Article

The unintended consequences of argument dilution in direct-to-consumer drug advertisements

Received:
Accepted:
Published online:

Abstract

Direct-to-consumer (DTC) advertising of pharmaceutical drugs is often cited as the culprit for inflated patient demand for advertised drugs. Further to this economic concern, we provide an evidence-based psychological account of another concern that warrants the re-examination of the merits of DTC advertising of prescription drugs. Across six experiments and a sample of 3,059 US participants, we find reliable evidence for the argument dilution effect. Specifically, when commercials list severe side effects along with those that are most frequent (which include both serious and minor side effects), as required by the Food and Drug Administration, it dilutes consumers’ judgements of the overall severity of the side effects, compared with when only the serious side effects are listed. Furthermore, consumers’ reduced judgement of severity leads to greater attraction to those drugs. In regulating pharmaceutical advertisements, the Food and Drug Administration appear to have paradoxically dampened consumers’ judgements of overall severity and risk, and increased the marketability of these drugs.

  • Subscribe to Nature Human Behaviour for full access:

    $99

    Subscribe

Additional access options:

Already a subscriber?  Log in  now or  Register  for online access.

References

  1. 1.

    Vastag, B. US aims to tighten rules on direct-to-consumer drug ads. Nat. Biotechnol. 25, 267–267 (2007).

  2. 2.

    Food and Drug Administration Regulatory Modernization Act of 1997, H.R. 1411, 105th Cong., 1st Sess. (1997); https://www.fda.gov/RegulatoryInformation/LawsEnforcedbyFDA/SignificantAmendmentstotheFDCAct/FDAMA/FullTextofFDAMAlaw/default.htm

  3. 3.

    Mogull, S. A. Chronology of direct-to-consumer advertising regulation in the United States. Am. Med. Writ. Assoc. J. 23, 106–109 (2008).

  4. 4.

    Pines, W. L. A history and perspective on direct-to-consumer promotion. Food Drug Law J. 54, 489–518 (1999).

  5. 5.

    Mintzes, B. Direct to consumer advertising is medicalising normal human experience. BMJ 324, 908–911 (2002).

  6. 6.

    Saul, S. Senate leader calls for limits on drug ads. The New York Times Politics (2 July 2005).

  7. 7.

    Kahneman, D. & Tversky, A. On the psychology of prediction. Psychol. Rev. 80, 237–251 (1973).

  8. 8.

    Nisbett, R. E., Zukier, H. & Lemley, R. E. The dilution effect: nondiagnostic information weakens the implications of diagnostic information. Cognit. Psychol. 13, 248–277 (1981).

  9. 9.

    Zukier, H. Situational determinants of behavior. Soc. Res. 49, 1073–1091 (1982).

  10. 10.

    Zukier, H. & Jennings, D. L. Nondiagnosticity and typicality effects in prediction. Soc. Cogn. 2, 187–198 (1984).

  11. 11.

    Meyvis, T. & Janiszewski, C. Consumers’ beliefs about product benefits: the effect of obviously irrelevant product information. J. Consum. Res. 28, 618–635 (2002).

  12. 12.

    Troutman, C. M. & Shanteau, J. Inferences based on nondiagnostic information. Organ. Behav. Hum. Perform. 19, 43–55 (1977).

  13. 13.

    Anderson, N. H. Integration theory and attitude change. Psychol. Rev. 78, 171–206 (1971).

  14. 14.

    Anderson, N. H. Application of an additive model to impression formation. Science 138, 817–818 (1962).

  15. 15.

    Biegler, P. & Vargas, P. Ban the sunset? Nonpropositional content and regulation of pharmaceutical advertising. Am. J. Bioeth. 13, 3–13 (2013).

  16. 16.

    Petty, R. E. & Cacioppo, J. T. Communication and Persuasion 1–24 (Springer, New York, 1986); https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-4964-1_1

  17. 17.

    Zhao, X., Lynch, J. G. & Chen, Q. Reconsidering Baron and Kenny: myths and truths about mediation analysis. J. Consum. Res. 37, 197–206 (2010).

  18. 18.

    MacKinnon, D. P., Krull, J. L. & Lockwood, C. M. Equivalence of the mediation, confounding and suppression effect. Prev. Sci. 1, 173–181 (2000).

  19. 19.

    Schwartz, L. M. & Woloshin, S. The drug facts box: improving the communication of prescription drug information. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 110, 14069–14074 (2013).

  20. 20.

    Aikin, K. J., O’Donoghue, A. C., Swasy, J. L. & Sullivan, H. W. Randomized trial of risk information formats in direct-to-consumer prescription drug advertisements. Med. Decis. Mak. 31, E23–E33 (2011).

  21. 21.

    Deese, J. & Kaufman, R. A. Serial effects in recall of unorganized and sequentially organized verbal material. J. Exp. Psychol. 54, 180–187 (1957).

  22. 22.

    Spencer, S. J., Zanna, M. P. & Fong, G. T. Establishing a causal chain: why experiments are often more effective than mediational analyses in examining psychological processes. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 89, 845–851 (2005).

  23. 23.

    Relations, A. M. AMA calls for ban on direct to consumer advertising of prescription drugs and medical devices. American Medical Association (17 November 2015); http://www.ama-assn.org/ama/pub/news/news/2015/2015-11-17-ban-consumer-prescription-drug-advertising.page

  24. 24.

    Thaler, R. H. & Sunstein, C. R. Libertarian paternalism. Am. Econ. Rev. 93, 175–179 (2003).

  25. 25.

    Daft, R. L. & Lengel, R. H. organizational information requirements, media richness and structural design. Manag. Sci. 32, 554–571 (1986).

Download references

Acknowledgements

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript. We thank N. Ramkumar for assistance with materials for Study 2a.

Author information

Affiliations

  1. Department of Organisational Behaviour, London Business School, Regent’s Park, London, NW1 4SA, UK

    • Niro Sivanathan
    •  & Hemant Kakkar

Authors

  1. Search for Niro Sivanathan in:

  2. Search for Hemant Kakkar in:

Contributions

N.S. developed the research idea. N.S. and H.K. designed the experiments; H.K. analysed the data; and N.S. and H.K. wrote the paper.

Ethics statement

The ethics approval for this project was provided by London Business School as per the school’s guidelines. In line with ethical guidelines, all participants provided informed consent before taking part in the studies.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Niro Sivanathan.

Electronic supplementary material

  1. Supplementary Information

    Supplementary Methods, Supplementary Table 1.

  2. Life Sciences Reporting Summary

    Life Sciences Reporting Summary