Introduction

Among the multiple stresses affecting honey bee health in contemporary apiculture, pesticide residues within hives and in agricultural landscapes where bees forage have been identified as significant contributors to bee mortality and sublethal impacts1,2,3. Formulated pesticides are often more toxic to insects, including non-target pollinators, than are the active ingredients of pesticides4,5,6,7, suggesting that chemicals other than active ingredients in formulated pesticides and tank-mixes may play a role in the mortality of pollinators and other non-target insects in agroecosystems.

Adjuvants, which are "inert components" in pesticide formulations or tank mixes of agrochemicals, are used to enhance the performance of the active ingredients of pesticides by increasing solubility, dispersion, attachment, residual activity, persistence, and stability8. Because they are considered biologically inert, most adjuvants are not subject to the same level of rigorous safety testing as are the insecticidal ingredients. As a result, the toxicity of adjuvants, alone or in combination with pesticides, is not well understood. Adjuvant residues have been detected at high levels (up to 40.5 mg/kg) in North American beehives9, as well as on almond flowers10 and beebread11. Previous studies have shown that adjuvants can inadvertently increase off-target effects of pesticide active-ingredients on animals12,13, including honey bees and other pollinators5,8,14,15,16,17. Despite the toxicity risks, almond pesticide application data from the California Pesticide Information Portal (CalPIP; https://calpip.cdpr.ca.gov/) show that the use of adjuvants, with other pesticides, is common in almond orchards before and during bloom, when two-thirds of U.S. bee colonies are present in almond orchards to provide pollination service. The combined application of pesticides and adjuvants during bloom aims to control pests such as the peach twig borer, as well as fungal diseases such as brown rot, anthracnose, leaf blight, and scab. Commercial spray adjuvants, especially silicone surfactants, can have lethal and sub-lethal effects on honey bees5,15,17. For example, organosilicone surfactant adjuvants can impair olfactory learning18 and cause acute toxicity to honey bee adults19. Additionally, adjuvants can synergize the toxicity of some pesticides4,6,20,21 as well as the virulence of black queen cell virus22. Given this previous work, there is a distinct possibility that adjuvant applications in pollinator-dependent crops may harm honey bees and potentially other pollinators.

Honey bees have a sophisticated olfactory system that allows them to perceive and respond to social cues in their hive environment, including those involved in interactions with nestmates23,24,25. Insecticide interference with olfactory-mediated responses has been documented in a wide range of species26,27,28, raising the expectation that exposure to pesticides could also hinder the functionality of the olfactory system in honey bees. Even sublethal doses of pesticides can cause deformation of the antennal sensilla of honey bees29 and impair learning and memory30, resulting in reduced foraging efficiency and colony productivity 31,32.

Colony losses characterized by larval and pupal mortality have frequently been reported after almond pollination, and use of tank-mixed insecticide, fungicide, and adjuvants has been implicated in these losses33. Dyne-Amic, the fungicide propiconazole, and the insecticide chlorantraniliprole have been used as tank-mixed pesticides21. The adjuvant Dyne-Amic, a blend of methylated vegetable oils combined with organosilicone-based non-ionic surfactants, serves as a surfactant and penetrant in agriculture. It enhances pesticide dispersion on plant surfaces and aids in penetrating the leaf cuticle, leading to improved uptake and distribution. Based on the CALPIP database, Dyne-Amic was the most commonly used adjuvant in almond orchards, reaching almost 244,000 cumulative acres in 2016–2018 during the pollination window (February 15–March 15), approximately 50% greater acreage than the second most commonly used adjuvant (reaching approximately 163,000 cumulative acres). Furthermore, synergistic interactions between the fungicide propiconazole and the insecticide chlorantraniliprole can increase toxicity to honey bee larvae34 and adults35. The combination of the adjuvant Dyne-Amic with this fungicide-insecticide mix can reduce the LC50 ratios of these pesticides on adult honey bees21 and lower queen emergence rate36.

