. The latter may facilitate the search for treatments that target selectively each of these abnormalities. In particular, it is of interest to determine whether specific muscarinic receptor agonists, which were shown to exhibit antipsychotic properties in the clinic, would reverse both scopolamine- and amphetamine-induced LI deficits as was shown in other animal models (Bymaster et al, 2002; Jones et al, 2005; Stanhope et al, 2001), or would show selectivity like found here for physostigmine. In addition, the capacity of cholinergic cognitive enhancers to ameliorate these deficits should be examined. Finally, it is of interest to examine whether the negative/cognitive symptom spectrum of antimuscarinic psychosis can also be modeled in the LI model.