Introduction

Genetics studies have become increasingly prevalent, leading to a large body of information on disease susceptibility, with the potential to improve health care.1 Although some of the data can be characterized as exploratory, that is to say, non-validated or hypothesis generating, with no clinical utility, there is a growing international trend for research participants to be offered access to their personal research data if they choose to see it.2, 3, 4 The guidelines governing research with human subjects are relatively extensive with respect to clinical trials.2 Researchers have a duty to return the results if the latter meet three criteria: scientific validity, clinical relevance and benefits to the participant.4 This framework, however, does not help in a context of exploratory results, such as in genetics.5 In such a context, how to envisage the return of research results? Researchers are facing the issue of whether to follow this trend, and if so, how research participants might receive the results? and more particularly if they can receive individualized results.3, 6 But what are the attitudes and needs of research participants themselves? There is very little empirical data on this.2, 7, 8, 9

It is important to note that this article does not address the return of incidental findings picked up during routine data cleaning and data analyses. Most incidental findings cannot be directly correlated with a particular disease or condition. Even if researchers may incidentally identify genetic abnormalities that might influence the clinical care of an individual, it is beyond the aims of their study. Unlike research results which are data obtained as part of the research protocol to answer a given study question, most incidental findings are not information anticipated from the study and cannot be generalized for a wide or general application.

Ravitsky and Wilfond (2006) report two main approaches concerning the return of research results. The first one focuses on research with a reluctance to return results for three main reasons: the potential harm to participants10, 11, 12 budgetary constraints surrounding such a practice13 and lack of practical guidance in the guidelines.14 The second one focuses on the autonomy of the participant and considers the return of research results as a ‘moral imperative’.15 This vision is conveyed by the guidelines for research involving humans in which the obligation of the researcher to return results to participants that wish it4, 16 is increasingly common. But these guidelines are unclear on the desired practical achievements, such as what type of information should be returned? By whom? When? How? and Who is to bear the cost?4 Such vagueness leaves research teams puzzled. Although they are aware of this emerging ethical issue, they do not know how to return research results to study participants, and they do not know about participants’ wishes in this respect. In this paper, we present empirical data on one set of participants’ expectations, as well as their needs and concerns about the return of research results. Data were gathered using a postal survey of parents of an autistic child having participated in a genetic research project on autism.

Subjects and methods

Study participants

An ethical approval was obtained from the regional Research Ethics Committee (Centre de recherche du centre hospitalier de l’Université de Montréal or CR-CHUM). Consent to participate in the study was presumed upon receipt of a completed questionnaire.

Data used in this paper originate from a genetic research on autism in which no individual result was communicated. According to the database provided, 194 families were eligible to participate in our survey. The information letter accompanying the survey indicated that it was independent of the genetic study, and although no individual result was given out, our study aimed to obtain research participants’ point of view on the return of information in research projects, so as to analyze the perceived risks and benefits related to the communication of either general or individual findings. Respondents were asked to fill out an anonymous questionnaire and each parent was specifically asked to fill one out. A reminder was sent to all 194 families 4 weeks after the original mail-out.

We sent 388 questionnaires to the 194 eligible families (to the mother and the father). In all, 158 questionnaires were filled out (89 mothers and 69 fathers) and sent back to us resulting in a response rate of 41% after one reminder. Due to the fact that the questionnaire was anonymous, it was impossible to know in how many families each parent filled out the questionnaire. In all, 92.4% of respondents indicated that they live with a spouse, but they did not mentioned if the spouse was the parent of the child enrolled in the genetic study; 7% of respondents mentioned that they did not live with a spouse.

Study instrument

Our anonymous postal questionnaire was validated by researchers in the fields of genetics and research ethics, by parents of autistic children and by different actors working with children with mental retardation in order to empirically test the reliability of the questionnaire.

The questionnaire took 30 min to fill out. A first section aimed to contextualize the respondents’ experience in a research setting (why did they participate in the genetic study? what were the advantages and disadvantages they envisioned for themselves and for their child? what importance did they accord to genetics? and what they knew about the return of research results?). Their attitudes toward participation in genetic studies and their expectations about the return of ‘general’ or ‘individual’ results were also discussed in this part of the questionnaire. The terminology used in our study as explained to research participants is summarized in Table 1. The next section of the questionnaire inquired as to their views on their wish to receive, or not, research results (what kind of information they wanted? what benefits they envisioned, from whom they wanted to receive information? when? who should bear the expense of result dissemination? what they thought they would do with the information? and what impact they thought the return of research results would have on their lives?). Finally, the last part documented the socio-demographic data of the participants.

