Research articles

Filter By:

Year
  • What conditions produce a willingness to sacrifice our own self-interest for others? McGrath and Gerber find that collaboration increases willingness to sacrifice, distinct from considerations of accountability, in-group favouritism or disparity.

    • Mary C. McGrath
    • Alan S. Gerber
    Letter
  • How good are people at choosing between exploration and exploitation? In a task that captures the essence of such decisions, Song et al. found systematic deviations from optimality that were associated with the sequence of decisions participants can make.

    • Mingyu Song
    • Zahy Bnaya
    • Wei Ji Ma
    Letter
  • Bocanegra and colleagues present a new variation of the Raven intelligence test, an established measure of cognitive function; better performance on this new version, which allows problem-solving to be externalized, is associated with students’ success in exams.

    • Bruno R. Bocanegra
    • Fenna H. Poletiek
    • Andy Clark
    Letter
  • Data from three diverse post-conflict societies show that individuals with greater war exposure, several years later, were more likely to participate in religious groups and rituals. This reveals a link from violent conflict to religiosity.

    • Joseph Henrich
    • Michal Bauer
    • Benjamin Grant Purzycki
    Letter
  • A daily, city-level happiness metric constructed from the sentiment expressed in 210 million tweets on Sina Weibo from 144 cities shows that high levels of air pollution significantly reduce Chinese urbanites’ expressed happiness on social media.

    • Siqi Zheng
    • Jianghao Wang
    • Matthew E. Kahn
    Letter
  • Using data from 765 million online music plays chosen by 1 million individuals in 51 countries, Park et al. reveal diurnal and seasonal affective rhythms in musical intensity that are consistent across diverse cultures and demographic groups. They also report differences in baseline preferences for musical intensity across cultures and ages.

    • Minsu Park
    • Jennifer Thom
    • Michael Macy
    Letter
  • An individual’s social ties contain up to 95% of the potential predictive accuracy achievable about that individual. In principle, a social platform may therefore profile an individual from their ties only, without access to their data.

    • James P. Bagrow
    • Xipei Liu
    • Lewis Mitchell
    Letter
  • When do groups exhibit collective ‘wisdom’ vs maladaptive ‘herding’? Toyokawa et al. use modelling and experimentation to show that crowd intelligence versus herding can be predicted on the basis of the task and the social learning strategies used.

    • Wataru Toyokawa
    • Andrew Whalen
    • Kevin N. Laland
    Article
  • This scoping review identified, summarized and critiqued 15 ontologies related to human behaviour change. The review finds that no existing ontology covers the breadth of human behaviour change and identifies the need for an intervention ontology.

    • Emma Norris
    • Ailbhe N. Finnerty
    • Susan Michie
    Article
  • Why do we continue processing external events during sleep, yet remain unresponsive? Legendre et al. use electroencephalography to show that sleepers enter a ‘standby mode’, continuing to track relevant signals but doing so transiently.

    • Guillaume Legendre
    • Thomas Andrillon
    • Sid Kouider
    Letter
  • Adolescents regularly use digital technology, but its impact on their psychological well-being is unclear. Here, the authors examine three large datasets and find only a small negative association: digital technology use explains at most 0.4% of well-being.

    • Amy Orben
    • Andrew K. Przybylski
    Article
  • Nearby small objects appear larger than distal large objects, reflecting a dissociation between perceived and actual object size. Collegio et al. show that inferences of true object size scale spatial attention to objects.

    • Andrew J. Collegio
    • Joseph C. Nah
    • Sarah Shomstein
    Letter