The mortality experienced by larval and pupal bees, as well as the reduced survival of queens, could conceivably result from the sublethal effects of a tank-mixed combination of adjuvant-fungicide-insecticide to which adult honey bees are exposed. Considering the superorganism structure of honey bees societies, adverse impacts of neurotoxic pesticides on worker bees responsible for nurturing developing brood could include reduced olfaction-mediated responses to nestmates, including larvals signals (reviewed in Table 1), with colony-level consequences. The impacts are particularly significant in specific individuals within the colony, particularly queen-destined larvae21,36. These larvae experience exposure to remarkably low pesticide levels36 or might not even directly interact with the pesticide stressor37. Accordingly, we investigated the impact of a sublethal dose of a tank-mix combination on the olfactory responses of bees. To ensure comparability, we used the same batch of treated pollen as was used in the queen rearing experiments conducted by Ricke et al.36, who reported lower survival of next-generation queens that had been raised by nurse bees exposed to the combination of Dyne-Amic, propiconazole, the active ingredient in Tilt, and chlorantraniliprole, the active ingredient of Altacor.

Table 1 Olfaction-mediated sublethal adverse effects of pesticides on eusocial pollinators.

We conducted electroantennogram (EAG) assays to evaluate the tank-mix combination of fungicide, insecticide, and adjuvant in terms of changes induced in olfactory responses to social signals. These signals included the semi-volatile brood ester pheromone, simulated by a mix of 10 fatty acids38, and the larval volatile pheromone β-ocimene, including both (E)- and (Z)-β-ocimene. Previous research has demonstrated that bee larvae produce the brood ester pheromone39,40 and both forms of β-ocimene41, which trigger responses from nurse bees. Our aim was to investigate whether these changes in olfactory responses could serve as a potential mechanism underlying alterations in nursing behavior and other social behaviors within honey bee colonies24,25. Additionally, we measured changes in olfactory responses to the alarm pheromone 2-heptanone, which has the potential to function as a repellent pheromone, as an explanatory factor underlying changes in defense behaviors42. The behaviors encompass actions such as guard bees utilizing alarm pheromones to tag intruders following an attack or other worker bees using the same alarm pheromone to display heightened vigilance and increased agitation42,43. The main purpose of this study thus was to uncover potential mechanisms that contribute to changes in social behaviors in response to pesticides and to provide insights that might be linked to the reported decrease in the survival of next-generation brood and queens of honey bees, as observed in our previous study36.

Results

In general, the olfactory electroantennogram (EAG) responses of honey bees to the 10-in-1 brood-emitted ester pheromone (BEP) showed a dose-dependent pattern (Fig. 1A). However, adjuvant-pesticide-mediated changes in olfactory responses were observed primarily at higher concentrations of BEP. No significant differences were found between any of the treatments and the control at the lowest concentration of BEP stimulus. In addition, in terms of the effects of pesticides on olfactory perception of BEP, exposure to the combination of the fungicide Tilt and the insecticide Altacor enhanced the amplitude of olfactory responses of bees to BEP only at a concentration of 40% (Fig. 1A).

Figure 1
figure 1

Electroantennogram (EAG) responses of adult honey bee workers to social signals after exposure to the organosilicone adjuvant Dyne-Amic, the fungicide Tilt and the insecticide Altacor, alone or in combination: (A) brood-emitted ester pheromone (BEP), (B) β-ocimene (larval volatile pheromone), and (C) 2-heptanone (alarm pheromone). The EAG responses of bees that consumed pollen with water, representing the control group, are shown in blue. The other three groups, including the Dyne-Amic adjuvant treatment (shown in yellow), the Altacor + Tilt pesticide treatment (shown in red), and the Altacor + Tilt + Dyne-Amic adjuvant-pesticide mixture treatment (shown in orange), are referred to as treatment groups. The asterisk symbol (*) indicates a statistically significant difference in the EAG responses of each treatment group (Dyne-Amic, Altacor + Tilt, or Altacor + Tilt + Dyne-Amic) compared to the EAG responses of bees in the control group that consumed pollen with water, using the same concentration of tested stimuli. The estimated marginal mean of EAG response ± SE is listed. (NS: p > 0.05, *: p < 0.05, **: p < 0.01, ***: P < 0.01, N = 106, GEE test).

No changes occurred in olfactory responses to BEP when the organosilicone adjuvant Dyne-Amic was administered alone. However, a increased effect was observed in the olfactory responses of bees to 20%, 40% and 100% BEP when the Dyne-Amic was used in combination with Tilt and Altacor. In contrast, Dyne-Amic alone did alter the olfactory response of bees to the larval volatile pheromone β-ocimene (Fig. 1B) and the alarm pheromone 2-heptanone (Fig. 1C) but did not have any significant effects on the responses to the BEP. Exposure of honey bees to Altacor and Tilt resulted in a significant increase in olfactory responses to β-ocimene (Fig. 1B) and 2-heptanone (Fig. 1C), but no synergistic effect on olfactory responses to β-ocimene was observed (Fig. 1B) when the adjuvant Dyne-Amic was used in combination with Tilt and Altacor. Notwithstanding, exposure to Dyne-Amic + Altacor + Tilt still led to heightened sensitivity to 2-heptanone compared to the control group (Fig. 1C). Thus, the effect of the adjuvant-pesticide mixture on β-ocimene indicated a compensatory effect, where the combined influence offset the individual effects of the pesticide or adjuvant alone, resulting in responses similar to the control. On the other hand, for the alarm pheromone, the effect of the adjuvant-pesticide mixture reduced the responses compared to their individual effects, and they were still different from the control, indicating a dampening effect.

Discussion

The eusocial lifestyle of A. mellifera involves a complex series of behavioral and physiological events that affect hive function and reproductive success. Sublethal doses of pesticides can disrupt olfaction-mediated behaviors and olfactory neurotransmission (Table 1), which can affect the coordination of events and result in a reduction in colony survival. In this study, we found that the fungicide Tilt and the insecticide Altacor, with the active ingredients propiconazole and chlorantraniliprole, respectively, together and in combination with the organosilicone surfactant adjuvant Dyne-Amic altered olfactory responses of worker bees to the brood ester pheromone and the alarm pheromone 2-heptanone. This finding suggests that these commonly used pesticides, when applied in a tank mix, may interfere with honey bee communication and reproductive systems that rely on olfactory cues23,24,25.

In addition, we observed complex interacting impacts on honey bee olfactory responses when honey bees were exposed to a combination of pesticides and adjuvants. The effects are not additive (i.e., the sum of the individual effects of pesticides and adjuvants), indicating a more complex relationship among them. The adjuvant Dyne-Amic in combination with a DMI (demethylation inhibitor) fungicide and a diamide insecticide synergistically affected the olfactory responses to the brood ester pheromone, while the combination triggers other modulatory effects on responses, including dampening effects on the alarm pheromone response and compensatory responses to the larval signal β-ocimene. Our results with the BEP are consistent with findings reported in several previous studies. Combinations of pesticides at sublethal levels can lead to additive or synergistic effects on olfactory responses and other aspects65,66. Additionally, the organosilicone surfactant adjuvants, such as those present in Dyne-Amic, interfere with the olfactory learning ability of adult honey bees18 and can also increase the toxicity of some pesticides4,6,20,21. The impaired olfactory responses may be attributed to toxic effects on the peripheral olfactory system of honey bees, central nervous system, or both. Sublethal doses of pesticides can induce neurotoxic effects in honey bees, including altered synaptic transmission, oxidative stress, and apoptosis31,32,60,67,68,69. Furthermore, Asian honey bees (Apis cerana) from areas with high pesticide use exhibit impaired olfaction, characterized by decreased proboscis extension response during olfactory learning, alterations in the expression of Calpain 1 (an important calcium-binding protein), and reduced calcium levels in their brains29. Therefore, our study suggests that the combination of pesticides and adjuvants can alter honey bee neurons and thereby interfere with olfactory-mediated behaviors.

One of the unexpected findings in our study was the observation that pesticide and adjuvant exposure led to an increase in the amplitude of EAG responses to brood and alarm pheromones. The amplitude of EAG responses is influenced by factors such as the number of receptors, the receptor response of individual olfactory receptor neurons, the density of responsive neurons, and the firing rate of neurons involved in olfaction70. The larger EAG responses we observed suggest an enhanced sensitivity or altered selectivity of olfactory receptors in the olfactory system to the tested stimuli. Because adult bees are less likely to exhibit an increase in receptor number, this enhanced sensitivity may be attributed to several mechanisms, e.g., alterations in receptor sensitivity, changes in the expression of receptors, chemosensory or odorant-binding proteins, or modifications in signaling pathways71 due to exposure to pesticides or adjuvants. Odorant-binding proteins and chemosensory proteins play a crucial role in modulating the sensitivity of the olfactory system72. In Drosophila, the absence of odorant-binding proteins in certain sensilla might lead to increased electrophysiological responses73,74. Mayack75 identified 22 chemicals, including agricultural chemicals and alpha-terpineol, an adjuvant and fungicide, as high-affinity binders of OBP14, an odorant-binding protein of honey bees. These chemicals have all been found in beehives, suggesting that they may affect olfactory perception. Additionally, exposure to neonicotinoid pesticides can alter the expression of odorant-binding proteins and chemosensory proteins in bees76,77,78, potentially changing the sensitivity and perception of odors, including brood pheromone, queen mandibular pheromone, and trail pheromone78.

Another possible mechanism is that pesticides and adjuvants may disrupt the structure of antennal sensilla, where olfactory receptor neurons are housed. This disruption can increase sensilla permeability to odorants, potentially enhancing receptor sensitivity to BEP. Evidence from A. cerana from pesticide-intensive sites has revealed distortions in sensilla, including crack-like marks on the sensory placode and campaniform structures across the antennal segments29. Similarly, in the aphid Aphis craccivora and the tick Haemaphysalis longicornis, exposure to certain essential oils deformed antennal segments and sensilla79,80, indicating impacts of such substances, oil-based adjuvants and pesticides, on olfactory structures. In addition, permeability is also a function of adjuvants, particularly oil-based or surfactant-based ones, as they significantly enhance the absorption of pesticides on plant surfaces81 and on insect cuticle82. Thus, adjuvants may also facilitate increased penetration of odors and pheromones through the sensilla cuticle, ultimately leading to altered olfactory receptor sensitivity and synergistic effects.

Our results suggest that, at the level of antennal (peripheral) olfactory receptor neurons, the first step in olfactory perception, adjuvants and insecticides altered neural responses. However, the correlated behavioral output of bees in response to these changes is not yet clear. It is important to acknowledge that the response of insects to volatile chemicals or pheromones is not determined by the peripheral olfactory receptor neurons alone. The peripheral information undergoes complex integration and reshaping83, involving the combinations and interaction of various stimuli in the antennal lobe and higher brain centers84,85,86,87. In the moth Agrotis ipsilon, Rabhi et al.88,89 found that the neonicotinoid clothianidin could either increase or decrease behavioral sex pheromone responses depending on pesticide concentration, but the responses of olfactory receptor neurons remained unchanged, while the sensitivity of intracellularly recorded antennal lobe output neurons was correspondingly altered. These findings suggest the involvement of the antennal lobe in the mediation of clothianidin-induced behavioral changes89. Lucas and Renou90 also showed that deltamethrin, bioresmethrin, and DDT did not alter the amplitude of the EAG in response to pheromone stimulation, but action potential initiation was sensitive to these insecticides.

As a rule, olfaction involves the integration of signals within the central nervous system, including external cues and internal physiological states of bees91,92. While an increase in EAG amplitude was observed, it does not necessarily indicate a corresponding change in behavioral responses, as sensory processing in the brain also plays a role in shaping behavior70,93. However, in the parallel study that used the same batch of treated pollen as this study, Ricke et al.36 found that queens raised by workers fed with pollen treated with Tilt, Altacor, and the Dyne-Amic exhibited reduced overall survival rates compared to the control group36. Our findings suggest that the alterations in EAG responses to brood pheromones in workers may be linked to subsequent nursing quality and reproductive success of brood and the next generation of queen-rearing36.

In addition, our findings suggest that pesticide exposure can have varying effects on different sensory stimuli. Even when exposed to the same combination of pesticides and/or adjuvants, the responses of olfactory neurons can be selectively affected, resulting in inconsistent levels of interfering pheromone perception94,95. This may be potentially due to factors like receptor saturation or feedback loops, leading to altered behaviors25,96. Receptor specificity for different odors may also contribute to the observed diverse responses25. This phenomenon of inconsistent interference levels is not limited to honey bees, as Lalouette et al.97 observed selective and differential effects of the pyrethroid insecticide deltamethrin on different sensory stimuli in the cotton leafworm Spodoptera littoralis. They found that deltamethrin had an impact on male responses to sex pheromones, while their response to host plant odorants remained unaffected. These findings indicate that the effects of pesticides on sensory perception can vary depending on the specific stimuli involved.

Moreover, bees interact and manage colony organization using chemical cues, and almost every aspect of honey bee colony life involves pheromones. In this study, we examined only the alarm and brood pheromones, which are used to recruit bees for colony defense and for larval care, respectively. Other pheromone responses may also be affected by the combination of adjuvant and pesticides. Overall, our data, combined with other studies showing olfaction-mediated communication deficits in bees exposed to adjuvants and insecticides, suggest that commonly used adjuvants, insecticides, and fungicides could be partially responsible for the observed decline in eusocial pollinator populations. Additionally, concurrent exposure to multiple pesticides and adjuvants may increase risks to the health of these indispensable pollinators in agroecosystems.

Materials and methods

Experimental bees and their diet treatments

The experiment was conducted with the western honey bees, Apis mellifera, kept in an apiary of the University of Illinois Bee Research Facility, Urbana, IL (40.072773, -88.21944). Frames of capped brood were collected from two naturally mated queen hives and incubated in a dark room (34 ± 1 °C, 50 ± 10% RH) to obtain newly emerged workers.

Adult honey bee workers were collected within 24 h after emergence and were randomly introduced into Petri dish cages98, 10 bees per cage, each with 1 g of commercially purchased pollen (Betterbee, Greenwich, NY) infused with pesticide treatments described below. Pollen was kept in the cages to provide continuous pollen intake for the bees throughout the pre-EAG testing period. Bees within the natural nurse bee window, seven to nine days old, were randomly selected for EAG testing. Thus, pollen feeding (or pesticide exposure) spanned at least seven days and up to nine days. Each cage also contained two tube feeders, one with 2 mL of sucrose water (50% w/v) and another with 2 mL of deionized water. To ensure comparability with the results of the queen-rearing assay conducted by Ricke et al.36, we used the same batch of treated pollen from the study. This approach ensured consistency in pollen composition and minimized potential variations in nutrition, associated phytochemicals, and technical factors during preparation and enabled a valid cross-comparison of the results of the two studies. Four pollen treatments were prepared by mixing homogenized commercial bee-collected pollen (BetterBee, Greenwich, NY) with formulated adjuvant, insecticide, and fungicide products at the subsequent concentrations: (1) 0.8% (w/w) Dyne-Amic, (2) Altacor (targeting 40 ppm of the active ingredient chlorantraniliprole) plus Tilt (targeting 90 ppm of active ingredient propiconazole), (3) 0.8% Dyne-Amic mixed Altacor and Tilt at the same targeted active ingredient concentrations, and (4) distilled water as the negative control. Concentrations were chosen based on the maximum field application rates for each product in almonds (see Supplementary Table 1 in Ricke et al.36) to simulate field exposure following a single pesticide application event. Each cage was randomly assigned one type of treated pollen. Based on the daily pollen consumption of 3.4 to 4.3 mg per bee reported by Crailsheim et al.99, we estimated that each bee ingested approximately 0.31–0.39 μg, 0.14–0.17 μg, and 27.20–34.40 μg per day of propiconazole, chlorantraniliprole, and Dyne-Amic, respectively. In the study by Ricke et al.36, nurse bees had average residues of 0.08 μg propiconazole and 0.01 μg chlorantraniliprole per bee per day, using the same treated pollen for bee feeding as in our study. This pollen sharing provides a reliable reference basis for hypothesized residue levels in our study bees36. Surprisingly, the reported pesticide residues in bees are lower than the estimates. This difference may be due to differences in pollen consumption between studies. In addition, field conditions may contribute to this difference, as bees in the study Ricke et al.36 had the opportunity to defecate, which likely reduced toxicant accumulation, whereas our estimate takes into account all possible toxicants ingested. Tubes of sucrose-water and water were replaced every two days. Bees were kept in a dark incubator (34 ± 1 °C, 50 ± 10% RH) before the EAG assay.

Antenna preparation for electroantennogram (EAG) recording

Due to time, equipment, and environmental constraints, our capacity was limited to recording only 6 to 12 antennae per day. Therefore, to ensure a consistent supply of bees within the age range for sampling and conducting the EAG assay, day-old bees were arranged in cages with treated pollen, with one replicate performed per day. Each replicate covered a two- to three-day sampling period. This setup was repeated every two or three days over the course of six weeks. Bees within the nurse bee time window from a natural hive100, i.e., seven-, eight-, or nine-day old adults, were randomly sampled in cages. The sampled bee group alternated between the water control group and the other chemical groups.

After anesthetizing a bee on ice, one antenna was randomly selected and carefully removed by cutting at the base of the antennal scape using a micro-scissor. The proximal end of the antenna was inserted into a glass capillary filled with bee saline (130 mM NaCl, 6 mM KCl, 4 mM MgCl2, 5 mM CaCl2, 160 mM sucrose, 25 mM glucose, 10 mM HEPES, pH 6.7, 500 mOsmol)101. A droplet of biomedical-grade adhesive (KWIK-SIL, World Precision Instruments) was immediately applied to seal the opening between the capillary wall and the antenna to prevent saline evaporation and minimize electrical potential drift during the EAG recording. The glass capillary, containing the mounted antenna was fixed onto a micromanipulator (MM-3, Narishige International USA, INC.). The tip of the flagellum was also cut off to create a wound opening, which was then immersed and sealed in a separate glass capillary filled with the bee saline. This capillary was also mounted on another micromanipulator. The positioning of the antenna allowed for exposure to an odorant-carrying air stream by the antenna, except for the 10th segment of flagellum, distal part of 9th segment of flagellum, and proximal part of scapus, which were blocked from stimulus by saline and/or adhesive. Thus, our study may miss the contribution of these antenna segments to odor perception because their receptors may not receive the odorant stimulus tested. Our goal was to include as many segments as possible, and the sacrifice of some segments for the connection of electrical probes for recording was a necessary compromise that may, however, result in underestimating their role in odor perception. Mounting and sealing procedures were performed under a surgical microscope at 32 to 45 × magnification to ensure accurate and consistent preparation of the antennae for EAG recordings.

Two silver chloride-coated silver electrodes were immersed in the saline within the glass capillaries to pick up the electrophysiological signals from the antenna. The received electrical signal was then measured by a differential amplifier equipped with a headstage (Model 3000, A-M Systems). Subsequently, the signal underwent filtering using a notch filter and a 100 Hz low-pass filter, and was amplified by a factor of 1,000. A 16-bit multifunction data acquisition (DAQ) device (PCI-6036E, National Instrument) was utilized to digitalize the amplified signal at a sampling rate of 1000 samples per second. All data acquisition and processing were controlled by a custom LabVIEW program (ver. 2018, National Instrument) developed specifically for this purpose.

Odor stimulus preparation

To elicit the olfactory response of the bee antenna, three types of stimuli were used:

  1. 1.

    A 10-fatty acid ester-blend was prepared to mimic a semi-volatile brood-emitted ester pheromone (BEP)39,40 based on the study of Pankiw et al.38. The composition of the blend included methyl oleate (24.95%; 311111, Sigma-Aldrich), methyl linolenate (21%; 62200, Supelco), methyl stearate (17%; S0080, TCI America), ethyl linolenate (13%; 10008199, Cayman Chemical), ethyl oleate (8%; 268011, Sigma-Aldrich), ethyl stearate (7%; S0079, TCI America), methyl palmitate (3%; P0750, Sigma), ethyl palmitate (3%; P9009, Sigma-Aldrich), methyl linoleate (2%; 62280, Supelco), ethyl linoleate (1%; L0055, TCI America), and the preservative, tertiary-butylhydroquinone (0.05%; 112941, Sigma-Aldrich). Solutions of this synthetic BEP in mineral oil (M8410, Sigma-Aldrich) were prepared in concentrations at intervals on a logarithm scale (0%, 5%, 10%, 20%, 40%, 100%) to obtain dose-dependent responses. These solutions were stored at -20 °C until used in the assays.

  2. 2.

    The terpene β-ocimene (CAS No. 13877–91-3; W353901, Sigma-Aldrich), comprising both (E)-β-ocimene and (Z)-β-ocimene, was employed to mimic another highly volatile larval signal. (E)-β-ocimene is known to act as a brood pheromone from larvae that has both releaser and primer effects41. Additionally, Noël et al.41 suggested that nurse bees may be capable of detecting and utilizing both β-ocimene enantiomers, because (Z)-β-ocimene is also abundantly released by older larvae and pupae within the brood.

  3. 3.

    The ketone 2-heptanone (2185, Eastman Kodak Company) was used to imitate a volatile worker-emitted alarm pheromone for defense42.

To prepare the odor stimuli, 500 µL of the solutions of BEP, β-ocimene, and 2-heptanone were pipetted into uncapped 4-mL glass vials, which were then placed into 20-mL headspace gas sampling vials. The chemicals were allowed to vaporize and emit odors into the headspace of the gas sampling vials during the assay (Fig. 2A). The 4-mL glass vials containing the chemicals were changed daily to ensure consistent odor presentation.

Figure 2
figure 2

Experimental setup and features of electroantennogram responses. (A) Diagram illustrating the overall setup of the electroantennogram (EAG) recording and stimulus delivery apparatus. (This diagram is not drawn to scale. DAQ: data acquisition, Amp: amplifier) (B) Features of EAG responses monitored. Duration of odor stimulation is 200 ms. Lag time or response delay is the measured time difference (~ 160 ms) from stimulus introduction to start of voltage drop.

Delivery of odor stimuli

The odor stimuli were delivered to the tested antenna using an olfactometer utilizing a round glass tube with a 1-cm opening diameter, which was positioned 1 cm away from the antenna. A stimulus controller (CS-55, Syntech), incorporating a charcoal filter, two air pumps, and a solenoid valve, supplied clean air as the airstream source and controlled the rate and ratio of the diverted flows.

The delivered airstream, flowing at a constant rate of approximately 15 cm/s, was used to allow the bristle sensilla to adapt. Six-sevenths of the airstream’s volume flow consisted of a continuous stream of air moisturized through a heated humidifier, while the remaining one-seventh alternated between a stimulus air pulse and a complementary air pulse. During odor stimulation, the stimulus air pulse was generated and directed through a set of solenoid valves (three-way, normally closed, VT317V, SMC Corporation). To ensure consistent volume flows for the stimulus and complementary air pulses across all paths, we regulated diverted flows with the stimulus air controller and used airflow control valves at the outlets of solenoid valves connecting to sampling vials. The stimulus pulse was passed through one of the headspace gas sampling vials containing the stimulus odor, blowing it out into the airstream.

To overcome the limited volatilization of the BEP stimuli at room temperature, we used a heating technique that has previously been shown to be effective in capturing responses to low-volatility odorants102. Six headspace gas sampling vials containing the synthetic BEP and mineral oil were heated in an oven at 55 °C to facilitate their volatilization102. The headspace gas sampling vials containing volatilized β-ocimene and 2-heptanone were kept at room temperature to accommodate their high volatility. Heating β-ocimene and 2-heptanone might lead to excessive concentrations of volatiles, potentially saturating the antenna receptors.

The overall electroantennogram recording and stimulation apparatus set-up is shown in Fig. 2A. The tubes carrying the humidified continuous air, the stimulating air pulse, and the complementary air pulse were combined in a glass mixing chamber within the oven. Airflows were mixed in the chamber before delivery to the tested antenna. The temperature of the final delivered air stream was maintained at 30 ± 2 °C, monitored by a type K bead wire thermocouple with a thermometer (800077 & 800004, Sper Scientific).

Each tested antenna underwent a minimum of six series of eight odor stimuli, with a 17-s interval between each stimulus. The duration of each air pulse was 200 ms (Fig. 2B). The overall response delay, including the mechanical delay of the odor delivery system and the delay of the EAG response, was approximately 160 ms.

EAG recording

EAG measures and records the potential drop across the antenna resulting from the activation of odorant receptors and the subsequent opening of ion channels. The response amplitude was calculated by determining the peak potential, which is the most negative potential recorded within 240 ms after the onset of the EAG response (Fig. 2B). The baseline potential was obtained by averaging the potentials recorded during the 100 ms period before the onset of the electrophysiological response. The response amplitude was then calculated by subtracting the baseline potential from the peak potential. To reduce the impact of background noise, two replicated stimulative air pulses of the same odor were administered with a 17-s interval, and the responses to these replicated stimuli were averaged.

The recording of responses from six series of stimuli took approximately 30 min. Only the data from antennae that continued to respond to 2-heptanone until the sixth round of stimuli were included in the final analysis. The final analysis encompassed the responses recorded from the antennae of 106 worker bees.

Data analysis

A total of 5,088 responses were recorded from 106 bees in two hives, with 23–30 bees in each treatment group (Table S1). Each bee antenna underwent exposure to eight odor stimuli in six replicates, resulting in 48 responses per antenna. The first two series of stimuli were used for flushing and priming the odor delivery system and were not included in the final analysis ((6 - 2 replicate series) × 8 odor stimuli × 106 bees = 3392 responses). Two accidental airflow interference events resulted in the exclusion of two sets of stimulation repetitions (3392 - 8 × 2 = 3376 responses). Additionally, seven negative amplitude responses, which occasionally occurred in response to mineral oil odors and were likely due to noise, were excluded from the final analysis as they appeared unrealistic (3376 - 7 = 3369 responses). The final analysis included 3,369 EAG responses (Table S2).

The Generalized Estimating Equations (GEE) method, as described by Hardin and Hilbe103, was employed for analyzing the EAG data. Individual bees were treated as subjects, and the recorded response to each chemical at each time point served as the within-subject variable for the analysis of repeated measures. An unstructured correlation matrix was used under the model-based estimator. The dependent variable was the amplitude of the EAG response, while the fungicide treatments were considered fixed factors, with the water control group designated as the reference category. The hives and the series stimuli order were adjusted as covariates in all analyses. The models were fitted using IBM SPSS Statistics v. 24 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA).