Introduction

Reading is a cognitive skill unique to humans and crucial for living in the modern society. To be a successful reader, one must rapidly integrate a vast circuit of brain areas with both great accuracy and remarkable speed. This ‘reading circuit’ is composed of neural systems that support language as well as visual and orthographic processes, working memory, attention, motor functions and higher-level comprehension and cognition.1 Nevertheless, for about 5 to 12% of the population, learning to read is extremely difficult.2 These individuals are affected by a complex neurodevelopmental disorder called developmental dyslexia (DD), which represents the most common learning disability among school-aged children and across languages. DD is a lifelong impairment2 characterized by impaired reading acquisition in spite of adequate neurological and sensorial conditions, educational opportunities and normal intelligence.3 This difficulty in reading is often associated with undesirable outcomes for children as well as with social impact and economic burden.2

Although the field is immature, the role of genetics in DD is rapidly growing and much has been learned regarding the possible downstream effects of DD risk genes on the brain structure, function and circuitry. Similarly, cognitive and psychophysic studies have provided initial evidence about the usefulness of testing well-identified cognitive and sensorial deficits associated with and causative of DD to pursue the biological and genetic components of this disorder. Following the increasing findings provided by molecular genetic, cognitive and imaging–genetic studies of DD, this review aims to propose an interdisciplinary, multilevel, imaging–genetic approach to disentangle the pathways from genes to behavior. An interdisciplinary integration of particular cognitive/sensorial, selective genetic, and imaging data, will provide a critically important bridge for ‘connecting the dots’ between genes, cells, circuits, neurocognition, functional impairment and personalized treatment selection, and will pave the way for new candidate gene–candidate phenotype imaging association studies.4

Genetics of DD

Following earlier descriptions of strong familial aggregation of the disorder,5 substantial heritability typical of a complex trait has been reported6 with estimates across DD and DD-related quantitative phenotypes ranging from 0.18 to 0.72.7 Since the early 1980s, at least nine DD risk loci termed DYX1DYX9 on eight different chromosomes have been mapped (that is, 1p36-p34, 2p16-p15, 3p12-q13, 6p22 and 6q13-16.2, 11p15.5, 15q21.3, 18p11.2 and Xq27.3) and the involvement of several genes spanning these regions in the etiology of DD has been reported (that is, DYX1C1, DCDC2, KIAA0319, C2ORF3, MRPL19, ROBO1, FAM176A, NRSN1, KIAA0319L and FMR1).8, 9, 10, 11, 12, 13 Apart from these DYX loci, other genes implicated in other disorders, before being examined for DD, have also been associated with reading (dis)ability, that is, FOXP2, CNTNAP2, DOCK4 and GTF2I on chromosome 7,14, 15, 16, 17 GRIN2B and SLC2A3 on chromosome 12,18, 19, 20 ATP2C2 and CMIP on chromosome 16,15, 21 PCNT, DIP2A, S100B and PRMT2 on chromosome 21.21, 22, 23 Recent genome-wide association and sequencing studies further strengthened the role of previously identified DD-candidate genes22, 24, 25 and identified novel associations with markers spanning new chromosomal regions.12, 22, 24, 26, 27, 28, 29, 30 Among all these genes, nine DD-candidate genes have been replicated in at least one independent sample: DYX1C1, DCDC2, KIAA0319, C2ORF3, MRPL19, ROBO1, GRIN2B, FOXP2 and CNTNAP2.8, 9, 10, 11, 12, 18, 20,,31 Interestingly, initial evidence has been provided of the presence of putative functional genetic variants influencing the expression of some of the above-described DD-candidate genes. A functional effect of two single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in DYX1C1, rs3743205 (-3G→A) and rs57809907 (1249C→T), has been hypothesized on the basis of bioinformatics predictions.32 In particular, the -3G→A SNP is located in the binding sequence of the transcription factors Elk-1, HSTF and TFII-I, and affects the Kozak sequence, which has a major role in the translation process. The coding 1249C→T-SNP truncates the protein and thus likely disrupts its functionality.32 These two DYX1C1 variants have been associated with DD and DD-related phenotypes,32, 33, 34 although opposite patterns of effects35, 36, 37, 38, 39, 40, 41, 42 and negative findings43 have also been observed. A three-SNP risk haplotype spanning across TTRAP, THEM2 and KIAA0319 genes, has been described, that is, rs4504469, rs2038137 and rs2143340.44 This risk haplotype is associated with 40% lower levels of the expression, splicing or transcript stability of any of the KIAA0319, TTRAP or THEM2 genes as compared with the non-risk haplotype.44 Furthermore, it has been shown to associate with DD in three independent clinical samples,44, 45, 46, 47 as well as in two large unselected samples.48, 49 Further characterization of KIAA0319 has led to the identification of a marker in the risk haplotype, that is, rs9461045, found to be strongly associated with DD and to influence gene expression, possibly due to the alteration of the binding site to transcriptional silencer OCT-1 by luciferase-based assays.47 Interestingly, a 168-base pair purine-rich region in the intron 2 of the DCDC2 gene harboring a highly polymorphic, short-tandem repeat (BV677278) has been reported.50 This non-coding region might serve as a regulatory node as it contains 131 putative transcription factor binding sites, is rather conserved across species and has the capacity of enhancing activity, as BV677278 changes the reporter gene expression from the DCDC2 promoter in an allele-specific manner.51 Although more work is needed to confirm it, Powers et al.52 recently identified the BV677278-binding protein as the transcription factor ETV6, confirmed BV677278 as a regulatory element and proposed ‘regulatory element associated with dyslexia 1’ (READ1) as a new name. As such, READ1 could substantially act as a modifier of DCDC2 gene expression. A naturally occurring deletion in intron 2 of the DCDC2 gene (hereafter, DCDC2d), encompassing READ1, has been associated with DD and DD-related phenotypes,34, 37, 46, 50, 53, 54 although negative findings have also been reported.41, 55 In accordance with works showing that cognitive traits can be useful in the search for the susceptibility genes of neurodevelopmental disorders,56 two recent psychophysical studies showed that DCDC2d specifically influences the inter-individual variation in motion perception both in children with DD57, 58 and in normal readers.58 Finally, one of the most informative reports of a specific loss of CNTNAP2 function has come from a study of an old-order Amish population in which 13 probands were found to carry the same homozygous point mutation within CNTNAP2, that is, 3709delG.59 This change introduced a premature stop codon (I1253X) predicted to produce a non-functional protein.59, 60

Recent evidence has shown that DD-susceptibility genes affect neuronal migration, neurite outgrowth, cortical morphogenesis and ciliary structure and function.25, 27, 50, 61, 62, 63, 64, 65, 66, 67, 68, 69, 70, 71, 72, 73, 74, 75, 76, 77, 78, 79, 80, 81, 82 In particular, ROBO1 is known to be an axon guidance receptor regulating the connections between brain hemispheres.25, 61, 62, 63 The protein encoded by DYX1C1 has been linked to neuronal migration, estrogen receptor transport and cilia structure and functions.64, 65, 66, 71, 74, 78, 81 Animal studies showed that in utero RNAi of DYX1C1 is related to deficits in both RAP, spatial working memory performance, as well as learning and memory performance.9, 83 The expression pattern of KIAA0319 in the developing neocortex is consistent with its hypothesized role in neuronal migration, and recent bioinformatics analysis has suggested its involvement in ciliary functions.69, 70, 72, 75, 79, 80, 84 The embryonic RNAi of KIAA0319 expression results in RAP and spatial learning deficits.9, 85 The DCDC2 gene encodes a protein with two DCX domains which are essential for neurite outgrowth and neuronal migration and it is involved in ciliary functions.27, 50, 67, 81, 86 DCDC2 knockout mice show impairments in visuospatial memory, visual discrimination and long-term memory, auditory processing, working memory and reference memory.9, 87, 88 Similarly, animal studies have shown that the Glun2b subunit is required for neuronal pattern formation in general and for channel function and formation of dendritic spines in hippocampal pyramidal cells in particular.68, 89, 90, 91 Recently, DCDC2 knockout mice were shown to have increased excitability and decreased temporal precision in action potential firing,92 as well as increased functional excitator connectivity between layer 4 lateral connections in the somatosensory neocortex93 mediated by subunit Grin2B. Focused functional investigations of cellular and mouse models uncovered connections between FOXP2 and neurite outgrowth.73, 77FOXP2 was first implicated in a family segregating a severe form of dyspraxia of speech, designated the KE family.94, 95 Since its original identification, many studies reported that rare variants disrupting one copy of FOXP2 cause language-based learning (dis)abilities-related impairment.31 Mice carrying mutant Foxp2 exhibit abnormal ultrasonic vocalizations as well as other disorders including developmental delay, deficits in motor-skill learning and impairments in auditory–motor association learning.96, 97, 98, 99, 100, 101 FOXP2 encodes a forkhead domain transcription factor expressed in several brain structures102 and modulates the DNA transcription at numerous loci throughout the genome. CNTNAP2 is one of its gene targets103 and it has recently been implicated in a broad range of phenotypes including autism spectrum disorder, schizophrenia, intellectual disability, DD and language impairment.104 CNTNAP2 encodes a cell-surface neurexin protein, that is, CASPR2, implicated in neuronal connectivity at the cellular and network level, interneuron development/function, synaptic organization and activity and migration of neurons in the developing brain.104 Recently, a genetic knockout of the rodent homolog Cntnap2 has been associated with poor social interactions, behavioral perseveration and reduced vocalizations, as well as with delayed learning and cross-modal integration.105, 106 In contrast, little is known about the C2ORF3 and MRPL19 candidate genes. C2ORF3 protein is suggested to have a potential function in ribosomal RNA (rRNA) processing,107 and, as for MRPL19, is highly expressed in all areas of fetal and adult brain.108Furthermore, their expression was strongly correlated with DYX1C1, ROBO1, DCDC2 and KIAA0319 across different brain regions.108 All these findings depict DD as a disorder at the mild end of the spectrum of a number of pathways producing developmental disturbances in neuronal positioning and axonal outgrowth,109 consistent with the neuroanatomical findings of focal architectonic dysplasia and neuronal ectopias in the brains of people with DD.110

Imaging in DD

Postmortem studies in DD patients showed reduced left–right asymmetry of the planum temporale,111 as well as neuronal ectopias and architectonic dysplasias in the left perisylvian regions.110 More recently, magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) has been extensively used to investigate both morphological, structural and functional brain abnormalities in DD patients (Figure 1). Being noninvasive and allowing in vivo studies, MRI is a unique and valuable tool for disentangling tissue modifications and functional (re)organization in developmental disorders like DD. Among different MRI-based techniques, voxel-based morphometry (VBM) is used to quantify gray and white matter (GM and WM, respectively) volumes, while diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), which probes water diffusivity in the micron scale, detects alterations in WM structure and indirectly in the architecture of fiber pathways. Finally, functional MRI (fMRI) investigates brain activations during cognitive and sensory tasks, and when at rest.

Figure 1
figure 1

Rows show the findings obtained with structural and functional MR techniques in DD subjects. The size and the color of the spheres reflect the amount of papers reporting differences in the specified area. Longitudinal fascicoli and arcuate fasciculus are shown as edges. fMRI findings are not divided by task. Task specific findings are available in Supplementary Tables 1 and 2. DD, developmental dyslexia; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging. Figure was created with ExploreDTI (http://exploredti.com). DTI, diffusion tensor imaging; VBM, voxel-based morphometry.

VBM analysis

By applying VBM, altered GM density has been identified in several areas, that is, in the left temporal and parietal regions,112, 113, 114, 115, 116, 117, 118, 119 bilaterally in the fusiform gyrus, lingual gyrus, temporo-parieto-occipital junction, frontal lobe, planum temporale, inferior temporal cortex, caudate, thalamus and cerebellum,115, 118, 119, 120, 121, 122, 123, 124, 125, 126 and in the right parietal lobe.123, 125 Moreover, VBM analysis has revealed altered WM density in the bilateral temporal and frontal lobes, in the left cuneus and arcuate fasciculus, and in the right precuneus and cerebellum.113, 116, 117, 118, 119, 122, 124, 125

DTI analysis

Alterations of WM structure have been found in bilateral tracts within the frontal, temporal, occipital and parietal lobes,124, 127, 128, 129 in the superior longitudinal fasciculus,130, 131 in the left superior corona radiata, in the left centrum semiovale,132 in the left inferior frontal gyrus and temporo-parietal WM,133 in the left middle and inferior temporal gyri113 and in the left arcuate fasciculus.113, 134 Moreover, several studies have reported significant differences in the corpus callosum.135, 136

fMRI analysis

fMRI has had an important role in understanding the pathophysiology of DD by analyzing the brain areas activated while performing specific tasks. The brain activations associated with the reading process have been extensively analyzed using fMRI, as well as other reading-related functions, such as phonological processing, integration of letters and speech, visual perception and attention, working memory and acoustic stimuli.137, 138 Depending on the task performed during fMRI, several altered activation patterns have been reported.

With reading-related tasks, altered activations were found in the DD subjects in the left hemispheric temporo-parietal regions (Brodman’s areas (BAs) 20, 21, 37, superior and middle temporal gyrus, operculum, supplementary motor area), and in the bilateral frontal and occipital areas (BAs 44 and 45, inferior and middle frontal gyrus, visual areas and extrastriate cortex).139, 140, 141, 142, 143, 144, 145, 146, 147, 148

Subjects with DD showed abnormal activity during phonological tasks in the left hemispheric temporal areas (Rolandic operculum, middle and superior temporal gyrus, fusiform gyrus, planum temporale and Wernicke’s area), in bilateral parietal (superior and inferior parietal gyrus, BA40), frontal (BAs 44 and 45, middle and inferior frontal gyrus, precentral gyrus, superior medial gyrus and prefrontal cortex), occipital cortex (middle and superior occipital gyrus, lingual gyrus, calcarine sulcus, BAs 18 and 19, striate cortex), cerebellum, and right hemispheric subcortical structures (putamen, basal ganglia).149, 150, 151, 152, 153, 154, 155, 156, 157, 158, 159, 160, 161

During semantic tasks, diffuse activations have been reported in DD subjects in the left hemispheric temporal (BA22, fusiform gyrus, parahippocampal gyrus and middle and superior temporal gyrus) and occipital (V5/MT), as well as bilateral parietal (inferior parietal lobule, supramarginal gyrus), frontal (BAs 44 and 45, precentral gyrus, superior frontal gyrus) cortex, cerebellum and subcortical structures.162

Children with DD showed altered activations during auditory tasks in the right temporal areas (middle and superior temporal gyrus, BAs 41 and 42, Heschl gyrus, superior temporal cortex), anterior insular cortex, cingulate cortex, thalamus and cerebellum, in the left occipital (cuneus) and parietal (inferior parietal region, supramarginal gyrus, angular gyrus) regions and in bilateral frontal areas (supplementary motor area, inferior and middle frontal gyrus, precentral gyrus, inferior frontal sulcus, prefrontal cortex).152, 153, 163, 164, 165, 166, 167, 168, 169

Working memory-related tasks elicited altered activations in the bilateral parietal (superior parietal cortex, inferior parietal lobule) and frontal (BA46, prefrontal cortex, inferior frontal gyrus) areas in children with DD.170, 171, 172, 173

The reduced activation of the primary visual cortex, extrastriatal areas and the V5/MT area during fMRI using visual stimuli,174, 175, 176 as well as increased right frontal activation in areas 44 and 45 (ref. 152) have been consistently reported in subjects with DD. Visual spatial tasks elicited altered activation in the right temporal (temporal pole, fusiform gyrus, temporal gyrus, motor/premotor cortex) and frontal (precentral gyrus, frontal gyrus) areas, and in bilateral parietal (intraparietal sulcus, inferior and superior parietal lobes, precuneus), occipital (cuneus, BAs 17–19), subcortical structures (putamen, basal ganglia), anterior cingulate and cerebellum.157, 166, 177, 178

Altered activations in bilateral temporal (inferior temporal cortex), parietal, frontal (middle frontal cortex), occipital (striate and extrastriate visual cortex) and cingulate cortex have been reported during attentional tasks in children with DD.179, 180, 181

Interestingly, the fMRI activation patterns in response to tasks requiring the processing of several demands (visuospatial, orthographic, phonologic and semantic) showed that subjects with DD tend to process using the visuospatial areas instead of the normal language processing areas.150, 169

Results of imaging studies on pre-reading children at risk for DD are in agreement with results found for children with DD,182, 183, 184, 185 suggesting that neural alterations in DD predate reading onset, reflect the differential developmental trajectory of reading brain networks and may serve as early biomarkers of risk for DD.

Given the heterogeneity of imaging modalities and findings, it is difficult to summarize MR results into a unifying perspective (Figure 1). According to previous findings showing a consistent link between reading and both subcortical structures and cortical systems, structural techniques (VBM and DTI) identify temporo-parietal and, partially, middle frontal areas as the targets of cerebral derangement that may occur in DD, whereas more anterior and occipital areas seem to be less frequently involved. It is even harder to sum up the findings derived from functional MR studies. In broad terms, a pattern of cerebral hypoactivation seem to prevail over hyperactivity during task-based fMRI. Circuits involving temporo-basal, parietal and frontal lobes are more frequently impaired, without a clear lateralization between the left and right hemispheres.

The details about the study design and results are reported in Supplementary Information 1 and 2.

Imaging–genetics in DD

Taken together, these findings show how neuroimaging and genetic research have substantially enhanced understanding of the mechanisms underlying atypical reading development. Despite the successful characterization of DD-susceptibility genes, we are far from achieving a comprehensive understanding of the pathways underlying the development of DD.186 By focusing mainly on clinical phenotypes, the molecular genetics approach has yielded mixed results,187 including negative findings for the DD-candidate genes.42, 188, 189, 190 This could be ascribed to at least three possible sources: (1) as complex traits are substantially polygenic, with each variant having a small effect, larger sample sizes are needed,191 (2) the pathway from genes to phenotypes is not straightforward (see for example, ‘the missing heritability problem’)192 and can be influenced by incomplete linkage disequilibrium between causal variants and genotyped SNPs,193 environmental, gene-by-gene and gene-by-environment effects,2, 186 (3) it is unlikely that a single model connects all the DD-candidate genes and their corresponding proteins at the molecular level, therefore several etiological cascades involved in neuronal migration and neurite outgrowth contributing to DD likely exist.194

An alternative approach is to focus on the phenotypes thought to reflect lower-level processes, hypothesizing that individual differences in the areas responsible for reading acquisition might be important end points, better reflective of the underlying biology and more tractable to genetic mapping than behavioral phenotypes.56, 195 In addition, the brain is the most complex of all organs, and behavior is not merely the sum of the phenotypic output of complex interactions within and between endogenous and exogenous environments during development. Therefore, more optimally reduced measures of functioning (hereafter, intermediate phenotypes—IPs) should be more useful than behavioral ‘macros’ in studies pursuing the biological and genetic components of neurodevelopmental disorders.196 Genetic determination of an IP will likely be less complex than determination of the related behavioral/clinical phenotype, as the latter incorporates multiple neural systems and is influenced by multiple genes and environmental etiologic variables.186 Even if concerns have been raised about how to interpret the relationship between IPs and psychiatric disorders,197 such use of IPs has had a crucial role in improving the knowledge of the gene to phenotype gap in other neurodevelopmental disorders (for example, schizophrenia—SKZ, autism spectrum disorder).195

Imaging data provide a viable IP for complex neurobehavioral disorders like DD, reducing the inherent complexity of brain functioning and of the intricate clinical outcome of these disorders.56, 196, 197, 198 Performing joint genetic and neuroimaging studies in humans, where the association between genotypes and brain phenotypes can be tested, is an emerging strategy to link DD-candidate genes to brain structure and function. To date, imaging–genetic studies, including both structural and functional imaging, have focused on at least one of the above-described DD-candidate genes and on the proposed functional variants spanning them (Table 1).17, 199, 200, 201, 202, 203, 204, 205, 206, 207, 208, 209, 210, 211, 212, 213, 214, 215 Although some of the studies involving DD-candidate genes have been carried out on populations other than DD (that is, healthy subjects, SKZ), they have been taken into consideration for the purpose of this review, that is, to propose an interdisciplinary, multilevel, imaging–genetic approach to disentangle the pathways from genes to behavior, by focusing on selective, functional genetic variants and particular, well-defined cognitive/sensorial phenotypes. Structural MRI studies have shown that in subjects with SKZ and controls, DYX1C1 and KIAA0319 genes are significantly correlated with the inferior and superior cerebellar networks,201 with WM volume in the left temporo-parietal region,203, 204 and with cortical thickness in the left orbitofrontal region in typically developing children.208 A pilot resting-state fMRI study failed to find a significant link between DYX1C1 markers and functional connectivity of language-related regions in both subjects with SKZ and healthy controls.202 Functional MRI studies showed associations between KIAA0319 and asymmetry in functional activation of the superior temporal sulcus,205 and the inter-individual variability in activation of reading-related regions of interest (that is, the right and left anterior inferior parietal lobe)199 during reading-related tasks in two independent samples of subjects with DD and normal readers. Moreover, KIAA0319 was found to influence functional connectivity in language-related regions (that is, a left Broca-superior/inferior parietal network, a left Wernicke-fronto-occipital network and a bilateral Wernicke-fronto-parietal network) in both subjects with SKZ and healthy controls.202 In healthy adults, an allelic variation in the DCDC2 gene has been associated with individual differences in cortical thickness,204 and in fiber tracts, which are commonly found altered in neuroimaging studies of reading and DD (that is, the connection of the left medial temporal gyrus with the angular and supramarginal gyri, the superior longitudinal fasciculus and the corpus callosum).203 Interestingly, in a sample of subjects with SKZ and controls, DCDC2 was found to be associated with distributed cortical structural abnormalities in language-related superior prefrontal, temporal and occipital networks,201 and with inter-individual variations in functional connectivity in a Broca-medial parietal network.202 Furthermore, in healthy adults, DCDC2d has been associated with altered GM volumes in reading/language-related brain regions especially in the left hemisphere,200 and with both common and unique alterations of WM fiber tracts in subjects with DD.207 In an fMRI study, Cope et al.199 found significant associations between DCDC2-READ1 and brain activations in the left antero-inferior parietal lobe and in the right lateral occipital temporal gyrus during reading tasks, and a nominally significant association between DCDC2d and activation in the left antero-inferior parietal lobule. Further imaging–genetic studies investigated the effects of C2Orf3/MRPL19 and GRIN2B genes upon neuroanatomical structures. By using VBM, Scerri et al.206 revealed that WM volume in the bilaterally posterior part of the corpus callosum and the cingulum varied depending on one variant in the C2Orf3/MRPL19 region. Finally, in healthy individuals, GRIN2B correlated negatively with dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activity during a working-memory-related task.209 Imaging–genetics of FOXP2 and CNTNAP2 has implicated common genetic variants spanning these genes. Multiple imaging studies of the KE family have found both structural and functional alterations in subjects with dyspraxia of speech and the mutant FOXP2.216, 217, 218, 219 Even if no evidence for effects of FOXP2 on variability in brain structures in a sample of >1300 people from the general population have been recently reported,210 common variants spanning this gene were associated with altered levels of activation in temporo-parietal and inferior frontal brain areas during both reading and speech listening tasks in DD samples.17, 205 CNTNAP2 has been associated with structural brain connectivity and brain activation in BA7, BA44 and BA21 during a language processing task in healthy individuals.211, 212 Moreover, it has been significantly associated with FA in the uncinate fasciculus of subjects with SKZ,213 with reduction of GM and WM volume and lower FA in the cerebellum, fusiform gyrus, occipital and frontal cortices,214 and with modulation in functional frontal lobar connectivity215 in subjects with a diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder.

Table 1 Imaging genetic studies in DD

Limitations of current imaging–genetic studies

Clearly, neuroimaging is playing a fundamental part in disentangling the role of genetic variants in the etiology of complex cognitive functions like reading. However, the complexity of the ‘reading circuit’ is still far from being completely understood, as revealed by the heterogeneous and sometimes conflicting results of brain MRI studies.

Study design and data processing are important factors increasing complexity and heterogeneity in neuroimaging research. The inclusion of subjects with an unknown genetic profile will likely enhance inter-subject variability, as different DD genes may cause different deficits in different, particular cognitive and sensorial phenotypes (see ‘Genetics of DD’ paragraph). Nevertheless, even if some imaging–genetic studies of DD have been proposed,17, 199, 200, 201, 202, 203, 204, 205, 206, 207, 208, 209, 210, 211, 212, 213, 214, 215 the number of these works is still too low to draw definitive conclusions about the role of each DD-candidate gene.

Moreover, it is interesting to note some technical evidence that might limit the integration of these results. Of the 19 aforementioned imaging–genetic studies, 10 have used 1.5T scanners,199, 203, 204, 205, 206 eight were performed with 3T scanners200, 201, 202, 205, 207, 208, 209, 215 and one acquired with a 4T scanner.211 Two of them used similar acquisition protocols and performed VBM to investigate GM,200, 201 but their results were only partially overlapping. These different findings may be owing to the different disorders included in the studies (that is, DD and SKZ) and/or to the different analysis pipelines (linear regression versus independent component analysis). Genetic data can be integrated with every parametric map derived from MRI, whether a simple measure of volume, a microstructure-related metric or a measure of chemical properties. Three of the aforementioned studies integrated genetic data in the VBM analysis of WM volume as an attempt to reveal genetically related alterations, limiting the analysis of DTI data to the detection of the major fiber bundles included in altered WM areas.203, 204, 206 Nevertheless, DTI analysis can provide parameters that are more specific to WM microstructure than VBM,220 including fractional anisotropy (FA) and measures of diffusivity along different spatial axes. These maps can be analyzed similarly to VBM, but may provide additional characterization of the genetic effect at the microstructural level. To date, only three studies have used DTI-derived maps to detect voxel-based WM modifications related to DD-candidate genes.207, 208, 214 One of the studies213 computed FA maps and tried to perform region-of-interest-based analysis of covariance regression with the SNPs of CNTNAP2; however, only one genotype was a significant predictor of FA in the uncinate fasciculus after Bonferroni correction, despite the relatively high number of subjects included in the study (n=125). Further studies with rigorous advanced diffusion MRI protocols (that is, high-field magnets, multiple directions and b-values) and populations with a specific genetic characterization are therefore needed. Moreover, more complex diffusion-based techniques, such as NODDI (Neurite Orientation Dispersion and Density Imaging), have recently provided more specific metrics of GM and WM in several applications.221, 222, 223 The application of NODDI or other affine techniques might be beneficial to the study of DD, providing additional disentanglement of the connections between genetic variations and structural alterations.

Similar considerations apply to fMRI, where the choices of stimuli and the analysis pipeline are fundamental. To date, functional imaging–genetic studies of DD have investigated the effects of DD-candidate genes only during reading tasks,199, 205, 209 irrespective of the deficits each DD gene is likely to produce (see ‘Genetics of DD’ paragraph). Moreover, while task-based fMRI might help investigate the effects of DD-candidate genes on specific brain functions through correlation analysis or linear regressions, resting-state fMRI might offer a more reproducible/reliable approach to the investigation of genetic effects on brain functionality. It is worth noticing that while imaging–genetic studies are at their early stages in DD, they are more popular in the context of other diseaes.224, 225, 226, 227 For example, the ADNI (Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative)228 has performed MRI and positron emission tomography acquisitions with genetic profiling in more than 1000 subjects over time. Along with genetic profiling, the success of the initiative is strongly supported by the standardization of multicentric acquisition protocol and processing methods, all factors that are unfortunately still lacking in imaging–genetic studies on DD.

Toward a new approach

As aforementioned, learning to read requires the accurate, fast and timely integration of different neural systems supporting different cognitive and sensorial processes. Molecular genetic studies have consistently identified DD-candidate genes and provided initial evidence of the presence of putative functional genetic variants influencing gene expression. Recent findings in both animal and humans studies support the role of specific genetic variants on the different cognitive and sensorial processes underlying reading acquisition. Similarly, neuroimaging data can be considered IPs to genetics in identifying the causes of DD.198 New studies must therefore gain momentum to understand the function of neuronal migration genes and their relationships with specific cognitive and sensorial vulnerability, and to establish links between such susceptibility variants and neuroanatomical phenotypes. Following a probabilistic and multifactorial etiological model of reading acquisition, the emergence of DD is rooted at multiple levels, and may reflect the global failure of interacting mechanisms, each with degrees of impairment that vary across children.2, 186, 229, 230, 231, 232 It is therefore reasonable to predict a low specificity and high heterogeneity of imaging findings, especially when dealing with small sample sizes. Furthermore, according to this model, the fundamental role of genetics in the selection of homogeneous DD subtypes population suitable for imaging investigation appears reasonable. The integration of specific cognitive/sensorial, selective genetic and imaging data can lead to the identification of regions with gene- and cognitive/sensorial-specific effects (that is, only a risk genetic variant alters structure/function in this region tapping specific cognitive/sensorial mechanisms) or with universal effects (that is, all/many-risk gene function in this region). Identifying the dots connecting putative functional genetic variants, neuroanatomical structures and functions, and reading-related cognitive/sensorial pathways, will be important areas for imaging–genetics research in the future and will pave the way for new candidate gene–candidate phenotype imaging association studies.4 However, some have argued that neuroimaging studies reporting effects of candidate genes are also at risk for false-positive effects due to small sample sizes, and questions about the statistical power of imaging techniques may be risen.233, 234 Some possible strategies could be used to overcome such variability. First, accordingly to what is proposed in this review, an alternative way to avoid false positives is to focus on selective variants with known molecular function and to take into account the increment in effect sizes enabled by careful selection of phenotypes.235, 236 By narrowing the search space to genes that are likely to have a role—and whose functions have more chance of being understood—the power of the study is directly increased, as is its practical value for neuroscience and medicine.235 The identification of what constitutes a phenotype is crucial as the identification of the phenotype itself. Going beyond classical association studies, where heterogeneous patient groups selected by clinical symptoms are compared with controls, is crucial to identify reliable biomarkers and to guide the diagnosis of neurodevelopmental disorders.4 More specific, elementary, straightforward IPs may help to interpret the results of genetic studies of psychiatric diseases,233 increasing the statistical power in smaller sample size.236 Recent studies on relatively small samples show that using IPs can be very useful for researching susceptibility genes in DD26, 237, 238 and for explaining their effects on the phenotypic variance.35, 57, 58 Second, there is a growing perception of reproducibility as a fundamental building block in science. Some have argued that small individual studies—when replicated—may lead to useful observations to address the impact of genetic variation on a neural system that is abnormal in a given illness, despite the problem of false-positive findings. An alternative strategy is to recruit large data sets through multicenter studies. Many neuroimaging consortia have been recently established (for example, the ADNI, the functional Brain Imaging Research Network, the Mind Clinical Imaging Consortium, the Enhancing NeuroImaging Genetics through Meta-Analysis consortium, the Pediatric Imaging Neurocognition Genetics study) to expand the promise of imaging–genetic studies and to detect factors that affect the brain that could hardly be detected by single site studies.12, 235 However, as some limitations apply (for example, it is difficult to aggregate data from cohorts that are heterogeneous in terms of duration of illness and demographics, spoken languages, ethnic differences in allele frequency), novel, harmonized data analysis and meta-analysis protocols checking for the effects of possible confounders, are crucial to the success of these projects.235, 239 Third, it would help to develop an interdisciplinary multilevel approach aimed at defining MRI protocols heavily guided by genetics and cognitive findings. The best outcomes result from cooperation within a multidisciplinary team to address the different levels of investigation underlying such complex neurodevelopmental disorders.240, 241 Nonetheless, addressing the statistical power problem in imaging studies is nontrivial. We depicted DD as a heterogeneous disease, and the MRI findings also reported the same to date (Figure 1). Generally speaking, the estimation of the minimum sample size required to highlight structural or functional imaging alteration is prohibitive. One may argue that some areas, that have been reported more consistently in literature, are more consistently altered and thus require a smaller sample size to be detected. The problem is worsened by the variability introduced by MRI techniques and methods as the multiple comparisons correction, that greatly limits the comparability of results across studies. New candidate gene–candidate phenotype imaging association studies should integrate investigations of the effects of selective genetic variants upon neuroanatomical pathways underlying the specific reading-related cognitive and sensorial processes each gene is supposed to target by applying the most sensitive and robust neuroimaging techniques. Future hypothesis-driven imaging–genetic studies should therefore take advantage of recent genetic findings in both animal and human studies to focus their attention on innovative interdisciplinary analyses of well-defined, specific cognitive and sensorial, imaging and selective genetic data. In this way, the effect of a known genetic diversity, naturally occurring among human populations, is studied by brain imaging to determine whether one of its forms can cause a difference in the level of such cognitive/sensorial phenotypes and hence could make people more vulnerable to neurodevelopmental disorders.4 A fruitful outcome is particularly possible when fMRI is used to examine the neurobiological effect of a well-validated gene. If DD-candidate genetic variants are selectively associated with inter-individual variation in one of the reading-related processes at brain level, children carrying these genetic variants would be considered as ‘biologically at-risk’. Early identification of these children would be crucial to defining adequate and well-timed prevention strategies.197, 242 Furthermore, candidate gene–candidate phenotype might be fundamental to understanding the relationship between traditional diagnostic categories and the new classifications of mental disorders based on dimensions of observable behavior and neurobiological measures.186, 187, 195, 196, 198 Neuroimaging may provide evidence for or against existing theories, or provide unique and sensitive insight unexplained solely by behavioral measures.198 Although producing interesting results, the hypothesis-driven approach of imaging genetics represents a way for validation/replication studies of selective genes and do not reveal other genetic contributors to the overall neurobehavioral reading deficits nor the imaging phenotype changes associated with DD.4, 12, 31 By implementing a ‘gene hunting’ strategy,4 hypothesis-free approach, similar to those commonly seen in human genetics such as genome-wide association studies and new DNA sequencing technologies, could detect common variants with small effect sizes and could reveal new genes and pathways, rare and de novo variants, that contribute to alterations in brain imaging phenotypes, and how they contribute to the ultimate neurobehavioral phenotypes.12, 31, 235 However, the question that arises from imaging–genetics as a hypothesis-free field is how to use and analyze such large and diverse datasets. Data reduction or hypothesis-free processing methods, such as parallel independent component analysis,201, 202 multivariate pattern analysis,227 endophenotype ranking value,243 polygenic risk score,244 as well as new analytical methods to collapse and/or integrate a variety of data types into relevant risk models (for example, support vector machine analysis) are potentially needed.

Conclusion

This review aimed to highlight the promising imaging–genetics approach as a way to unravel new insights behind the pathophysiology of reading (dis)ability. As the presence of putative functional genetic variants influencing the expression of some of the DD-candidate genes has been provided and as genetic associations with specific, well-defined cognitive/sensorial mechanisms have been reported, current knowledge of genetics of DD could help target imaging more selectively. The integration of particular cognitive/sensorial, selective genetic and imaging data, as well as the implementation of candidate gene–candidate phenotype imaging association studies would result in a better consideration of what constitutes a phenotype. Clearly, such an approach is essentially interdisciplinary given the multiple levels of analysis simultaneously achieved. Even if there are weaknesses despite strengths in this perspective, such hypothesis-driven approach in imaging–genetics as a field would lead to the optimization of criteria to diagnose DD and to the early identification of ‘biologically at-risk’ children. This means the definition of adequate and well-timed prevention strategies and the implementation of novel, specific and evidence-based remediation approach training specifically the reading-related cognitive/sensorial impairment. These insights will aid in the earlier detection of children with DD and aid their overall academic and remediation potential. Naturally, these developments should be considered in parallel with the advance made by the hypothesis-free approach that will aid in the identification of new mechanisms (genetic and imaging) that contribute to reading deficits in DD.