Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Review Article
  • Published:

Factors that amplify and attenuate egocentric mentalizing

Abstract

Egocentrism is a hallmark of human mentalizing endeavours. People frequently use their own minds as a point of departure when generating inferences about the minds of others. Although this starting point is rarely the end point, self-referential information often persists in biasing social inferences. In this Review, we describe models that can account for egocentric mentalizing in adults. We then identify factors that amplify and attenuate egocentrism in reasoning about the content of other minds. Specifically, we consider features of mentalizing targets that determine the extent to which they are socially proximal versus distant and, therefore, the extent to which they activate self-information; features of mentalizers that influence their ability or motivation to override an egocentric default; and features that can be deliberately modified to attenuate egocentrism during mentalizing. Finally, we conclude with several open questions that point to promising directions for future research in this area.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: Accounts of egocentric mentalizing.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Piaget, J. The Child’s Conception of the World (Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner, 1926).

  2. Rakoczy, H. Foundations of theory of mind and its development in early childhood. Nat. Rev. Psychol. 1, 223–235 (2022).

    Google Scholar 

  3. Birch, S. A. & Bloom, P. Understanding children’s and adults’ limitations in mental state reasoning. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 255–260 (2004).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Nickerson, R. S. How we know — and sometimes misjudge — what others know: imputing one’s own knowledge to others. Psychol. Bull. 125, 737–759 (1999).

    Google Scholar 

  5. Royzman, E. B., Cassidy, K. W. & Baron, J. “I know, you know”: epistemic egocentrism in children and adults. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 7, 38–65 (2003). This article reviews evidence of egocentric mentalizing from the developmental literature on theory of mind in children and from the judgment and decision-making literature on heuristics and biases in adults.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Goldman, A. I. Simulating Minds: the Philosophy, Psychology, and Neuroscience of Mindreading (Oxford Univ. Press, 2006).

  7. Katz, D. & Allport, F. H. Students’ Attitudes (Craftsman, 1931).

  8. Cronbach, L. J. Processes affecting scores on understanding of others and assumed similarity. Psychol. Bull. 52, 177–193 (1955).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Holmes, D. S. Dimensions of projection. Psychol. Bull. 69, 248–268 (1968).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Buckner, R. L. & Carroll, D. C. Self-projection and the brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 49–57 (2007).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Ross, L., Greene, D. & House, P. The false consensus effect: an egocentric bias in social perception and attribution processes. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 13, 279–301 (1977).

    Google Scholar 

  12. Fischhoff, B. Hindsight is not equal to foresight: the effect of outcome knowledge on judgment under uncertainty. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1, 288–299 (1975).

    Google Scholar 

  13. Camerer, C., Loewenstein, G. & Weber, M. The curse of knowledge in economic settings: an experimental analysis. J. Pol. Econ. 97, 1232–1254 (1989).

    Google Scholar 

  14. Chambers, J. R., Epley, N., Savitsky, K. & Windschitl, P. D. Knowing too much: using private knowledge to predict how one is viewed by others. Psychol. Sci. 19, 542–548 (2008).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Vorauer, J. D. & Claude, S. D. Perceived versus actual transparency of goals in negotiation. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 24, 371 (1998).

    Google Scholar 

  16. Gilovich, T., Medvec, V. H. & Savitsky, K. The spotlight effect in social judgment: an egocentric bias in estimates of the salience of one’s own actions and appearance. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 78, 211–222 (2000).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Gilovich, T., Savitsky, K. & Medvec, V. H. The illusion of transparency: biased assessments of others’ ability to read one’s emotional states. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 75, 332–346 (1998).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Bernstein, D. M., Atance, C., Loftus, G. R. & Meltzoff, A. We saw it all along: visual hindsight bias in children and adults. Psychol. Sci. 15, 264–267 (2004).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Birch, S. A. & Bloom, P. The curse of knowledge in reasoning about false beliefs. Psychol. Sci. 18, 382–386 (2007).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Crosby, J. R., King, M. & Savitsky, K. The minority spotlight effect. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 5, 743–750 (2014).

    Google Scholar 

  21. Krueger, J. & Clement, R. W. The truly false consensus effect: an ineradicable and egocentric bias in social perception. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 67, 596–610 (1994).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Schaerer, M. et al. The illusion of transparency in performance appraisals: when and why accuracy motivation explains unintentional feedback inflation. Organ. Behav. Hum. Dec. Process. 144, 171–186 (2018).

    Google Scholar 

  23. Van Boven, L. & Loewenstein, G. Social projection of transient drive states. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 29, 1159–1168 (2003).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Critcher, C. R. & Dunning, D. Egocentric pattern projection: how implicit personality theories recapitulate the geography of the self. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 97, 1–16 (2009).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. De Lillo, M. & Ferguson, H. J. Perspective-taking and social inferences in adolescents, young adults and older adults. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 152, 1420–1438 (2023).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Luce, K. & Almor, A. Inconsistency in perspective-taking during comprehension. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 30, 2351–2362 (2023).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Warnell, K. R. & Redcay, E. Minimal coherence among varied theory of mind measures in childhood and adulthood. Cognition 191, 103997 (2019).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Qureshi, A. W., Monk, R. L., Samson, D. & Apperly, I. A. Does interference between self and other perspectives in theory of mind tasks reflect a common underlying process? Evidence from individual differences in theory of mind and inhibitory control. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 27, 178–190 (2020).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Wang, J. J., Tseng, P., Juan, C.-H., Frisson, S. & Apperly, I. A. Perspective-taking across cultures: shared biases in Taiwanese and British adults. R. Soc. Open. Sci. 6, 190540 (2019).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  ADS  Google Scholar 

  30. Apperly, I. A., Back, E., Samson, D. & France, L. The cost of thinking about false beliefs: evidence from adults’ performance on a non-inferential theory of mind task. Cognition 106, 1093–1108 (2008).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Meltzoff, A. N. ‘Like me’: a foundation for social cognition. Dev. Sci. 10, 126–134 (2007).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Gordon, R. M. The simulation theory: objections and misconceptions. Mind Lang. 7, 11–34 (1992).

    Google Scholar 

  33. Shanton, K. & Goldman, A. Simulation theory. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 1, 527–538 (2010).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Waytz, A. & Mitchell, J. P. Two mechanisms for simulating other minds: dissociations between mirroring and self-projection. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 20, 197–200 (2011).

    Google Scholar 

  35. Epley, N., Keysar, B., Van Boven, L. & Gilovich, T. Perspective taking as egocentric anchoring and adjustment. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 87, 327–339 (2004). This study uses response times and time pressure to show that adults adjust from an egocentric anchor during mentalizing and that adjustment stops once a plausible estimate is reached.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Leslie, A. M., Friedman, O. & German, T. P. Core mechanisms in ‘theory of mind’. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 528–533 (2004).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Qureshi, A. W., Apperly, I. A. & Samson, D. Executive function is necessary for perspective selection, not level-1 visual perspective calculation: evidence from a dual-task study of adults. Cognition 117, 230–236 (2010).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Leslie, A. M., German, T. P. & Polizzi, P. Belief-desire reasoning as a process of selection. Cogn. Psychol. 50, 45–85 (2005).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Friedman, O. & Leslie, A. M. Mechanisms of belief-desire reasoning: inhibition and bias. Psychol. Sci. 15, 547–552 (2004).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Keysar, B., Barr, D. J., Balin, J. A. & Brauner, J. S. Taking perspective in conversation: the role of mutual knowledge in comprehension. Psychol. Sci. 11, 32–38 (2000).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Keysar, B., Lin, S. & Barr, D. J. Limits on theory of mind use in adults. Cognition 89, 25–41 (2003).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Tamir, D. I. & Mitchell, J. P. Anchoring and adjustment during social inferences. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 142, 151–162 (2013). This study uses response time data to show that mentalizers anchor on self-referential information and then serially adjust away, particularly for similar targets.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Wang, Y. A., Simpson, A. J. & Todd, A. R. Egocentric anchoring-and-adjustment underlies social inferences about known others varying in similarity and familiarity. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 152, 597–610 (2023).

    Google Scholar 

  44. Decety, J. & Sommerville, J. A. Shared representations between self and other: a social cognitive neuroscience view. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 527–533 (2003).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Spengler, S., von Cramon, D. Y. & Brass, M. Control of shared representations relies on key processes involved in mental state attribution. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 3704–3718 (2009). 

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Steinbeis, N. The role of self–other distinction in understanding others’ mental and emotional states: neurocognitive mechanisms in children and adults. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. 371, 20150074 (2016). This article highlights the key premise that people must distinguish between self and other to overcome egocentric defaults, an ability that first emerges in early childhood.

    Google Scholar 

  47. Tamir, D. I. & Mitchell, J. P. Neural correlates of anchoring-and-adjustment during mentalizing. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 107, 10827–10832 (2010).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  ADS  Google Scholar 

  48. Hartwright, C. E., Apperly, I. A. & Hansen, P. C. Multiple roles for executive control in belief–desire reasoning: distinct neural networks are recruited for self perspective inhibition and complexity of reasoning. NeuroImage 61, 921–930 (2012).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Hartwright, C. E., Apperly, I. A. & Hansen, P. C. The special case of self-perspective inhibition in mental, but not non-mental, representation. Neuropsychologia 67, 183–192 (2015).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Van der Meer, L., Groenewold, N. A., Nolen, W. A., Pijnenborg, M. & Aleman, A. Inhibit yourself and understand the other: neural basis of distinct processes underlying theory of mind. Neuroimage 56, 2364–2374 (2011).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. McCleery, J. P., Surtees, A. D., Graham, K. A., Richards, J. E. & Apperly, I. A. The neural and cognitive time course of theory of mind. J. Neurosci. 31, 12849–12854 (2011).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Ramsey, R., Hansen, P., Apperly, I. & Samson, D. Seeing it my way or your way: frontoparietal brain areas sustain viewpoint-independent perspective selection processes. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 25, 670–684 (2013).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Reidler, J. S., Huang, C. & Buckner, R. L. Evidence for the default network’s role in spontaneous cognition. J. Neurophysiol. 104, 322–335 (2010).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Meyer, M. L. & Lieberman, M. D. Why people are always thinking about themselves: medial prefrontal cortex activity during rest primes self-referential processing. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 30, 714–721 (2018).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Mildner, J. N. & Tamir, D. I. The people around you are inside your head: social context shapes spontaneous thought. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 150, 2375–2386 (2021).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Mildner, J. & Tamir, D. How does real-world social isolation affect spontaneous thought? Preprint at PsyArXiv https://osf.io/preprints/psyarxiv/uv4cm/ (2022).

  57. Jenkins, A. C., Macrae, C. N. & Mitchell, J. P. Repetition suppression of ventromedial prefrontal activity during judgments of self and others. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 105, 4507–4512 (2008).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  ADS  Google Scholar 

  58. Mitchell, J. P., Banaji, M. R. & Macrae, C. N. The link between social cognition and self-referential thought in the medial prefrontal cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 1306–1315 (2005).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Kawamichi, H. et al. Medial prefrontal cortex activation is commonly invoked by reputation of self and romantic partners. PLoS ONE 8, e74958 (2013).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  ADS  Google Scholar 

  60. Saxe, R. in Handbook of Theory of Mind (eds Leslie, A. & German, T.) 1–35 (Psychology Press, 2010).

  61. Young, L., Camprodon, J. A., Hauser, M., Pascual-Leone, A. & Saxe, R. Disruption of the right temporoparietal junction with transcranial magnetic stimulation reduces the role of beliefs in moral judgments. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 107, 6753 (2010).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  ADS  Google Scholar 

  62. Welborn, B. L. & Lieberman, M. D. Person-specific theory of mind in medial pFC. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 27, 1–12 (2015).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Samson, D., Houthuys, S. & Humphreys, G. W. Self-perspective inhibition deficits cannot be explained by general executive control difficulties. Cortex 70, 189–201 (2015).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Greenwald, A. G. The totalitarian ego. Am. Psychol. 35, 603–618 (1980).

    Google Scholar 

  65. Dunning, D. & Hayes, A. F. Evidence for egocentric comparison in social judgment. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 71, 213–229 (1996).

    Google Scholar 

  66. Higgins, E. T. in Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles (eds Higgins, E. T. & Kruglanski, A. W.) 133–168 (1996).

  67. Aron, A., Aron, E. N., Tudor, M. & Nelson, G. Close relationships as including other in the self. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 60, 241–253 (1991).

    Google Scholar 

  68. Slotter, E. B. & Gardner, W. L. Where do you end and I begin? Evidence for anticipatory, motivated self–other integration between relationship partners. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 96, 1137–1151 (2009).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Vorauer, J. D. & Cameron, J. J. So close, and yet so far: does collectivism foster transparency overestimation? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 83, 1344–1352 (2002).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Cikara, M., Bruneau, E. G. & Saxe, R. R. Us and them: intergroup failures of empathy. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 20, 149–153 (2011).

    Google Scholar 

  71. Cikara, M. & Fiske, S. T. Bounded empathy: neural responses to outgroup targets’ (mis)fortunes. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 3791–3803 (2011).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  72. Denning, K. R. & Hodges, S. D. When polarization triggers out-group “counter-projection” across the political divide. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 48, 638–656 (2022).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Weisbuch, M. & Ambady, N. Affective divergence: automatic responses to others’ emotions depend on group membership. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 95, 1063–1079 (2008).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. McPherson, M., Smith-Lovin, L. & Cook, J. M. Birds of a feather: homophily in social networks. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 27, 415–444 (2001).

    Google Scholar 

  75. Liviatan, I., Trope, Y. & Liberman, N. Interpersonal similarity as a social distance dimension: implications for perception of others’ actions. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 44, 1256–1269 (2008).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  76. Trope, Y. & Liberman, N. Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychol. Rev. 117, 440–463 (2010).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  77. Smith, E. R. & Henry, S. An in-group becomes part of the self: response time evidence. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 22, 635–642 (1996).

    Google Scholar 

  78. Smith, E. R., Coats, S. & Walling, D. Overlapping mental representations of self, in-group, and partner: further response time evidence and a connectionist model. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 25, 873–882 (1999).

    Google Scholar 

  79. Mussweiler, T. & Bodenhausen, G. V. I know you are, but what am I? Self-evaluative consequences of judging in-group and out-group members. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 82, 19–32 (2002).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Marr, D., Willshaw, D. & McNaughton, B. Simple Memory: a Theory for Archicortex (Springer, 1991).