Table 1 Terminology for the types of result

Statistical analysis

Information from the completed questionnaires was transferred to an Excel database. Coding and analysis were conducted using the Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS) software v. 16.0 (http://www.spss.com). A descriptive analysis documented the expectations of participants as well as providing avenues to address the practical issues of returning research results to participants. The interpretative analysis, based on χ2 and factorial analyses, allowed us to better characterize the respondents and to reach a deeper analysis of what motivated and guided their choices. Factorial correspondence analysis is an inductive exploration of a data set for finding patterns in the data when several traits have been recorded for each individual.17 By reducing the number of primary traits into a smaller number of factors that are combinations of the original variables and that account for the most variance, the redundancy in the original data set is eliminated, and true associations between traits may be found. In regard to our sample, a factorial correspondence analysis was carried out to identify and quantify distinct components in the respondents’ population based on their responses to the survey questions. A typological analysis was also carried out to determine the main characteristics of respondent groupings toward the return of individual research results. We considered a P-value of 0.05 or less as statistically significant.

Results

The characteristics of the respondents are given in Table 2. Of the 158 completed questionnaires, fathers filled out 69, mothers 89. Even if respondents were informed that no individual result concerning their child would be disclosed, 97% of them (n=153) still indicated a wish to be informed of individual research results, whether favorable or not.

Table 2 Characteristics of the respondents (n=158)

Opinions

Given that an important concern of researchers and ethics committees is the impact on participants of receiving unfavorable individual findings, our analysis focused on the comparison of expectations and needs of research participants toward the reception of a general finding vs an unfavorable individual finding (Figure 1). The vast majority of respondents indicated that the research team should take on the responsibility of communicating results (85% for general results and 80% for unfavorable individual ones), rather than a health professional or a research ethics committee. Another finding was that although the participants wanted to obtain results, they did not expect to act to get them. A majority of respondents indicated that the best time to receive the results would be at the end of the research project (respectively 78% for a general result and 69% for an unfavorable individual result) and before the results were made public. It seems important for them to get the information, acquired thanks to them, before any dissemination to a wider public. Again, respondents did not expect to contact the research team to receive the results. As a matter of fact, they would not know when to do so. As shown in Figure 1, the preference to receive research results such as they were had priority over that of receiving them only once validated and before a prevention or a treatment became available. The favored means of communication was via mail for any type of result (75% for an unfavorable individual result), much higher than by going to a clinic. This finding challenges the ethical guidelines governing research according to which disclosure of personal information should preferably be done during a face-to-face meeting. Finally, the vast majority of respondents indicated that the costs associated with the return of research results should be covered by the Ministry of Health and Social Services (78% for an unfavorable individual result), neither the research team nor the research ethics board, much less the doctor of the child.

Figure 1
figure 1

Preferences regarding the return of research results, depending on the type of result, reported by parents of autistic children (n=158).

Intentions

Two points of particular interest were, first, the participants’ perceptions of the expected impact on them of receiving individual information, and second, what the participants thought they would do with such information, if available, whether favorable or unfavorable. While a consensus emerged as to the desired means to be used to return research results, there was a great diversity of views on the two previous points.

To the open question ‘If the communication of individual findings turns out to be possible (whether positive or negative), what would you expect to do with this information?’, less than half of the respondents indicated that the communication of individual findings would allow them to make better decisions for the future (37%, n=57), or would allow them to inform their family circle (19%, n=29) (Figure 2). In all, 14% of respondents (n=21) even indicated that they would do nothing with the information, although they still would like to receive it. Although the respondents vary on how they would use such results, their expectations seemed to be realistic.

Figure 2
figure 2

What the respondent expects to do with an individual finding.

The same plurality of opinions appears regarding the expected impact of the results on each respondent's life or on their child's life (Figure 3). If the communication of individual findings turns out to be possible, whether favorable or unfavorable, those findings would allow some respondents ‘to be prepared for the future’ (37%, n=57), without necessarily having an impact (21%, n=32), but at least bringing relief or understanding (14%, n=21). All in all, the perceived impact seems either positive or neutral.

Figure 3
figure 3

The impact an individual finding could have on the respondent's life.

Two typical profiles

With the diversity of intentions expressed by respondents and in spite of unanimous views favoring the return of research results, whether favorable or unfavorable, a factor analysis brought out two typical profiles in favor of returning individual research results (Figure 4): (a) a profile of persons concerned by a possible transmission of autism and (b) a profile of persons who ‘want to know’. The first profile (a), representing 20% of the respondents, will make practical use of the research results, that is, they will prepare for the future, including making reproductive choices accordingly. The expectations of this group contrast with those of the other profile (b), which represents 15% of the respondents. They participated in the study for the sole purpose of knowing, with no associated intentions. In fact, respondents in this group participated in the research project to contribute to the advance of scientific knowledge.

Figure 4
figure 4

Characterization of two profiles: (a) profile of persons concerned by a possible transmission of autism and (b) profile of persons who want to know.

