Introduction

Pig production contributes to environmental pollution: applying pig manure to fields releases nitrogen compounds, leading to eutrophication of soils and water bodies1,2. Especially in areas with high livestock density and low availability of arable land, this problem has become particularly severe. Furthermore, the low self-sufficiency of protein sources for pig feed of the EU results in the need to import soybean meal for animal feed, mainly from South America, USA and China. This exposes the European pig industry to an increased risk of sustainability problems due to differing production standards, long-distance transport and land-use changes in regions where soy is grown on deforested land3, as well as shortages and price volatility of soybeans on the world market3,4. Increased efforts are being made across Europe to limit pigs’ protein intake to mitigate the environmental damage from pig production. Pomar and Remus5 showed that each percentage reduction in dietary nitrogen leads to a 1.5% decrease in nitrogen excretion. However, van der Meer et al.6 reported an increase in damaging behaviors, including tail and ear biting, during a 20% reduction in pigs’ dietary protein, especially when combined with poor sanitary conditions. In a trial where a 20% reduction in protein was combined with restricted feeding and an unbalanced supply of essential amino acids (EAA), especially methionine, an outbreak of tail biting occurred after two months, which subsided when access to feed was increased7. Conceivably, deficiencies in certain essential AAs, such as methionine, threonine, and tryptophan, result in neurotransmitter system dysfunction, as specific AAs serve as neurotransmitters or are required for neurotransmitter synthesis6. Moreover, foraging behavior could increase due to the relative scarcity of AAs, potentially leading to obsessive manipulation and biting behaviors, especially when straw is unavailable as enrichment8. Further, AA deficiency is thought to make blood taste more attractive9. The trade-off between ensuring animal welfare and rendering pig production more environmentally friendly is apparent: while restricting the protein in feed can potentially reduce nitrogen emissions, it is important to examine whether this measure will affect pigs’ wellbeing.

Damaging behaviors, including tail biting, are common in pig production, resulting in compromised victim welfare and economic losses. Tail biting and excessive manipulation also indicate that a pig’s behavioral needs are unmet, suggesting poor wellbeing of the biters8. Outbreaks of tail biting are often difficult to identify, have a multifactorial origin and occur when stressors accumulate, including a lack of suitable occupational materials, poor climate conditions in the barn, inadequate cleanliness, unbalanced diet and poor health. Behavioral changes at the pen level, such as in feeding patterns, can be detected up to one month before a tail biting outbreak occurs10. At an individual level, tail posture has also been shown to be an early pen-level indicator of an impending outbreak of tail biting11 and behavioral problems caused by various stressors begin before escalating into harmful behavior leading to serious injury. For instance, a pig could proceed from gently manipulating a conspecific’s tail or ear, with no noticeable reaction from the target, to biting and inducing a bloody wound6,12,13. Ursinus et al.14 found a positive correlation between the number of pen-mate manipulations, a generally natural and harmless behavior, and a high rate of tail biting. There is a delicate boundary between natural pig behaviors, including exploring and manipulating conspecifics, and damaging behaviors, such as biting. For instance, engaging in positive social behaviors has a positive impact on growth, presumably by reducing stress through oxytocin release13. However, redirected “abnormal” behaviors, such as excessive belly nosing, ear biting, and “tail-in-mouth”15, can severely disturb the receiver16, cause permanent stress and even impact growth13.

Individual differences in protein efficiency (PE), i.e., the ability to utilize dietary proteins17, are heritable18,19. Therefore, it is to be expected that individual pigs will differ in how their behavior is affected by a protein reduction. Harnessing these heritable individual differences in breeding would therefore effectively reduce the long-term nitrogen pollution from pig production. The molecular basis of PE is not yet well established, and there are legitimate concerns that breeding for increased PE could induce behavioral problems and reduced welfare. For instance, Breuer et al.20 reported a weak but significant genetic correlation between the lean tissue growth rate and tail biting behavior in Landrace pigs, but not in Large White pigs, the breed used in the present study. Whether PE or related traits have any connection with the likelihood of becoming a victim of damaging or problematic behaviors is not yet known. Therefore, there seems to be a certain risk of inadvertently co-selecting pigs with an increased predisposition to behavioral problems when breeding for a higher protein accretion rate. However, the aim of sustainable pig production should not be to increase the rate of protein accretion, but rather to increase PE, as this takes into account not only the output in terms of muscle mass, but also the input in terms of the amount of protein consumed. PE is a trait arising from a combination of several processes, including gastrointestinal tract absorption and protein turnover, which occur in different tissues and organs. Genetic selection for higher PE could improve these processes or could alter the allocation of proteins toward lean tissue growth and away from other processes, including immune or endocrine system functioning, potentially compromising homeostasis and thus health and reproduction. The genetic architecture of PE or nitrogen excretion is likely complex, as multiple regions on different chromosomes were found associated with nitrogen excretion traits21. Some of these quantitative trait loci (QTLs) overlap with various production traits, while others are unique to nitrogen excretion21, though the functions of genes within these QTLs are still unclear.

In this exploratory study, we aimed to investigate whether PE is associated with indicators of impaired welfare on a phenotypic level in pigs subjected to dietary protein restriction. Particularly, whether the level of PE is associated with performing or receiving tail biting and other damaging behaviors was examined, as were the performed and received manipulation behaviors of pen mates. Furthermore, we explored the relationship between PE and straw rooting, which is considered an indicator for positive welfare, allowing animals to explore the environment, thereby reducing boredom and manipulation of pen mates16. Here, we also investigated the outcome of social encounters and confrontations related to PE as an indicator of whether aggressiveness or dominance is associated with efficiency. Finally, we explored the relationship of the frequency of lesions, tail position, cleanliness, and activity with PE.

Material and methods

Animals and diets

The pigs were part of a larger experiment comprising 681 pigs in 14 farrowing series (batches) of the Swiss Large White dam line herd at the experimental farm Agroscope Posieux, with the goal of estimating genetic parameters of PE19. The pigs were not selected for increased PE. In this study, 95 non-tail-docked pigs (53 females and 42 castrated males) born in two farrowing series were observed between August and December 2020. One male pig died before slaughter, so PE could not be assessed. About seven days after birth, male piglets were surgically castrated. The piglets were removed from the box and an anti-inflammatory drug (ketoprofen) was injected. After 15 min, the piglets were anaesthetized with isoflurane inhalation for 90 s and the testes were removed. The piglets were placed in a warm cradle until they woke up (3–4 min after the end of the isoflurane inhalation) and, when fully awake, were returned to their box with the sow and littermates. The pigs were raised in 36.78 m2 pens with 23 or 24 pigs in each, i.e., two pens per series. The floor space was above legal requirements, with only partial slatted flooring (as required by law in Switzerland). Pens were cleaned daily, and the pigs had ad libitum access to drinking water and a low-crude-protein diet, which was distributed by single-spaced automatic feeding stations (Schauer Maschinenfabrik GmbH & Co. KG, Prambachkirchen, Austria). The feeders were accessible to the pigs via individual identification by an RFID chip in the ear from 07:40 to 23:30, allowing individual feed intake monitoring. Pigs were fed a standard starter feed after weaning until reaching an average body weight of 22.4 (± 1.6) kg. They were then mixed in groups of 24 pigs, in which they stayed until slaughter, and received a grower diet that was 20% protein-reduced compared to the recommended diet in Switzerland22. From an average body weight of 63.5 (± 2.4) kg, the pigs were fed a 20% protein reduced finisher diet. All EAA were reduced to the same level to avoid deficiencies and thus a nutritional imbalance. The chemical composition of the grower and finisher diet calculated from tabular values of raw materials is shown in Table 1. Each pen was covered with a thin layer of straw on the concrete floor, contained two mobile and two fixed straw baskets, and held four suspended metal chains as occupational materials. Following the Swiss Animal Welfare Act, it was ensured that occupation material, in our case unchopped straw, was always available. The two groups had auditory and olfactory contact. Pigs had no outdoor access. Natural light was provided through windows in the barn. The health status of the pigs on each observation day was considered, and each injured animal was treated on the same day.

Table 1 Dietary ingredients and gross chemical composition calculated (%, g or MJ/kg as-fed) of the grower and finisher dietsa.

Behavioral observations

Between days 98 and 115 after birth, each pig’s behavior was recorded following an ethogram (Table 2), which had been created based on literature research and two days of observations, and refined during a preliminary study prior to this work. The first farrowing series was observed in August 2020 and the second in December 2020. Direct behavioral observations were conducted in daylight between 12:45 and 16:00. Prunier et al.23 suggested that pig activity peaks in the early morning and afternoon. As the pens were cleaned in the morning, the observations were made in the afternoon, as this was the time of least external disturbance. However, the pigs might have rested more during hot periods in the first farrowing series. The observer (LR) stood outside the pen to limit any influence on the animals. Observations began after a 15-min habituation period. All pigs in one pen were individually marked with colored spray and directly observed for 5 min using focal sampling24 in a random sequence on one afternoon25. Each pen was observed over four different days; thus, each pig was observed for 20 min total, resulting in approximately 65 h of observation, including scanning for lesions, tail position, and cleanliness, as described in Table 3. During observation, the observer was blind to the PE of the individual pigs, as this was only recorded several weeks later, at the time of slaughter. The total number of separate instances of nasal and oral manipulation of objects (metal chains, pen barriers, and drinkers), as well as of pen mates during the 5 min of individual observation, was recorded. These behaviors comprised biting, seizing (with the mouth, but not the teeth), and manipulation (with a closed mouth or the snout). Behaviors directed toward the head, body, ears, tail, and vulva or perineal area of a conspecific were recorded separately, and we recorded whether the focal animal performed (active) or received the behavior (passive). Due to the scarcity of manipulation behaviors directed at pen mates, we later combined the behaviors: all bites directed (received) toward the ears, tails, and vulva or perineal (ETV) region were summed into the variable “damaging behaviors”. All other behaviors, including biting, seizing, and manipulation with the snout, directed (received) at the ETV region, were aggregated into one variable, termed “problematic behaviors,” and biting, seizing, and manipulation with the snout directed toward the rest of the body (excluding the ETV region) into another variable (“potentially problematic behaviors”) by totaling the instances of each behavior. The same was done for behaviors received.

Table 2 Ethogram of observed pig behaviours (modified from Roch et al.25).
Table 3 Observations of lesions, tail posture and cleanliness of the pigs (from Roch et al.25).

The start and the outcomes of confrontations, i.e., agonistic situations arising when two pigs meet and both want an object or place simultaneously, were recorded, and the following were possible: win (domination), lose (submission), or a tie, when the outcome was unclear. The number of instances of straw rooting was determined as the sum of the number of times the focal pig was engaging with the baskets and whether it performed straw rooting behaviour on the floor (never in 5 min = 0, ≥ 1 s in 5 min = 1). In addition, all wounds and scratches (classified as mild scratches or severe wounds), the tail posture (raised curled, hanging straight, or tucked between legs), and the cleanliness of each pig were recorded after each observation, using the protocol in Table 3.

Protein efficiency and performance traits

The PE of the 94 pigs surviving until slaughter was calculated as the amount of protein in the carcass after slaughter divided by the amount of protein ingested. In this study, we worked with the naturally occurring variation in PE in pigs not selected for this trait. Pigs were slaughtered at an average live body weight of 105.2 kg (± 6.5) at the Agroscope experimental slaughterhouse in Posieux. After 16 h of feed deprivation, the pigs were individually transported in a trolley to the research slaughterhouse (located 100 m from the barn). They were stunned with a CO2 stunner (87% CO2; Samson C1 L 803; MPS Group, Holbaek, Denmark) for 180 s and immediately exsanguinated. The intestines and viscera, as well as the hair, hooves and blood, were removed, and the carcass was cut in half. Half-carcasses were scanned on a dual energy X-ray absorptiometry (DXA) device (GE Lunar i-DXA, GE Medical Systems, Glattbrugg, Switzerland) to determine the lean meat content. The following regression Eq. (1), which was developed in a previous study30, was used to estimate the protein content from the lean meat content, determined by DXA:

$$protein~ content ~carcass (g)= -482.745+0.23 (\text{g lean tissue }DXA \times P)$$
(1)

P is the proportion of the weight of the left cold carcass-half weight (including the whole head and the tail) to the total cold carcass weight. The amount of protein ingested from the start of the switch to the protein-reduced grower feed was recorded by the automated feeders. The estimated protein content of the carcass at the time of the feed change was subtracted from the total weight of protein in the carcass at slaughter (for more details see18). The lean and fat mass in the carcass were obtained from DXA. The information needed to calculate average daily feed intake (ADFI), average daily gain (ADG) (2) and feed conversion ratio (FCR) (3) were automatically recorded by the feeder stations and during the weekly individual weighing of the pigs.

$$ADG=\frac{live~ BW~ \left(kg\right) slaughter-live ~BW ~\left(kg\right) ~start}{age~ \left(days\right) ~slaughter-age~ \left(days\right) ~start}$$
(2)
$$FCR= \frac{ADFI}{ADG}$$
(3)

\(Live~ BW ~\left(kg\right)~ slaughter\) And \(age~ \left(days\right)~ slaughter\) are the live body weight in kg shortly before slaughter and the age in days at slaughter, respectively, and \(live~ BW~ \left(kg\right)~ start\) and \(age ~\left(days\right) ~start\) are the exact body weight in kg and the age in days at the start of the grower phase, respectively. Note that in this study, we report protein efficiency in the carcass, excluding organs and blood, which is therefore necessarily lower than the PE of the empty body. A previous study reported a mean carcass PE of 0.38 ± 0.03 for pigs of around 100 kg live weight, while the mean empty body PE was 0.47 ± 0.0418.

Statistical analysis

We grouped the single behaviors directed toward conspecifics into the following categories with decreasing intensity and damaging capacity by calculating the total number of occurrences: (i) “damaging behaviors”—biting directed toward the ETV regions of conspecifics, which were too rare for statistical analysis; (ii) “problematic behaviors”—seizing and manipulation of conspecifics’ ETV regions; and (iii) “potentially problematic behaviors”—biting, seizing, and manipulation of all other body (non-ETV) regions of conspecifics, including the head. All different behaviors directed at objects, as well as rooting in the floor straw and in the basket, were combined into a single variable (termed “object manipulation” and “straw rooting”, respectively), and all analyses were conducted with the R software version 4.2.231. We identified PE outliers using the Grubbs test in the outliers package version 0.1532. This resulted in the removal of two individuals, with a PE of 0.574 and 0.469, from further statistical analysis, leaving a data set of 92 pigs. In consideration of these data, which were count variables with likely more zeros than expected due to an important proportion of pigs not performing the behavior in question during the observation window (i.e., zero-inflated) or data that might not satisfy the strict mean–variance relationship of a Poisson distribution (i.e., over-dispersion), we used the R package glmmTMB version 1.1.433. The effect of PE on problematic behaviors, confrontation outcomes, and straw rooting in terms of behavior counts was analyzed with generalized mixed-effects models with a Poisson, quasi-Poisson, and negative binomial family, and the latter two were chosen to account for different mean–variance relationships, i.e., over-dispersion. We included sex as a covariate in all models, and body weight at the time of observation in the models on confrontations.

Since the error distribution that would best fit each behavioral count variable was unknown at the start of the analyses, we ran several models for all variables of interest to determine the best fitting model in a model selection procedure: First, a full model with PE and sex as fixed effects was performed for each family (Poisson, quasi-Poisson and negative binomial), and the intercept was modelled as zero-inflated with a logit link. If the zero-inflation term was not significant, we reran the model without zero-inflation. If it was significant, zero-inflation was maintained for the respective error distribution models. The two nested models included one with PE only, and one with only the intercept for each model family. Thus, in total, 9 models were run. For initiating a confrontation and its outcomes, we also included body weight at the time of the observation, because it might influence the ability to perform in confrontations. Thus, we ran 12 models for confrontations initiated, won and lost: the full model including PE, sex and weight, a model with PE only, one with body weight only, and one with only the intercept. The individual ID and the pen ID nested in the farrowing group were added as random effects to correct for multiple observations and the effects of the social group on behavior, and to account for the fact that observations were carried out for two farrowing series where the temperature was rather different. Using the AICctab command from the bbmle package version 1.0.2534, the best model was chosen based on AICc (modified AIC for small sample sizes). Following Burnham and Anderson35, we considered models with a difference in AICc (ΔAICc) of less than two to have equal evidence and presented them as confidence sets. A note of caution: with this approach, a potential relationship between PE and the behaviors studied cannot be ruled out on the basis of non-significant p-values alone, i.e., p > 0.05, as it is always the case with providing conclusive evidence for the absence of an effect in the framework of null-hypothesis testing. We therefore also interpret the inclusion or absence of PE (or any other variable) in the models within the confidence sets as additional evidence. For the models in the confidence set, we used the R package DHARMa36 to diagnose any violations of distribution assumptions and model misspecifications. Plots were created using the ggplot2 package version 3.3.637 with the ggstatsplot extension38 to compute group and sex differences.

Ethical approval

The experimental procedure was approved by the Office for Food Safety and Veterinary Affairs of the Canton of Fribourg (animal experimentation license 2018_30_FR), and all procedures were conducted in accordance with the Swiss Ordinance on Animal Protection and the Ordinance on Animal Experimentation and the ARRIVE guidelines.

Results

Animal performance

Some performance traits differed across farrowing series and sexes. In general, we found significant sex differences for ADFI, ADG, lean mass, crude protein (CP) content and fat mass. Farrowing series differed significantly in lean mass, CP and fat mass (Table 4). Castrated males consumed significantly more feed and had a higher ADG than females in the second series (Table S1). Series 1 had higher lean mass and thus higher CP than series 2 in both sexes, but females had higher lean mass and CP only in series 2. Males in series 1 had lower lean mass and CP than females in series 2. Fat mass in the carcass differed between the farrowing series, with the second series having significantly lower fat mass than the first one, and males in series 1 had higher fat mass than females in series 2. Within the second, but not within the first series, females had significantly lower fat mass than males (Table S1, Fig. 1). Neither the series nor the sexes differed in FCR (Table 4).

Table 4 Means and standard deviations of protein efficiency, performance and carcass traits compared across sex and farrowing series. Significant effects (p < 0.05) are highlighted in bold. Post-hoc comparisons are indicated as superscripts and fully shown in Table S1. Different subscript letters (within a row) indicate statistically significant differences between the groups, while the same letters indicate no difference.
Figure 1
figure 1

Comparison of fat mass between farrowing series and sexes using 2-sided Games-Howell pairwise tests. P-values are Holm-adjusted and bars are shown only for significant comparisons. From left to right: green: females in first farrowing series, yellow: males in first farrowing series, blue: females in second farrowing series, pink: males in second farrowing series.

Protein efficiency

The average PE was 0.39 ± 0.02, but the farrowing series differed significantly in their PE (Fig. 2, left; Welch two-sample t-test, t = -4.03, df = 89.87, p < 0.001). The first farrowing series had a mean PE of 0.38 ± 0.02 and the second a mean PE of 0.40 ± 0.02. To account for this difference, we included the pen ID nested in farrowing series (in addition to the individual ID) in the following models. The sexes did not differ significantly in terms of PE (Fig. 2, right; Welch two-sample t-test, t = 0.20, df = 78.28, p = 0.84).

Figure 2
figure 2

Protein efficiency (proportion of protein ingested that was retained in the carcass) in the two farrowing series (left) and sexes (right). Abbreviations: F females, M castrated males.

Damaging and potentially problematic behaviors

Of the total 5,479 actions recorded over the four observations, 11% were directed at chains, barriers or drinkers, 27% at straw, and 61% at conspecifics. Most pig-directed behaviors targeted the body and head, i.e., less vulnerable regions, and fewer actions were directed at vulnerable regions (ETV regions; Fig. 3). Damaging behaviors, i.e., biting directed at a pen mate’s EVT regions, was observed on only nine occasions. The eight pigs performing these damaging behaviors all had an average or below-average PE. Of all 92 pigs, only three were involved in tail biting (3.2%): two bit pen mates and one was both a biter and a victim. Most pigs performed potentially problematic behaviors, but 16 pigs (17%) did not exhibit these behaviors in any of the observations. Only four pigs (4%) were not subjected to these behaviors, and only one pig was not involved at all (neither as performer nor receiver).

Figure 3
figure 3

The percentages of behaviors directed at the body, head, ears, tails, vulva or perineum of all active pig-directed behaviors recorded during all observations. Absolute values are shown below in smaller print. The grouping of the behaviors into damaging (biting directed toward ear, tail, vulva or perineum) or problematic behaviors (manipulation with mouth or nose except for biting towards these vulnerable regions) is illustrated with a blue line, and potentially problematic behaviors (biting and manipulation with mouth or nose to body and head) is illustrated with a green line.

We did not find strong evidence that the number of problematic behaviors (seizing and manipulation directed toward a conspecific’s ETV regions) was associated with PE. In best model, PE was present but not significantly associated with the behavior (Table 5; Fig. 4). Sex was not included in the best model. There were four other models with a similar fit in terms of AICc. PE was included in two of them, but it was never significantly associated with problematic behaviors (Table S2). Neither PE nor sex was included in the best model of the number of potentially problematic behaviors (biting, seizing, and manipulation with the snout) directed at a conspecific’s less vulnerable body areas (non-ETV regions) (Table 5; Fig. 4). Another model had a similar fit in terms of AICc, and it included PE, but it was not significant (Table S2). The best model of problematic behaviors received toward ETV regions only included the intercept; thus, there was no evidence that PE or sex was associated with these behaviors (Table 5; Fig. 4). There was no other model in the confidence set (Table S3). Concerning manipulations of the whole body received, PE was included in the best model, but it was not significantly associated with the behaviors (Table 5; Fig. 4). Also in the other two equally fitting models, PE was either not included or not significant (Table S3).

Table 5 Best models from the model selection for problematic (ETV) and potentially problematic (head and body) behaviours performed and received as a function of protein efficiency and sex. Only the best fitting models in terms of AICc are shown.
Figure 4
figure 4

Counts of behaviors directed at or received by pen mates as a function of protein efficiency. (A) Number of problematic behaviors performed, i.e., toward ear, tail, vulva or perineum (ETV), (B) Number of potentially problematic behaviors performed, i.e., toward body or head, (C) Number of problematic behaviors received, i.e., toward ETV, (D) Number of potentially problematic behaviors performed, i.e., toward body or head. Note that the regression line in (A) is quasi-Poisson, for (B) to (D) Poisson (zero-inflation, as well as negative binomial regression lines are not implemented in the function).

Lesions, tail posture, and cleanliness

Thirteen pigs with a medium-range PE (mean PE = 0.39, min = 0.33, max = 0.42), had minor wounds, and only one pig had a part of the tail missing (PE = 0.41), with a PE slightly above average. The other animals had no wounds or only superficial scratches. Five pigs had a straight tail (mean PE = 0.38, min = 0.33, max = 0.41), and all others had curled tails. We observed one pig having a straight tail over all four observations, another over three, two over two, and one over one observation. No pigs with tucked tails were observed, and all pigs were clean, except one that was slightly soiled (PE = 0.37) during two observations.

Initiation and outcome of confrontations

The best model for the number of confrontations a pig initiated included PE, but it was not significantly associated, but close to significance (Table 6, Fig. 5). The confidence set contained another model that only included the intercept (Table S4). Only weight was included in the best model for winning a confrontation, but it was not significant (Table 6, Fig. 5). Two of the three other models within a ΔAICc of 2 of the best model indicated that PE and weight are associated with winning. In two of the models, high PE was significantly correlated with the number of confrontations won. Heavier pigs won confrontations more often. Sex was included in one of the models but was not significantly related to winning (Table S5). The best model for losing a confrontation included only the intercept (Table 6, Fig. 5). The other model in the confidence set included weight, which was not significant (Table S6).

Table 6 Best models from the model selection for initiation and outcome of confrontations as a function of protein efficiency, sex and body weight. Only the best fitting models in terms of AICc are shown. Significant effects (p < 0.05) are highlighted in bold, and effects with 0.05 ≥ p < 0.10 in italics.
Figure 5
figure 5

Counts of confrontations initiated, their outcomes and straw rooting counts as a function of protein efficiency. (A) Number of confrontations initiated, (B) number of confrontations won, (C) number of confrontations lost, (D) number of straw rooting bouts performed per sex (females in pink, males in blue). Note that the regression line in (A) to (C) is quasi-Poisson, and Poisson in (D) (zero-inflation, as well as negative binomial regression lines are not implemented in the function).

Straw rooting

The best model for straw rooting included PE, which was not significant, and sex, which was significant (Table 6, Fig. 5). PE was also not significantly associated with straw rooting in the other model with similar fit (ΔACIc ≤ 2 to the best model, Table S7). Sex was significantly associated with the number of straw rooting instances in both models, with females rooting more frequently than castrated males.

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to investigate the relationship between PE and harmful or potentially problematic behaviors in pigs. The relatively large reduction in dietary crude protein of 20% must be considered when interpreting the results, as this has been associated with an increased prevalence of damaging behaviors. Conversely, the pigs in this study were given fresh, unchopped straw daily, sufficient access to feed, floor space above the legal requirements, and daily pen cleaning, and were closely monitored for signs of damaging behavior. These are all favorable conditions that limit stress and the risk of damaging behavior. Great care was given to formulate a diet that included balanced amounts of EAA. Deficiencies in EAA, such as methionine, are suspected of fueling tail biting6,7. It should be noted that the experimental diet used in the project represents a relatively strict reduction, which can only be recommended for use in practice if the space available per pig is greater than recommended and the sanitary conditions are optimal, otherwise the risk of tail biting may be increased6. Throughout the larger project19, from October 2018 to June 2021, when the nearly 700 pigs were slaughtered, piggery staff reported their impression of increased nervousness in the pigs, which they attributed to the protein reduction. One tail biting outbreak was reported in February 2020, before the present behavioral observation study. This outbreak could be contained by providing the pigs with paper bags and hay several times per day. Engaging with these materials, which were novel to them, was sufficient to reduce tail biting. From then on, only 12 instead of 14 pigs were housed per feeding station.

Damaging or problematic behaviors

In the present study, we did not find evidence that PE is associated with an increased risk of damaging behavior toward pen mates. Only a few pigs showed damaging behavior in terms of biting the ETV regions of pen mates, and these pigs’ PE did not stand out in any direction. Further, there was no evidence that PE made an individual more susceptible to receiving damaging behaviors. However, to assess animal welfare more comprehensively, it is essential to consider other types of behaviors, such as other oral and nasal behaviors, directed at the ETV areas of pen mates (termed “problematic behaviors” in our study), and even behaviors directed at the head and the body (termed “potentially problematic behaviors”), in addition to tail (and ear) biting. Concerning problematic behaviors, we did not find strong evidence of an association with PE, but as PE was present in two confidence set models, we cannot completely rule it out. Furthermore, there was no evidence that PE was linked to potentially problematic behaviors. It is unclear whether the oral and nasal manipulation of conspecifics is harmful, as they represent positive interactions (allogrooming16,39). However, if performed excessively out of boredom or stress, they can affect and stress pen mates16 and even lead to injuries.

There was no evidence that sex affected any of the problematic or potentially problematic behaviors, which is consistent with what has been reported previously8. However, this might differ when entire males are reared. For instance, Clouard et al.40 reported that while female and non-castrated male piglets were generally equally active, the latter engaged in more social interactions than females. The increased protein requirements of boars compared to females and castrated animals could also lead to an increased frequency of damaging behavior if the diet does not meet the boars' needs12. It is unsurprising that many injuries occurred outside the 5-min observation periods. Moreover, not all behaviors resulted in visible injuries. Concerning tail posture, which has been suggested as an indicator of tail biting11, we could not establish a clear connection to PE, as the five animals in which a straight (hanging) tail posture was observed (all others had raised curled tails) had an approximately average PE. Some pigs with straight tails were observed as being exposed to damaging or potentially problematic behavior, but most straight-tailed pigs were not exposed to an unusual number of behaviors during the observation, therefore it is likely the straight-tail posture indicates interactions that occurred outside the observation intervals. It has to be noted that straight tails are not necessarily linked to tail biting, but also indicate a positive and/or relaxed state in pigs, especially in enriched environments39.

The relationship between PE and potentially problematic behaviors presented here is purely phenotypic. However, in the absence of genetic studies, it might be cautiously interpreted as a proxy for genetic correlations41,42. Thus, when engaging in genetic selection for increased PE, a dramatic increase in tail biting due to co-selection is highly unlikely, but it is advisable to monitor pig behavior carefully to detect any deterioration. The total observation time of 20 min per pig in this study is perhaps a rather short period of time in view of the fact that behaviors are labile traits to have any indication of a genetic basis for these traits. The number of behavioral observations available in a study is necessarily limited due to their laborious nature, but several promising computer vision tools are being developed43,44 that may allow automated detection of behaviors that can be used in high-throughput phenotyping for genetic studies.

Frequency and outcome of confrontations

Using the number of confrontations initiated and their outcomes as an indicator of whether aggression or dominance is associated with efficiency, we found some evidence of the role of PE in how often pigs started a confrontation and how often they won it. While a pig tended to initiate more confrontations when it had a higher PE, which was close to but not significant, there was also evidence that pigs with higher PE won confrontations more often, as PE was included in two of the four models with practically equal fit, and it was significant both times. One might speculate that this could be explained by the greater strength of protein-efficient pigs, since they might have a higher muscle mass. The number of confrontations lost was not linked to PE. If a pen mate occupies a place or resource that the focal pig wants to use, there will be a brief confrontation until the matter is settled, either one animal clearly wins and the other loses, or an undetermined outcome is also possible. Confrontations among familiar pigs in established groups are a normal part of pigs’ behavioral repertoire and reflect the dominance hierarchy in the pen40. If the pigs can solve a confrontation rather quickly without causing much injury, these are not a matter of concern.

Straw rooting

The number of pig-oriented actions was much higher than the number of object-oriented actions (including metal chains, pen barriers, drinking bowls, and straw) in this study, similar to the results of Meer et al.6, in whose study only a chain with a hard plastic tube was available as enrichment. In our case, straw was provided daily, and the animals engaged with it extensively. However, this might have satisfied only part of their urge to interact with mobile, flexible, and deformable objects6,45,46. One might speculate that the provision of straw has already prevented some of the abnormal manipulations of pen mates, which would have been more pronounced in the absence of this occupational opportunity16,40. PE did not seem to be linked with the frequency of straw rooting, but we found that females rooted more than castrated males, which has been reported previously47,48.

Conclusion

The potential consequences of selection for higher PE on pig welfare are thus far unknown, but a trade-off between sustainability and welfare is a concern. In this study, the first to examine the relationship between PE and tail biting or potentially problematic behaviors, we found no evidence of a major risk of an increase in harmful behaviors with higher PE, even in a scenario in which protein in feed is severely restricted. However, the statistical approach used here does not allow us to entirely rule out the possibility that PE may be related to an increased frequency of manipulation of pen mates. While this may not necessarily be problematic, as the behavior was not directed at vulnerable body regions, it could possibly indicate misdirected foraging behavior. Consequently, we believe it is important to provide pigs with an appropriate environment and sufficient space, possibly combined with close monitoring of pens through automatic early warning systems10, especially if they are to become more efficient through breeding. Moreover, the effects of breeding for higher PE should be carefully evaluated for possible negative effects on pigs’ physiology and immune systems to avoid these potential causes of damaging behavior. In addition, efforts should also be made to improve the stress susceptibility of pigs through breeding. While this has so far been hampered by low or zero heritability20, the development of biomarkers49,50and automated phenotyping43,44) could be successful in the future.