  81. Mills, C., Herrera-Bennett, A., Faber, M. & Christoff, K. in The Oxford Handbook of Spontaneous Thought: Mind-Wandering, Creativity, and Dreaming (eds Christoff, K. & Fox, K. C. R.) 11–22 (2018).

  82. Smith, E. R. & Mackie, D. M. Representation and incorporation of close others’ responses: the RICOR model of social influence. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 20, 311–331 (2016).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Rubin-McGregor, J., Zhao, Z. & Tamir, D. I. Simulation induces durable, extensive changes to self-knowledge. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 98, 104229 (2022).

    Google Scholar 

  84. Schneider, M., Rubin-McGregor, J., Elder, J., Hughes, B. & Tamir, D. Simulation requires activation of self-knowledge to change self-concept. Preprint at PsyArXiv https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/92mru (2022).

  85. Ames, D. R. Inside the mind reader’s tool kit: projection and stereotyping in mental state inference. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 87, 340–353 (2004). This study highlights the role of target similarity in shaping mentalizing strategies, with adults using self-projection more for similar targets and stereotyping more for dissimilar targets.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Ames, D. R., Weber, E. U. & Zou, X. Mind-reading in strategic interaction: the impact of perceived similarity on projection and stereotyping. Organ. Behav. Hum. Dec. Process. 117, 96–110 (2012).

    Google Scholar 

  87. Mitchell, J. P., Macrae, C. N. & Banaji, M. R. Dissociable medial prefrontal contributions to judgments of similar and dissimilar others. Neuron 50, 655–663 (2006).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Moore, W. E. III, Merchant, J. S., Kahn, L. E. & Pfeifer, J. H. ‘Like me?’: ventromedial prefrontal cortex is sensitive to both personal relevance and self-similarity during social comparisons. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 421–426 (2014).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Ames, D. R. & Iyengar, S. S. Appraising the unusual: framing effects and moderators of uniqueness-seeking and social projection. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 41, 271–282 (2005).

    Google Scholar 

  90. Ames, D. R. Strategies for social inference: a similarity contingency model of projection and stereotyping in attribute prevalence estimates. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 87, 573–585 (2004).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Schul, Y. & Vinokur, A. D. Projection in person perception among spouses as a function of the similarity in their shared experiences. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 26, 987–1001 (2000).

    Google Scholar 

  92. Davis, M. H. Social projection to liked and disliked targets: the role of perceived similarity. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 70, 286–293 (2017).

    Google Scholar 

  93. Machunsky, M., Toma, C., Yzerbyt, V. & Corneille, O. Social projection increases for positive targets: ascertaining the effect and exploring its antecedents. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 40, 1373–1388 (2014).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  94. Machunsky, M. & Walther, E. Of Caucasians, Asians, and giraffes: the influence of categorization and target valence on social projection. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 41, 1236–1246 (2015).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  95. Marks, G. & Miller, N. Target attractiveness as a mediator of assumed attitude similarity. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 8, 728–735 (1982).

    Google Scholar 

  96. Toma, C., Yzerbyt, V. & Corneille, O. Anticipated cooperation vs. competition moderates interpersonal projection. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 46, 375–381 (2010).

    Google Scholar 

  97. Riketta, M. & Sacramento, C. A. ‘They cooperate with us, so they are like me’: perceived intergroup relationship moderates projection from self to outgroups. Group Process. Intergr. Relat. 11, 115–131 (2008).

    Google Scholar 

  98. Echterhoff, G., Higgins, E. T. & Levine, J. M. Shared reality: experiencing commonality with others’ inner states about the world. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 4, 496–521 (2009).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  99. Sinclair, S., Huntsinger, J., Skorinko, J. & Hardin, C. D. Social tuning of the self: consequences for the self-evaluations of stereotype targets. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 89, 160–175 (2005).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  100. Toma, C., Corneille, O. & Yzerbyt, V. Holding a mirror up to the self: egocentric similarity beliefs underlie social projection in cooperation. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 38, 1259–1271 (2012).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  101. Murray, S. L., Holmes, J. G., Bellavia, G., Griffin, D. W. & Dolderman, D. Kindred spirits? The benefits of egocentrism in close relationships. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 82, 563–581 (2002).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  102. Chen, L., Mo, D., Zou, Q. & Lin, S. Closeness impeded self-perspective inhibition whereas facilitated explicit perspective calculation. Acta Psychol. 220, 103387 (2021).

    Google Scholar 

  103. Savitsky, K., Keysar, B., Epley, N., Carter, T. & Swanson, A. The closeness-communication bias: increased egocentrism among friends versus strangers. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 47, 269–273 (2011).

    Google Scholar 

  104. Rabin, J. S. & Rosenbaum, R. S. Familiarity modulates the functional relationship between theory of mind and autobiographical memory. NeuroImage 62, 520–529 (2012).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  105. Krienen, F. M., Tu, P. C. & Buckner, R. L. Clan mentality: evidence that the medial prefrontal cortex responds to close others. J. Neurosci. 30, 13906–13915 (2010).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  106. Mullen, B., Dovidio, J. F., Johnson, C. & Copper, C. In-group-out-group differences in social projection. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 28, 422–440 (1992).

    Google Scholar 

  107. Robbins, J. M. & Krueger, J. I. Social projection to ingroups and outgroups: a review and meta-analysis. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 9, 32–47 (2005). This meta-analysis shows that self-projection occurs for both ingroups and outgroups but is stronger for ingroups.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  108. O’Brien, E. & Ellsworth, P. C. More than skin deep: visceral states are not projected onto dissimilar others. Psychol. Sci. 23, 391–396 (2012).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  109. Todd, A. R., Simpson, A. J. & Tamir, D. I. Active perspective taking induces flexible use of self-knowledge during social inference. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 145, 1583–1588 (2016).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  110. Woo, B. M. & Mitchell, J. P. Simulation: a strategy for mindreading similar but not dissimilar others? J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 90, 104000 (2020).

    Google Scholar 

  111. Simpson, A. J. & Todd, A. R. Intergroup visual perspective-taking: shared group membership impairs self-perspective inhibition but may facilitate perspective calculation. Cognition 166, 371–381 (2017).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  112. Woltin, K.-A., Corneille, O. & Yzerbyt, V. Y. Retrieving autobiographical memories influences judgments about others: the role of metacognitive experiences. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 40, 526–539 (2014).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  113. Todd, A. R., Hanko, K., Galinsky, A. D. & Mussweiler, T. When focusing on differences leads to similar perspectives. Psychol. Sci. 22, 134–141 (2011).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  114. Krueger, J. & Zeiger, J. S. Social categorization and the truly false consensus effect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 65, 670–680 (1993).

    Google Scholar 

  115. Cadinu, M. R. & Rothbart, M. Self-anchoring and differentiation processes in the minimal group setting. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 70, 661–677 (1996).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  116. Cho, J. C. & Knowles, E. D. I’m like you and you’re like me: social projection and self-stereotyping both help explain self–other correspondence. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 104, 444–456 (2013).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  117. Clement, R. W. & Krueger, J. Social categorization moderates social projection. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 38, 219–231 (2002).

    Google Scholar 

  118. Sudo, M. & Farrar, J. Theory of mind understanding, but whose mind? Affiliation with the target is related to children’s false belief performance. Cogn. Dev. 54, 100869 (2020).