Following the characterization of these patterns, other profiles were searched for in order to understand who would make what use of research results once these were obtained. Due to concerns in the literature on the therapeutic misconception18, 19 we defined a ‘clinical profile’, that is people who take part in research with clinical expectations (to get treatment for their child or a clinical follow-up). This clinical profile was not present in a significant way among the respondents, indicating that the latter (1) do not seem to have unrealistic clinical expectations and (2) do not confuse the clinical setting with the research setting.

Discussion

Our study gathered empirical data from participants in an ongoing genetic research project on autism involving children and their parents. These data may help in broadening the discussion about the communication of research results, especially individual, unfavorable ones.

Noting an unanimous desire to receive individual research results, whether favorable or unfavorable, with varying intentions about subsequent action, from ‘doing nothing’ to preparing for the future, everyone was nonetheless clear about the difference between research and clinical settings. In that context, we reviewed the literature in search of arguments for and against the communication of research results (Table 3). The most common argument deals with the potential harm of receiving an unfavorable individual result, particularly one of uncertain clinical value.3 In our study, respondents did not perceive any potential harm, even if they were listed in the questionnaire. They rather saw the benefits. Some authors have mentioned a paternalistic attitude in researchers.20 Our results indicate that research participants are able to balance the risks and benefits of research, as they do not have unrealistic clinical expectations. Indeed, if one accepts to participate in a research project and is judged sufficiently autonomous to do so, it seems paradoxical that their autonomy cannot be recognized at the end of the study. Ironically, the argument that a research participant may not be able to understand the results is often put forward in the literature. Certain authors, like Brand, consider that ‘the scientific community will lose credibility if, on one hand, it is promoting research literacy and enabling and empowering individuals for informed decision making, while, on the other hand, ignoring and withholding genomics knowledge’.20

Table 3 Potential harm and benefits of communicating research results

A second argument is about the lack of resources, what with the limited means available to health-care systems.13, 14, 21 Although a lack of funds is the major obstacle to returning information,22 a real, practical evaluation of the supplementary allocation required to return research results has yet to be carried out. In our study, respondents preferred mail for communicating research results, whether favorable or unfavorable. With computerized databases, it becomes easier to manage and follow research participants. In cases where the communication of individual findings turns out to be possible, recontacting research participants would be doable. Other authors have suggested that a good approach would be to tell all participants, by means of generalized feedback, that a particular result has achieved clinical significance and that they should consider seeking testing via a clinical geneticist.1, 22

Last but not least, another argument against communicating research results is the lack of guidance in the ethical guidelines. We believe that the integration of different perspectives – such as those of researchers, clinicians, ethics committees and participants – will help in the debate. By identifying the specific needs, preferences and concerns of those various stakeholders, an integrative vision could emerge. To do so, more empirical studies are needed to understand the perspectives of various actors in different settings so as to mold ethical guidelines consistent with the reality of the practice.

Conclusion

This empirical study allows a better understanding of the opinions and intentions of research participants as to the communication of results. We note that the majority of respondents did not perceive a negative impact regarding the reception of research results, whatever those results were. In addition, they did not have unrealistic expectations. Consequently, is there still a benefit to withholding information?22 Can research participants have a right to be informed of research results, based on the right to self-governance, as Beauchamp and Childress state?23 Although this question emerges in the literature,20, 22, 24 it might be premature to move directly from empirical data showing a strong desire of respondents to receive research results to a claim that participants have a right to receive their personal results.

However, concrete avenues can be explored concerning the return of research results, all the while minimizing the potential harm and enhancing respect for the participants and their central role in research:

  • Offer the choice to research participants of receiving or not their personal research results, while specifying the nature of this information, throughout the study. This would respect their right to know or their right not to know.

  • The potential benefits and possible harm linked to the reception of individual research results should be explained to the participant at the beginning of the study.

  • An efficient and inexpensive way of returning research results to participants could be developed through a computerized database, a mailing list, etc, or, as suggested by Affleck,22 by means of a generalized feedback mechanism upon which participants could base themselves to seek testing via a clinical geneticist.

  • A sharing of the responsibility to return research results between the research team and a clinician also seems important, since a large majority of respondents indicated they would like to report the result to their physician. This would respect the clinician/researcher distinction and ensure that results were given by those best trained to do so.22

  • One could envisage a sharing of costs between the research team, the funding agency and the Ministry of Health and Social Services. Research funds could be specifically allocated to the dissemination of research results (to the participants but also to the public in general) in a perspective of scientific disclosure and knowledge translation.

Limitations

The results and the analysis rely on self-reported data from the questionnaire. It may be possible that people who answered the survey were those who wanted the results, as it may be possible that parents with autistic children may have a hunger for any medical information, which could explain the very high percentage in favor of disclosure of research results. However, we also think that people who were totally against disclosure would have also wanted to answer the questionnaire, in order to emphasize their point of view.

In addition, the nature of the population studied – parents with autistic children – may limit the generalizability of our findings to other similar disorders and not reflect the full spectrum of current genetic research.