    Google Scholar 

  119. Witt, S., Seehagen, S. & Zmyj, N. The influence of group membership on false-belief attribution in preschool children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 222, 105467 (2022).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  120. Vanbeneden, A., Woltin, K.-A. & Yzerbyt, V. Influence of membership in outgroups varying in competence and warmth on observers’ level‐2 visual perspective taking. Br. J. Psychol. 113, 938–959 (2022).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  121. Stathi, S. & Crisp, R. J. Imagining intergroup contact promotes projection to outgroups. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 44, 943–957 (2008).

    Google Scholar 

  122. Miller, N. & Marks, G. Assumed similarity between self and other: effect of expectation of future interaction with that other. Soc. Psychol. Q. 45, 100–105 (1982).

    Google Scholar 

  123. Ferguson, H. J., Brunsdon, V. E. & Bradford, E. E. Age of avatar modulates the altercentric bias in a visual perspective-taking task: ERP and behavioral evidence. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 18, 1298–1319 (2018).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  124. Surtees, A., Samson, D. & Apperly, I. Unintentional perspective-taking calculates whether something is seen, but not how it is seen. Cognition 148, 97–105 (2016).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  125. Todd, A. R., Cameron, C. D. & Simpson, A. J. The goal-dependence of level-1 and level-2 visual perspective calculation. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 47, 948–967 (2021).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  126. Van Boven, L., Judd, C. M. & Sherman, D. K. Political polarization projection: social projection of partisan attitude extremity and attitudinal processes. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 103, 84–100 (2012).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  127. Macrae, C. N. et al. Noticing future me: reducing egocentrism through mental imagery. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 42, 855–863 (2016).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  128. Macrae, C. N. et al. Turning I into me: imagining your future self. Conscious. Cogn. 37, 207–213 (2015).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  129. Golubickis, M., Tan, L. B., Falben, J. K. & Macrae, C. N. The observing self: diminishing egocentrism through brief mindfulness meditation. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 46, 521–527 (2016).

    Google Scholar 

  130. Ozdem-Mertens, C., Meshi, D. & Tamir, D. The reverse spotlight effect: failing to notice what others notice about us. Preprint at PsyArXiv https://osf.io/preprints/psyarxiv/hgaqm/ (2022).

  131. Hass, R. G. Perspective taking and self-awareness: drawing an E on your forehead. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 46, 788–798 (1984).

    Google Scholar 

  132. Mullen, B. et al. The false consensus effect: a meta-analysis of 115 hypothesis tests. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 21, 262–283 (1985).

    Google Scholar 

  133. Scaffidi Abbate, C., Boca, S. & Gendolla, G. H. Self-awareness, perspective-taking, and egocentrism. Self Identity 15, 371–380 (2016).

    Google Scholar 

  134. Spengler, S., Brass, M., Kühn, S. & Schütz-Bosbach, S. Minimizing motor mimicry by myself: self-focus enhances online action-control mechanisms during motor contagion. Conscious. Cogn. 19, 98–106 (2010).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  135. Stephenson, B. & Wicklund, R. A. Self-directed attention and taking the other’s perspective. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 19, 58–77 (1983).

    Google Scholar 

  136. Fenigstein, A. & Abrams, D. Self-attention and the egocentric assumption of shared perspectives. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 29, 287–303 (1993).

    Google Scholar 

  137. Birch, S. A., Brosseau-Liard, P. E., Haddock, T. & Ghrear, S. E. A ‘curse of knowledge’ in the absence of knowledge? People misattribute fluency when judging how common knowledge is among their peers. Cognition 166, 447–458 (2017).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  138. Ghrear, S., Fung, K., Haddock, T. & Birch, S. A. Only familiar information is a “curse”: children’s ability to predict what their peers know. Child Dev. 92, 54–75 (2021).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  139. Kelley, C. M. & Jacoby, L. L. Adult egocentrism: subjective experience versus analytic bases for judgment. J. Mem. Lang. 35, 157–175 (1996).

    Google Scholar 

  140. Zhao, Z., Sened, H. & Tamir, D. I. Egocentric projection is a rational strategy for accurate emotion prediction. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 109, 104521 (2023).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  141. Diamond, A. Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 135–168 (2013).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  142. Miyake, A. et al. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 49–100 (2000).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  143. Munakata, Y. & Michaelson, L. E. Executive functions in social context: implications for conceptualizing, measuring, and supporting developmental trajectories. Annu. Rev. Dev. Psychol. 3, 139–163 (2021).

    Google Scholar 

  144. Bailey, P. E. & Henry, J. D. Growing less empathic with age: disinhibition of the self-perspective. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 63, P219–P226 (2008).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  145. Brown-Schmidt, S. The role of executive function in perspective taking during online language comprehension. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 16, 893–900 (2009).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  146. Carlson, S. M. & Moses, L. J. Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Dev. 72, 1032–1053 (2001).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  147. Fizke, E., Barthel, D., Peters, T. & Rakoczy, H. Executive function plays a role in coordinating different perspectives, particularly when one’s own perspective is involved. Cognition 130, 315–334 (2014).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  148. German, T. P. & Hehman, J. A. Representational and executive selection resources in ‘theory of mind’: evidence from compromised belief-desire reasoning in old age. Cognition 101, 129–152 (2006).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  149. Lin, S., Keysar, B. & Epley, N. Reflexively mindblind: using theory of mind to interpret behavior requires effortful attention. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 46, 551–556 (2010).

    Google Scholar 

  150. Rakoczy, H. Executive function and the development of belief–desire psychology. Dev. Sci. 13, 648–661 (2010).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  151. Sabbagh, M. A., Moses, L. J. & Shiverick, S. Executive functioning and preschoolers’ understanding of false beliefs, false photographs, and false signs. Child Dev. 77, 1034–1049 (2006).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  152. Devine, R. T. & Hughes, C. Relations between false belief understanding and executive function in early childhood: a meta‐analysis. Child Dev. 85, 1777–1794 (2014).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  153. Samson, D., Apperly, I. A., Kathirgamanathan, U. & Humphreys, G. W. Seeing it my way: a case of a selective deficit in inhibiting self-perspective. Brain 128, 1102–1111 (2005).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  154. Cane, J. E., Ferguson, H. J. & Apperly, I. A. Using perspective to resolve reference: the impact of cognitive load and motivation. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 43, 591–610 (2017).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  155. Maehara, Y. & Saito, S. I see into your mind too well: working memory adjusts the probability judgment of others’ mental states. Acta Psychol. 138, 367–376 (2011).

    Google Scholar 

  156. Mills, K. L., Dumontheil, I., Speekenbrink, M. & Blakemore, S.-J. Multitasking during social interactions in adolescence and early adulthood. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2, 150117 (2015).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  ADS  Google Scholar 

  157. Navarro, E., Macnamara, B. N., Glucksberg, S. & Conway, A. R. What influences successful communication? An examination of cognitive load and individual differences. Discourse Process. 57, 880–899 (2020).

    Google Scholar 

  158. Newton, A. M. & de Villiers, J. G. Thinking while talking: adults fail nonverbal false-belief reasoning. Psychol. Sci. 18, 574–579 (2007).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  159. Horton, W. S. & Keysar, B. When do speakers take into account common ground? Cognition 59, 91–117 (1996).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  160. Steinbeis, N. & Singer, T. Projecting my envy onto you: neurocognitive mechanisms of an offline emotional egocentricity bias. NeuroImage 102, 370–380 (2014).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  161. Todd, A. R., Cameron, C. D. & Simpson, A. J. Dissociating processes underlying level-1 visual perspective taking in adults. Cognition 159, 97–101 (2017).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  162. Todd, A. R., Simpson, A. J. & Cameron, C. D. Time pressure disrupts level-2, but not level-1, visual perspective calculation: a process-dissociation analysis. Cognition 189, 41–54 (2019).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  163. Yeh, Y.-Y., Wang, C.-C., Cheng, S. & Chiu, C.-D. Dissociation of posture remapping and cognitive load in level-2 perspective-taking. Cognition 214, 104733 (2021).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  164. Deliens, G. et al. The impact of sleep deprivation on visual perspective taking. J. Sleep Res. 27, 175–183 (2018).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  165. Ferguson, H. J., Brunsdon, V. E. & Bradford, E. E. The developmental trajectories of executive function from adolescence to old age. Sci. Rep. 11, 1382 (2021).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  ADS  Google Scholar 

  166. Zelazo, P. D., Craik, F. I. & Booth, L. Executive function across the life span. Acta Psychol. 115, 167–183 (2004).

    Google Scholar 

  167. Birch, S. A. & Bloom, P. Children are cursed: an asymmetric bias in mental-state attribution. Psychol. Sci. 14, 283–286 (2003).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  168. Piaget, J. & Inhelder, B. The Child’s Conception of Space (Routledge & Kegan Paul, 1956).

  169. Wellman, H. M., Cross, D. & Watson, J. Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief. Child Dev. 72, 655–684 (2001).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  170. Wimmer, H. & Perner, J. Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition 13, 103–128 (1983).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  171. Apperly, I. A., Warren, F., Andrews, B. J., Grant, J. & Todd, S. Developmental continuity in theory of mind: speed and accuracy of belief–desire reasoning in children and adults. Child Dev. 82, 1691–1703 (2011).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  172. Surtees, A. D., Butterfill, S. A. & Apperly, I. A. Direct and indirect measures of level‐2 perspective‐taking in children and adults. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 30, 75–86 (2012).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  173. Surtees, A. D. & Apperly, I. A. Egocentrism and automatic perspective taking in children and adults. Child Dev. 83, 452–460 (2012).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  174. Aïte, A. et al. Taking a third-person perspective requires inhibitory control: evidence from a developmental negative priming study. Child Dev. 87, 1825–1840 (2016).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  175. Bernstein, D. M. Hindsight bias and false-belief reasoning from preschool to old age. Dev. Psychol. 57, 1387–1402 (2021).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  176. Dumontheil, I., Apperly, I. A. & Blakemore, S.-J. Online usage of theory of mind continues to develop in late adolescence. Dev. Sci. 13, 331–338 (2010).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  177. Epley, N., Morewedge, C. K. & Keysar, B. Perspective taking in children and adults: equivalent egocentrism but differential correction. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 40, 760–768 (2004).

    Google Scholar 

  178. Hayashi, H. & Nishikawa, M. Egocentric bias in emotional understanding of children and adults. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 185, 224–235 (2019).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  179. Hoffmann, F., Singer, T. & Steinbeis, N. Children’s increased emotional egocentricity compared to adults is mediated by age‐related differences in conflict processing. Child Dev. 86, 765–780 (2015).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  180. Sommerville, J. A., Bernstein, D. M. & Meltzoff, A. N. Measuring beliefs in centimeters: private knowledge biases preschoolers’ and adults’ representation of others’ beliefs. Child Dev. 84, 1846–1854 (2013).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  181. Steinbeis, N., Bernhardt, B. C. & Singer, T. Age-related differences in function and structure of rSMG and reduced functional connectivity with DLPFC explains heightened emotional egocentricity bias in childhood. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 10, 302–310 (2015).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  182. Perner, J. Understanding the Representational Mind (MIT Press, 1991).

  183. Wellman, H. M. Making Minds: How Theory of Mind Develops (Oxford Univ. Press, 2014).

  184. Lagattuta, K. H., Sayfan, L. & Harvey, C. Beliefs about thought probability: evidence for persistent errors in mindreading and links to executive control. Child Dev. 85, 659–674 (2014).

    Google Scholar 

  185. Bernstein, D. M., Thornton, W. L. & Sommerville, J. A. Theory of mind through the ages: older and middle-aged adults exhibit more errors than do younger adults on a continuous false belief task. Exp. Aging Res. 37, 481–502 (2011).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  186. Bradford, E. E., Brunsdon, V. E. & Ferguson, H. J. Cognitive mechanisms of perspective-taking across adulthood: an eye-tracking study using the director task. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 49, 959–973 (2023).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  187. Phillips, L. H. et al. Lifespan aging and belief reasoning: influences of executive function and social cue decoding. Cognition 120, 236–247 (2011).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  188. Henry, J. D., Phillips, L. H., Ruffman, T. & Bailey, P. E. A meta-analytic review of age differences in theory of mind. Psychol. Aging 28, 826–839 (2013). This meta-analysis shows that older adults display more egocentric mentalizing than younger adults.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  189. Riva, F., Triscoli, C., Lamm, C., Carnaghi, A. & Silani, G. Emotional egocentricity bias across the life-span. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8, 74 (2016).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  190. Cohen, D. & Gunz, A. As seen by the other…: perspectives on the self in the memories and emotional perceptions of Easterners and Westerners. Psychol. Sci. 13, 55–59 (2002).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  191. Kessler, K., Cao, L., O’Shea, K. J. & Wang, H. A cross-culture, cross-gender comparison of perspective taking mechanisms. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 281, 20140388 (2014).

    Google Scholar 

  192. Wu, S. & Keysar, B. The effect of culture on perspective taking. Psychol. Sci. 18, 600–606 (2007). This study shows that adults from a collectivist and interdependent culture (such as that of China) display less egocentric mentalizing than adults from an individualist and independent culture (such as that of the USA).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  193. Wu, S., Barr, D. J., Gann, T. M. & Keysar, B. How culture influences perspective taking: differences in correction, not integration. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7, 822 (2013).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  194. Luk, K. K., Xiao, W. S. & Cheung, H. Cultural effect on perspective taking in Chinese–English bilinguals. Cognition 124, 350–355 (2012).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  195. Markus, H. R. & Kitayama, S. Culture and the self: implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychol. Rev. 98, 224–253 (1991).

    Google Scholar 

  196. Goetz, P. J. The effects of bilingualism on theory of mind development. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 6, 1–15 (2003).

    Google Scholar 

  197. Greenberg, A., Bellana, B. & Bialystok, E. Perspective-taking ability in bilingual children: extending advantages in executive control to spatial reasoning. Cogn. Dev. 28, 41–50 (2013).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  198. Kovács, Á. M. Early bilingualism enhances mechanisms of false-belief reasoning. Dev. Sci. 12, 48–54 (2009).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  199. Navarro, E. & Conway, A. R. Adult bilinguals outperform monolinguals in theory of mind. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 74, 1841–1851 (2021).

    Google Scholar 

  200. Rubio-Fernández, P. & Glucksberg, S. Reasoning about other people’s beliefs: bilinguals have an advantage. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 38, 211–217 (2012).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  201. Schroeder, S. R. Do bilinguals have an advantage in theory of mind? A meta-analysis. Front. Commun. 3, 36 (2018).

    Google Scholar 

  202. Fan, S. P., Liberman, Z., Keysar, B. & Kinzler, K. D. The exposure advantage: early exposure to a multilingual environment promotes effective communication. Psychol. Sci. 26, 1090–1097 (2015).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  203. Yu, C. L., Kovelman, I. & Wellman, H. M. How bilingualism informs theory of mind development. Child Dev. Perspect. 15, 154–159 (2021).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  204. Bialystok, E. Bilingualism and the development of executive function: the role of attention. Child Dev. Perspect. 9, 117–121 (2015).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  205. Carlson, S. M. & Meltzoff, A. N. Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Dev. Sci. 11, 282–298 (2008).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  206. Lehtonen, M., Fyndanis, V. & Jylkkä, J. The relationship between bilingual language use and executive functions. Nat. Rev. Psychol. 2, 360–373 (2023).

    Google Scholar 

  207. Bialystok, E. & Senman, L. Executive processes in appearance–reality tasks: the role of inhibition of attention and symbolic representation. Child Dev. 75, 562–579 (2004).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  208. Diaz, V. & Farrar, M. J. The missing explanation of the false‐belief advantage in bilingual children: a longitudinal study. Dev. Sci. 21, e12594 (2018).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  209. Dick, A. S. et al. No evidence for a bilingual executive function advantage in the ABCD study. Nat. Hum. Behav. 3, 692–701 (2019).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  210. Donnelly, S., Brooks, P. J. & Homer, B. D. Is there a bilingual advantage on interference-control tasks? A multiverse meta-analysis of global reaction time and interference cost. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 26, 1122–1147 (2019).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  211. Nguyen, T.-K. & Astington, J. W. Reassessing the bilingual advantage in theory of mind and its cognitive underpinnings. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 17, 396–409 (2014).

    Google Scholar 

  212. Nichols, E. S., Wild, C. J., Stojanoski, B., Battista, M. E. & Owen, A. M. Bilingualism affords no general cognitive advantages: a population study of executive function in 11,000 people. Psychol. Sci. 31, 548–567 (2020).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  213. Paap, K. R. & Greenberg, Z. I. There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cogn. Psychol. 66, 232–258 (2013).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  214. Magee, J. C. & Galinsky, A. D. Social hierarchy: the self‐reinforcing nature of power and status. Acad. Manag. Ann. 2, 351–398 (2008).

    Google Scholar 

  215. Overbeck, J. R. & Droutman, V. One for all: social power increases self-anchoring of traits, attitudes, and emotions. Psychol. Sci. 24, 1466–1476 (2013).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  216. Toma, C., Yzerbyt, V., Corneille, O. & Demoulin, S. The power of projection for powerless and powerful people: effect of power on social projection is moderated by dimension of judgment. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 8, 888–896 (2017).

    Google Scholar 

  217. Kraus, M. W., Piff, P. K., Mendoza-Denton, R., Rheinschmidt, M. L. & Keltner, D. Social class, solipsism, and contextualism: how the rich are different from the poor. Psychol. Rev. 119, 546–572 (2012).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  218. Dietze, P. & Knowles, E. D. Social class predicts emotion perception and perspective-taking performance in adults. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 47, 42–56 (2021).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  219. Dietze, P. & Knowles, E. D. Social class and the motivational relevance of other human beings: evidence from visual attention. Psychol. Sci. 27, 1517–1527 (2016).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  220. Blader, S. L., Shirako, A. & Chen, Y.-R. Looking out from the top: differential effects of status and power on perspective taking. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 42, 723–737 (2016). This study shows that adults display more egocentric mentalizing when experiencing high social power (versus control) but less egocentric mentalizing when experiencing high social status (versus control).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  221. Galinsky, A. D., Magee, J. C., Inesi, M. E. & Gruenfeld, D. H. Power and perspectives not taken. Psychol. Sci. 17, 1068–1074 (2006).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  222. Ebersole, C. R. et al. Many Labs 3: evaluating participant pool quality across the academic semester via replication. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 67, 68–82 (2016).

    Google Scholar 

  223. Stern, C., West, T. V., Jost, J. T. & Rule, N. O. “Ditto heads”: do conservatives perceive greater consensus within their ranks than liberals? Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 40, 1162–1177 (2014).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  224. Stern, C., West, T. V. & Schmitt, P. G. The liberal illusion of uniqueness. Psychol. Sci. 25, 137–144 (2014).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  225. Stern, C. & West, T. V. Ideological differences in anchoring and adjustment during social inferences. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 42, 1466–1479 (2016). This study shows that adults with a right-wing political orientation display more egocentric mentalizing than adults with a left-wing political orientation, with motives for affiliating with like-minded others as a key mediator.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  226. Jost, J. T., Ledgerwood, A. & Hardin, C. D. Shared reality, system justification, and the relational basis of ideological beliefs. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 2, 171–186 (2008).

    Google Scholar 

  227. Maner, J. K., DeWall, C. N., Baumeister, R. F. & Schaller, M. Does social exclusion motivate interpersonal reconnection? Resolving the ‘porcupine problem.’ J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 92, 42–55 (2007).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  228. Hess, Y. D. & Pickett, C. L. Social rejection and self-versus other-awareness. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 46, 453–456 (2010).

    Google Scholar 

  229. DeWall, C. N., Maner, J. K. & Rouby, D. A. Social exclusion and early-stage interpersonal perception: selective attention to signs of acceptance. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 96, 729–741 (2009).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  230. Knowles, M. L. Social rejection increases perspective taking. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 55, 126–132 (2014).

    Google Scholar 

  231. Arndt, J., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T. & Schimel, J. Creativity and terror management: evidence that creative activity increases guilt and social projection following mortality salience. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 77, 19–32 (1999).

    Google Scholar 

  232. Pyszczynski, T. et al. Whistling in the dark: exaggerated consensus estimates in response to incidental reminders of mortality. Psychol. Sci. 7, 332–336 (1996).

    Google Scholar 

  233. Simon, L. et al. Perceived consensus, uniqueness, and terror management: compensatory responses to threats to inclusion and distinctiveness following mortality salience. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 23, 1055–1065 (1997).

    Google Scholar 

  234. Greenberg, J., Solomon, S. & Pyszczynski, T. in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology Vol. 29, 61–139 (Elsevier, 1997).

  235. Niedenthal, P. M., Halberstadt, J. B., Margolin, J. & Innes‐Ker, Å. H. Emotional state and the detection of change in facial expression of emotion. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 30, 211–222 (2000).

    Google Scholar 

  236. Silani, G., Lamm, C., Ruff, C. C. & Singer, T. Right supramarginal gyrus is crucial to overcome emotional egocentricity bias in social judgments. J. Neurosci. 33, 15466–15476 (2013).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  237. Von Mohr, M., Finotti, G., Ambroziak, K. B. & Tsakiris, M. Do you hear what I see? An audio-visual paradigm to assess emotional egocentricity bias. Cognit. Emot. 34, 756–770 (2020).

    Google Scholar 

  238. Maner, J. K. et al. Functional projection: how fundamental social motives can bias interpersonal perception. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 88, 63–78 (2005).

    PubMed  ADS  Google Scholar 

  239. Converse, B. A., Lin, S., Keysar, B. & Epley, N. In the mood to get over yourself: mood affects theory-of-mind use. Emotion 8, 725–730 (2008).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  240. Bodenhausen, G. V., Kramer, G. P. & Süsser, K. Happiness and stereotypic thinking in social judgment. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 66, 621–632 (1994).

    Google Scholar 

  241. Phillips, L. H., Bull, R., Adams, E. & Fraser, L. Positive mood and executive function: evidence from Stroop and fluency tasks. Emotion 2, 12–22 (2002).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  242. Yip, J. A. & Schweitzer, M. E. Losing your temper and your perspective: anger reduces perspective-taking. Organ. Behav. Hum. Dec. Process. 150, 28–45 (2019).

    Google Scholar 

  243. Todd, A. R., Forstmann, M., Burgmer, P., Brooks, A. W. & Galinsky, A. D. Anxious and egocentric: how specific emotions influence perspective taking. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 144, 374–391 (2015). This study shows that adults display more egocentric mentalizing when feeling anxious than when feeling other high-arousal negative emotions, with motives for uncertainty reduction as a key mediator.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  244. Todd, A. R. & Simpson, A. J. Anxiety impairs spontaneous perspective calculation: evidence from a level-1 visual perspective-taking task. Cognition 156, 88–94 (2016).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  245. Lerner, J. S. & Keltner, D. Fear, anger, and risk. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 81, 146–159 (2001).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  246. Raghunathan, R. & Pham, M. T. All negative moods are not equal: motivational influences of anxiety and sadness on decision making. Organ. Behav. Hum. Dec. Process. 79, 56–77 (1999).

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  247. Miceli, M. & Castelfranchi, C. Anxiety as an “epistemic” emotion: an uncertainty theory of anxiety. Anxiety Stress Coping 18, 291–319 (2005).

    Google Scholar 

  248. FeldmanHall, O. & Shenhav, A. Resolving uncertainty in a social world. Nat. Hum. Behav. 3, 426–435 (2019).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  249. Smith, C. A. & Ellsworth, P. C. Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 48, 813–838 (1985).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  250. Bodenhausen, G. V. in Affect, Cognition, and Stereotyping: Interactive Processes in Group Perception (eds Mackie, D. M. & Hamilton, D. L.) 13–37 (Elsevier, 1993).

  251. Sassenrath, C., Sassenberg, K. & Scholl, A. From a distance…: the impact of approach and avoidance motivational orientation on perspective taking. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 5, 18–26 (2014).

    Google Scholar 

  252. Mussweiler, T. ‘Seek and ye shall find’: antecedents of assimilation and contrast in social comparison. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 31, 499–509 (2001).

    Google Scholar 

  253. Ames, D. R., Mor, S. & Toma, C. The double-edge of similarity and difference mindsets: what comparison mindsets do depends on whether self or group representations are focal. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 49, 583–587 (2013).

    Google Scholar 

  254. Chartrand, T. L. & Lakin, J. L. The antecedents and consequences of human behavioral mimicry. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 285–308 (2013).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  255. Brass, M., Ruby, P. & Spengler, S. Inhibition of imitative behaviour and social cognition. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. 364, 2359–2367 (2009).

    Google Scholar 

  256. Santiesteban, I. et al. Training social cognition: from imitation to theory of mind. Cognition 122, 228–235 (2012). This study shows that adults display less egocentric mentalizing after undergoing a training procedure that entailed repeatedly inhibiting tendencies to imitate another person.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  257. Bukowski, H., Todorova, B., Boch, M., Silani, G. & Lamm, C. Socio-cognitive training impacts emotional and perceptual self-salience but not self-other distinction. Acta Psychol. 216, 103297 (2021).

    Google Scholar 

  258. Kampis, D., Duplessy, H. L., Askitis, D. & Southgate, V. Training self-other distinction facilitates perspective taking in young children. Child Dev. 94, 956–969 (2023).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  259. Wan, Y., Wei, Y., Xu, B., Zhu, L. & Tanenhaus, M. K. Musical coordination affects children’s perspective‐taking, but musical synchrony does not. Dev. Sci. 26, e13367 (2023).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  260. Vorauer, J. D., Gagnon, A. & Sasaki, S. J. Salient intergroup ideology and intergroup interaction. Psychol. Sci. 20, 838–845 (2009).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  261. Todd, A. R. & Galinsky, A. D. The reciprocal link between multiculturalism and perspective-taking: how ideological and self-regulatory approaches to managing diversity reinforce each other. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 48, 1394–1398 (2012).

    Google Scholar 

  262. Caruso, E. M., Epley, N. & Bazerman, M. H. The costs and benefits of undoing egocentric responsibility assessments in groups. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 91, 857–871 (2006).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  263. Eyal, T., Steffel, M. & Epley, N. Perspective mistaking: accurately understanding the mind of another requires getting perspective, not taking perspective. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 114, 547–571 (2018). This study shows that deliberate perspective-taking efforts reduce egocentric mentalizing but do not increase mentalizing accuracy.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  264. Savitsky, K., Van Boven, L., Epley, N. & Wight, W. M. The unpacking effect in allocations of responsibility for group tasks. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 41, 447–457 (2005).

    Google Scholar 

  265. Damen, D., van der Wijst, P., van Amelsvoort, M. & Krahmer, E. Can the curse of knowing be lifted? The influence of explicit perspective-focus instructions on readers’ perspective-taking. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 46, 1407–1423 (2020).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  266. Ames, D. L., Jenkins, A. C., Banaji, M. R. & Mitchell, J. P. Taking another person’s perspective increases self-referential neural processing. Psychol. Sci. 19, 642–644 (2008).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  267. Davis, M. H., Conklin, L., Smith, A. & Luce, C. Effect of perspective taking on the cognitive representation of persons: a merging of self and other. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 70, 713–726 (1996).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  268. Davis, M. H. et al. Cognitions associated with attempts to empathize: how do we imagine the perspective of another? Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 30, 1625–1635 (2004).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  269. Galinsky, A. D. & Moskowitz, G. B. Perspective-taking: decreasing stereotype expression, stereotype accessibility, and in-group favoritism. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 78, 708–724 (2000).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  270. Todd, A. R. & Burgmer, P. Perspective taking and automatic intergroup evaluation change: testing an associative self-anchoring account. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 104, 786–802 (2013).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  271. Vorauer, J. D. & Sucharyna, T. A. Potential negative effects of perspective-taking efforts in the context of close relationships: increased bias and reduced satisfaction. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 104, 70–86 (2013).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  272. Hutchings, R. J., Simpson, A. J., Sherman, J. W. & Todd, A. R. Perspective taking reduces intergroup bias in visual representations of faces. Cognition 214, 104808 (2021).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  273. Batchelder, W. H. & Riefer, D. M. Theoretical and empirical review of multinomial process tree modeling. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 6, 57–86 (1999).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  274. Sherman, J. W. et al. The self-regulation of automatic associations and behavioral impulses. Psychol. Rev. 115, 314–335 (2008).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  275. Ratcliff, R., Smith, P. L., Brown, S. D. & McKoon, G. Diffusion decision model: current issues and history. Trends Cogn. Sci. 20, 260–281 (2016).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  276. Hedge, C., Powell, G. & Sumner, P. The reliability paradox: why robust cognitive tasks do not produce reliable individual differences. Behav. Res. Methods 50, 1166–1186 (2018).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  277. Bar-Anan, Y., Liberman, N., Trope, Y. & Algom, D. Automatic processing of psychological distance: evidence from a Stroop task. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 136, 610–622 (2007).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  278. Tamir, D. I. & Mitchell, J. P. The default network distinguishes construals of proximal versus distal events. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 2945–2955 (2011).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  279. Meyer, M. L., Zhao, Z. & Tamir, D. I. Simulating other people changes the self. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 148, 1898–1913 (2019).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  280. Galinsky, A. D., Wang, C. S. & Ku, G. Perspective-takers behave more stereotypically. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 95, 404–419 (2008).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  281. Goldstein, N. J. & Cialdini, R. B. The spyglass self: a model of vicarious self-perception. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 92, 402–417 (2007).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  282. Lemay, E. P. Jr, Clark, M. S. & Feeney, B. C. Projection of responsiveness to needs and the construction of satisfying communal relationships. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 92, 834–853 (2007).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  283. Murray, S. L., Holmes, J. G. & Griffin, D. W. The benefits of positive illusions: idealization and the construction of satisfaction in close relationships. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 70, 79–98 (1996).

    Google Scholar 

  284. Phills, C. E., Kawakami, K., Tabi, E., Nadolny, D. & Inzlicht, M. Mind the gap: increasing associations between the self and Blacks with approach behaviors. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 100, 197–210 (2011).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  285. Tversky, B. & Hard, B. M. Embodied and disembodied cognition: spatial perspective-taking. Cognition 110, 124–129 (2009).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  286. Samson, D., Apperly, I. A., Braithwaite, J. J., Andrews, B. J. & Bodley Scott, S. E. Seeing it their way: evidence for rapid and involuntary computation of what other people see. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 36, 1255–1266 (2010).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  287. Qureshi, A. W. & Monk, R. L. Executive function underlies both perspective selection and calculation in level-1 visual perspective taking. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 25, 1526–1534 (2018).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  288. Baker, L. J., Levin, D. T. & Saylor, M. M. The extent of default visual perspective taking in complex layouts. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 42, 508–516 (2016).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  289. Furlanetto, T., Becchio, C., Samson, D. & Apperly, I. Altercentric interference in level 1 visual perspective taking reflects the ascription of mental states, not submentalizing. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 42, 158–163 (2016).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  290. Michael, J. et al. Seeing it both ways: using a double-cuing task to investigate the role of spatial cuing in level-1 visual perspective-taking. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 44, 693–702 (2018).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  291. Nielsen, M. K., Slade, L., Levy, J. P. & Holmes, A. Inclined to see it your way: do altercentric intrusion effects in visual perspective taking reflect an intrinsically social process? Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 68, 1931–1951 (2015).

    Google Scholar 

  292. Conway, J. R., Lee, D., Ojaghi, M., Catmur, C. & Bird, G. Submentalizing or mentalizing in a level 1 perspective-taking task: a cloak and goggles test. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 43, 454–465 (2017).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  293. Santiesteban, I., Catmur, C., Hopkins, S. C., Bird, G. & Heyes, C. Avatars and arrows: implicit mentalizing or domain-general processing? J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 40, 929–937 (2014).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  294. Vestner, T., Balsys, E., Over, H. & Cook, R. The self-consistency effect seen on the dot perspective task is a product of domain-general attention cueing, not automatic perspective taking. Cognition 224, 105056 (2022).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  295. Keysar, B. The illusory transparency of intention: linguistic perspective taking in text. Cogn. Psychol. 26, 165–208 (1994).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  296. Ross, L. & Ward, A. in Values and Knowledge (eds Turiel, E. & Brown, T.) 103–135 (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., 1996).

  297. Dawes, R. M. Statistical criteria for establishing a truly false consensus effect. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 25, 1–17 (1989).

    Google Scholar 

  298. Hoch, S. J. Perceived consensus and predictive accuracy: the pros and cons of projection. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 53, 221–234 (1987).

    Google Scholar 

  299. Krueger, J. I. & Chen, L. J. The first cut is the deepest: effects of social projection and dialectical bootstrapping on judgmental accuracy. Soc. Cogn. 32, 315–336 (2014).

    Google Scholar 

  300. Fiske, S. T. in The Handbook of Social Psychology (eds Gilbert, D. T. et al.) 357–411 (1998).

  301. Fagerlin, A., Ditto, P. H., Danks, J. H. & Houts, R. M. Projection in surrogate decisions about life-sustaining medical treatments. Health Psychol. 20, 166–175 (2001).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  302. Eyal, T. & Epley, N. How to seem telepathic: enabling mind reading by matching construal. Psychol. Sci. 21, 700–705 (2010).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  303. Lewis, K. L., Hodges, S. D., Laurent, S. M., Srivastava, S. & Biancarosa, G. Reading between the minds: the use of stereotypes in empathic accuracy. Psychol. Sci. 23, 1040–1046 (2012).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  304. Gopnik, A. & Wellman, H. M. Why the child’s theory of mind really is a theory. Mind Lang. 6, 145–171 (1992).

    Google Scholar 

  305. Karniol, R. Egocentrism versus protocentrism: the status of self in social prediction. Psychol. Rev. 110, 564–580 (2003).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  306. Thomas, R. C. & Jacoby, L. L. Diminishing adult egocentrism when estimating what others know. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 39, 473–486 (2013).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  307. Kunda, Z. & Spencer, S. J. When do stereotypes come to mind and when do they color judgment? A goal-based theoretical framework for stereotype activation and application. Psychol. Bull. 129, 522–544 (2003).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  308. Stinson, L. & Ickes, W. Empathic accuracy in the interactions of male friends versus male strangers. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 62, 787–797 (1992).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  309. Kampis, D. & Southgate, V. Altercentric cognition: how others influence our cognitive processing. Trends Cogn. Sci. 24, 945–959 (2020).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  310. Southgate, V. Are infants altercentric? The other and the self in early social cognition. Psychol. Rev. 127, 505–523 (2020).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  311. Back, E. & Apperly, I. A. Two sources of evidence on the non-automaticity of true and false belief ascription. Cognition 115, 54–70 (2010).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  312. Kovács, Á. M., Téglás, E. & Endress, A. D. The social sense: susceptibility to others’ beliefs in human infants and adults. Science 330, 1830–1834 (2010).

    PubMed  ADS  Google Scholar 

  313. Van der Wel, R. P., Sebanz, N. & Knoblich, G. Do people automatically track others’ beliefs? Evidence from a continuous measure. Cognition 130, 128–133 (2014).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  314. Edwards, K. & Low, J. Level 2 perspective-taking distinguishes automatic and non-automatic belief-tracking. Cognition 193, 104017 (2019).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  315. Ward, E., Ganis, G. & Bach, P. Spontaneous vicarious perception of the content of another’s visual perspective. Curr. Biol. 29, 874–880 (2019).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  316. Heyes, C. Submentalizing: I am not really reading your mind. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 9, 131–143 (2014).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors thank K. Lagattuta and the members of the Research in Social Cognition Lab at the University of California, Davis, for commenting on a draft of this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

The authors contributed equally to all aspects of the article.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Andrew R. Todd or Diana I. Tamir.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Peer review

Peer review information

Nature Reviews Psychology thanks Ian Apperly, Henryk Bukowski and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work.

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Todd, A.R., Tamir, D.I. Factors that amplify and attenuate egocentric mentalizing. Nat Rev Psychol 3, 164–180 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-024-00277-1

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-024-00277-1

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing