Introduction

Avoiding detection either for protection from predators or to go unnoticed by potential prey is essential for individual survival. The threat of predation has led to the evolution of various camouflage mechanisms, which make potential prey more difficult to detect or recognize. The most prominent mechanism is visual camouflage that includes highly adaptive colouration strategies among animals1. One strategy to achieve visual camouflage is background matching (also termed “crypsis” 2). For background matching, animals try to match colour, luminance and pattern of their background.

While background matching is one of the most common and frequently studied strategies of visual camouflage2,3,4,5,6, another important mechanism is concealing the outline of the body. Thayer7 proposed that detecting the outline of their prey is one of the ways predators locate and identify their prey. In general, the detection of edges is an essential task for object recognition8,9. In this regard, disruptive colouration makes animals less detectable. It involves a set of markings that creates false edges within the animal hindering the detection or recognition of its true outline and shape or parts of it1,4,7. Cott4 suggested that structural modifications of the organism’s outline themselves could contribute to camouflage by creating an ‘irregular marginal form’. This makes the animal’s true body outline effectively diffused and hence makes it harder to detect4. Recently, support for the ‘irregular form’ hypothesis was found in an experimental study showing that false holes markings reduce avian predations10.

Birds with their typically advanced vision and high plumage diversity have been featured prominently in camouflage research, either as predators or as prey5,6,10,11,12,13,14. When studying camouflage as an anti-predator defence in birds, much research has examined the clutches/eggs of ground-nesting birds6,15,16,17. These studies revealed that ground-nesting birds may increase background matching through adaptive egg colouration that matches the nest site6,13 and some species even improve the background matching of their clutches, by soiling their eggs to conceal them better18, using egg-matching nest materials19 or covering the clutch with debris or soil when predators approach20.

However, not only eggs are vulnerable to predation. Chicks are also often targeted by predators. Precocial chicks leave their nest within a few hours of hatching. Initially, those chicks suffer from high mortality as they are limited in their mobility and hence highly vulnerable to predation21,22,23. To improve their survival, chicks rely on camouflage provided by their feathers especially during the first days of their lives. The plumage colouration of precocial chicks featured prominently in the description of camouflage mechanisms such as disruptive colouration4,7,24. However, we know surprisingly little about plumage characteristics that improve camouflage in chicks, which represents an important gap in our knowledge. Precocial chicks hatch fully covered with neoptile down feathers25. With maturation, the neoptile feathers are shed, and the natal plumage is replaced by the teleoptile feathers, which can be categorised into, e.g. flight, contour and down feathers26. One striking feature of neoptile feathers is that they are protruding from the chick’s body. The unequal length of the very thin feathers creates a fringed feather region that may conceal the chick outline and hence make it harder to detect by predators.

In this study, we investigated whether neoptile down feathers improve camouflage through outline diffusion. Cott4 discussed this strategy of an ‘irregular marginal form’ mainly with examples of masquerade, where the irregular shapes of animals resemble elements of their environment, e.g. parts of plants. In contrast, we hypothesized that the fringed feathery outline helps the chick to better blend with the background by reducing edge contrasts and/or creating a transition zone of intermediate luminance.

In a first experiment, we explored the mechanism of outline diffusion by appendages in principle modelling a circular object with varying protruding appendages. We then used the Local Edge Intensity Analysis (LEIA)27 on the created images to investigate whether appendages decreased the edge contrast of the object’s outline. Additionally, we investigated how appendage characteristics such as their density, thickness, transparency, and variation in background complexity and spatial acuity of the predator’s visual system affected edge intensity in the contour region. With the Mean Luminance Comparison (MLC), we tested whether appendages altered the luminance of a narrow ‘transition zone’ between object and background as a second mechanism. We hypothesized that an intermediate mean luminance in the transition zone that reduces the contrast would help to blend the object better with the background.

In a second experiment, we tested whether the neoptile feathers contribute to the camouflage of precocial chicks hiding from an approaching human. For this, we analysed images taken from precocial snowy plover (Charadrius nivosus) chicks in natural habitats. Very young plover chicks rely on their crypsis to evade predation as they stay motionless on the ground when a threat is approaching21. We digitally cropped all chicks once with and once without protruding feathers and transferred them on to images of their hiding background taken after gently removing the chicks. For chicks cropped with their protruding feathers, we predicted the edge intensity of the chick outline to be reduced and the mean luminance difference of the transitions zone to be closer to intermediate optimum than for the images of those chicks cropped without their feathers.

Results

Experiment 1: artificial object

All created images of the modelled artificial object in all scenarios showed multimodal density distributions of pixels’ edge intensities (Fig. 1a). Pixels showing the highest edge intensities were found at the object outline. These high edge intensity pixels (HEI pixels) showed prominent modal peaks in all multimodal density distributions (Fig. 1a). For the object without appendages, 1.59% corresponded to pixels that made up a distinct modal area with a mean edge intensity of 2.7 (Fig. 1a, ‘0’). Consequently, we used a threshold of 1.59% to define HEI pixels for all images hereafter. Adding appendages reduced the mean edge intensities of the HEI pixels with the lowest mean edge intensity reached in the image with an intermediate number of 256 appendages (Fig. 1a,b).

Figure 1
figure 1

Local edge intensity analysis (LEIA) of the contour region in the artificial object experiment. (a) Ridgeline plots showing the density distribution of the edge intensity according to number of appendages. The highest 1.59% of the pixels are shaded in grey (high edge intensity pixels, HEI pixels). (b) Scenario 1: variation in appendage thickness. (c) Scenario 2: variation in appendage transparency. (d) Scenario 3: variation in appendage length. (e) Scenario 4: variation in background complexity. (f) Scenario 5: variation in spatial acuity.

Appendage characteristics

Increasing appendage thickness (Scenario 1) resulted in overall higher mean edge intensities suggesting higher detectability than in the basic scenario. With thicker appendages, the lowest mean edge intensity of the HEI pixels was reached already with 128 appendages. Images with more than 128 appendages had higher mean edge intensity values implying a deterioration of camouflage (Fig. 1b). Increasing appendage transparency (Scenario 2) yielded overall slightly higher mean edge intensities than observed in the basic scenario. The lowest mean edge intensities were reached with more appendages than in the basic scenario (Fig. 1c) with the minimum mean edge intensity shown for 512 appendages at 25% transparency and the full circle of appendages at 50% transparency (Fig. 1c). Increasing appendage length heterogeneity (Scenario 3) yielded the same low mean edge intensity values as the basic scenario (Fig. 1d). However, more appendages were required to reach minimal mean edge intensity values than in the basic scenario. The minimum mean edge intensity was reached with 512 appendages when half of the appendages had 50% of the length or with the full circle when half of the appendages had 25% and a quarter had 50% of the length (Fig. 1d).

Background complexity and spatial acuity

Introducing background complexity (Scenario 4) resulted in similar mean edge intensities of the HEI pixels for 256 appendages as in the basic scenario for large squares. The region of interest (ROI) on the background with small squares showed slightly higher mean edge intensities for the HEI pixels than for the background with large squares. More appendages did not lead to such a pronounced increase of mean edge intensities as in the basic scenario (Fig. 1e). Lowering the spatial acuity of the perceiver (Scenario 5) decreased the mean edge intensity severely. At a spatial acuity of 10 cpd, the minimum mean edge intensity of the HEI pixels in the image with 256 appendages was only half of the value obtained in the basic scenario (Fig. 1f).

ROI size

The main analysis focussed on the contour region as the ROI. It contains the area covered by the appendages and a small part of the adjacent background and object inside. Changing the ROI size and examining a larger part of background and object (Supplementary Fig. S2) produced qualitatively similar results except for variation in background complexity (Scenario 4). In that scenario, the number of appendages had no influence on the mean edge intensity of the HEI pixels (Supplementary Fig. S2) for the enlarged ROI.

Mean luminance comparison

The mean luminance of the area covered by appendages (appendage region) was generally intermediate between the luminance of object and background across all scenarios indicating the formation of a luminance transition zone (Supplementary Fig. S3). For the basic scenario, the optimal intermediate luminance would have been reached for objects that have between 256 and 512 appendages, when 50% of the appendage region were covered by appendages. With increased appendage thickness, the appendage region reached the optimum with fewer appendages. With increased appendage transparency and length heterogeneity, more appendages were needed to reach the optimum. Background complexity and spatial acuity did not affect the mean luminance of the appendage region.

Experiment 2: chick photographs

Based on sun position, the chicks shaded the background in a part of the contour region. Therefore, we excluded shaded areas from the analysis, which caused a reduction of the ROIs. On average, we were able to analyse on average 72% of the chick’s contour region with LEIA. Across the ROIs of the 15 chicks, the mean threshold for the HEI pixels was 0.9826 (Supplementary Table S3). Consequently, we compared on average 1.74% of the pixels between photographs of cropped chicks with and without the protruding neoptile feathers.

For 14 of 15 chicks (93.3%), the mean edge intensities of HEI pixels were lower for the cropped image of each chick with protruding neoptile feathers than for the corresponding images without protruding neoptile feathers (Fig. 2b–d). Accordingly, images including the protruding feathers showed lower mean edge intensities of HEI pixels than those excluding them (Fig. 2d, paired t-test: t = 5.289, df = 14, p-value < 0.001). The mean edge intensity difference of HEI pixels between measurements with and without feathers was 0.192 (95%CI: 0.114, 0.270).

Figure 2
figure 2

(a) A snowy plover chick hiding on the ground from an approaching predator, (b) cropped chick transferred to image of empty natural background with neoptile contour feathers protruding the outline, (c) cropped chick transferred without protruding neoptile contour feathers. The contour region (red) as the region of interest was analysed in the Local Edge Intensity Analysis (LEIA). Areas, where the background was shaded by the chick in the original image (blue), were excluded from the analysis. (d) Mean edge intensity of the HEI pixels in the contour region with and without feathers for 15 snowy plover chicks (t = 5.289, df = 14, p-value < 0.001). Measurements are paired by chick ID. The error bars indicate group mean ± standard error. The figure was produced using R28 Adobe Photoshop29and InDesign30.

For the MLC, presence of protruding neoptile feathers did not contribute to creating a transition zone between chick and background, as we did not observe more intermediate mean luminance values in the ROI in comparison to ROI of chick pictures without protruding feathers (Supplementary material, Fig. S4).

Discussion

The plumage of newly hatched chicks has several known functions. First, the feathers are important for thermoregulation31. Second, plumage colour variation is also an important signal that may reveal chick condition and facilitate individual recognition for parents24,32,33. Third, in precocial chicks, the plumage provides camouflage through cryptic colouration4,24. Here we tested whether neoptile feathers help to conceal the outline of chicks to make them harder to detect for predators. Our results from a proof of principle analysis (experiment 1) and analysis of real chick images in their natural environment (experiment 2) suggest that appendages, such as protruding neoptile feathers, improve concealment of the object outline, particularly by decreasing the edge intensity. Weak contrast edges are associated with low conspicuousness34. This enhances diffusion of the outline and decreases detectability as the shape is an important cue for predators locating and identifying a prey item7.

In the artificial setup (experiment 1), appendages both reduced edge intensity and created a transition zone with an intermediate mean luminance in the appendage region suggesting that both mechanisms help to conceal the object outline. However, when analysing the impact of neoptile feathers on outline concealment of chicks in their natural background (experiment 2), we found that the presence/absence of protruding feathers did only change edge intensity but not mean luminance of the ROI in the predicted way. ROIs on images where the chick was cropped including its protruding feathers had lower edge intensity but no consistent change in the intermediate luminance was found. This suggests that the lowering of edge intensity is a better mechanism for outline diffusion than creating a transition zone with intermediate luminance for concealing the outline of precocial chicks. However, the MLC may be methodologically problematic for these pictures. Measuring mean luminance across the ROI may not capture the outline diffusion when both object and background are not monochromatic coloured but consist of a mottled pattern, which is frequently the case for natural habitats.

Altering the characteristics of appendages, background and predator vision had mechanism-specific consequences. As we concluded that reduction of edge intensity is the more likely mechanism, we restrict our discussion here to the impact of parameter changes on edge intensity. In the artificial setup, we found that an intermediate number of regular appendages helped to conceal the outline of the monochromatic object best. Further, we found that appendage thickness, transparency and length heterogeneity influenced outline concealment. They altered the optimal number of appendages needed and, in some cases, changed also the edge intensity. Protruding neoptile feathers of precocial chicks are thin, somewhat transparent and vary in the extent to which they stand out from the outline. Our results show that thicker appendages would lead overall to higher detectability and in that case, fewer appendages would lead to better concealment. In contrast, higher transparency required more appendages for best concealment. Similarly, we found that with increasing length heterogeneity more appendages were needed to achieve low edge intensities and reduce detectability. The discrepancy between optimal characteristics for outline concealment and the true feather characteristics observed in snowy plover chicks might be partly explained by the multiple functions that feathers provide, as they also are important in thermoregulation and provide colouration for the cryptic chick plumage.

Variation in spatial acuity is high across visual systems of different predators and had the largest effect on edge intensity. Intermediate to high appendage numbers reduced the edge intensity of the ROI most, regardless of spatial acuity of the simulated predator. Yet, mean edge intensities were highest for the simulated system with the highest spatial acuity. From the same viewing distance, predators with high spatial acuity, such as humans or birds of prey, perceive a lot more details of an object compared to predators with a lower spatial acuity such as canids or corvids35. As spatial acuity decreases with viewing distance36, mammalian predators need to approach feathered chicks closer to detect their outline.

Interestingly, background complexity did not alter the optimal number of appendages nor impact overall edge intensities dramatically. Background complexity often makes detection of objects harder and therefore contributes to camouflage37,38. The multicoloured fringed feathers themselves could contribute to increasing complexity. Such an effect would have the largest impact on a more uniform background. The mixture of appendages and background will also create new false edges and increase disruptive colouration39. Nevertheless, any such effects by protruding feathers are likely to be small as the feather region is only very narrow and, hence, will only impact the immediate surrounding of the chick. Hence it is still unclear whether the effects that we detected are biologically relevant for detection through predators.

One drawback of our study is that we did not test empirically whether the appendages indeed reduce detectability by predators, e.g. through a predation experiment (e.g. similar to5,11). Measuring the detection time of objects with and without appendages similar to protruding neoptile feathers would be an important test for the relevance of this mechanism in nature. Concealing the outline is unlikely to be the main antipredator strategy of chicks. We rather suggest that it works in concert with the cryptic colouration of the downy plumage, chick behaviour such as finding optimal hiding places and predator distraction or defence through their parents. Yet our results regarding the spatial acuity suggest that the fringed feathers could be an important component of a visual antipredator strategy against mammalian predators. Even if the reduction in detectability is only small, concealing the outline may enhance survival of precocial chicks during early life when chicks face a very high predation risk21,22,23, especially as the costs for having the protruding feathers may not be high.

Appendages that alter the outline are commonly found in nature. Examples of vertebrates with irregular outlines are known, e.g. from cephalopods40, fish3, amphibians41 and reptiles42. A striking example is provided by many insect larvae such as hairy caterpillars which, similar to small chicks, have typically reduced mobility in comparison with the adult form. Birds have a strong influence on caterpillar mortality43 and hairy caterpillars are less preferred prey for avian predators than non-hairy caterpillars44. Concealing the outline might be one currently underappreciated function of hairy appendages although further research is required to establish firmly that hairy appendages improve camouflage.

Conclusion

The ‘irregular marginal form’ as a camouflage strategy has inspired early researchers on camouflage4 but evidence for this mechanism so far has been limited. Our results suggest that body appendages such as feathers or hairs can help to create an ‘irregular marginal form’ that serves to diffuse the object outline. Appendages with the characteristics of protruding neoptile feathers reduced the edge intensity in a proof of principle analysis and on images of precocial chicks taken in their natural environment. Appendages also served to reduce mean luminance differences when both object and background were uniformly coloured but this mechanism failed to contribute to outline diffusion when we analysed images of chicks in their natural backgrounds. Improved camouflage through outline diffusion could be an important function of heterogenous integuments which are found in a variety of organisms.

Material and methods

Experiment 1: proof of principle

As a proof of principle, we designed the first experiment to test whether appendages may help to conceal the outline. We created an image of a uniformly light grey coloured circular object with a size of 2950 pixels (px)/250.0 mm circumference and 470 px/39.8 mm radius on a dark grey background using Adobe InDesign CS6 version 8.030. The initial setup started with no appendages added to the outline (Fig. 3a, ‘0’). We then added object-coloured appendages (i.e. lines of 1 Pt/4 px/0.4 mm thickness and 118 px/10.0 mm length) with regular intervals resembling protruding neoptile chick feathers orthogonally to the object outline (‘Basic Scenario’, Fig. 3a). The first image with appendages had 32 appendages added to the outline (Fig. 3a, ‘32′). We then doubled the number of appendages stepwise creating denser spaced appendages to the outline until the extended outline was completely filled (Fig. 3a, ‘full circle’). For the vision of a simulated predator, we used the spatial acuity from humans (Homo sapiens, 72 cycles per degree, cpd)36,45,46 in the basic scenario. The full details for the parameters are provided in Supplementary Table S1 (a–g).

Figure 3
figure 3

(a) Basic Scenario: Seven stages of the artificial chick setup with varying number of thin, non-transparent appendages having all the same length. (b) Scenario 1: varying appendage thickness applied to the Basic Scenario. (c) Scenario 2: varying appendage transparency applied to the Basic Scenario. (d) Scenario 3: varying appendage length heterogeneity applied to the Basic Scenario. (e) Scenario 4: varying background complexity with chessboard backgrounds. (f) Scenario 5: high, medium and low spatial acuity applied to the Basic Scenario. (a–f) The analysed region of interest (ROI) is highlighted in red for clarification only. The figure was produced in Adobe Photoshop29 and InDesign30.

To further explore the mechanism, we altered appendage characteristics, background and the spatial acuity of the predator. First, we increased appendage thickness to 2 Pt/8 pixels/ 0.7 mm (Scenario 1a) and 3 Pt/12 pixels/1.1 mm (Scenario 1b) resulting in decreased inter-appendage intervals (Fig. 3b and Supplementary Table S1, h–u). Second, we changed appendage transparency to 25% (Scenario 2a) and 50% transparency (Scenario 2b) (Fig. 3c and Supplementary Table S1, v to ai). Third, we varied the appendage length heterogeneity; half of the appendages having 50% of the length (Scenario 3a), and half of the appendages at 25% and one quarter at 50% of the original appendage length (Scenario 3b) (Fig. 3d and Supplementary Table S1, aj to aw). Fourth, we investigated the effect of background complexity on the detectability of the outline. As background, we used a chessboard pattern with large squares (346 pixels/29.3 mm, Scenario 4a) and with small squares (86 pixels/7.3 mm, Scenario 4b) (Fig. 3e and Supplementary Table S1, ax to bk). Fifth, we altered the spatial acuity to test whether or how the visual systems of different predators would affect detectability. We simulated the spatial acuity of a corvid predator (30 cpd, Scenario 5a) and canid predator (10 cpd, Scenario 5b) (Fig. 3f and Supplementary Table S1, bl to by), the two most common predators of ground-nesting plovers16,47,48. This range also covered other potential predators (Supplementary Table S2).

We did not account for differences in colour vision between different predators as the setup mostly consists of greyscale images that predominantly differ in luminance. Note that in many animals, visual acuity is greater for achromatic than chromatic stimuli34,49.

We conducted visual modelling and visual analysis using the Quantitative Colour Pattern Analysis (QCPA) framework27 integrated into the Multispectral Image Analysis and Calibration (MICA) toolbox50 for ImageJ version 1.52a51. We converted the generated images into multispectral images containing the red, green and blue channel in a stack and transformed them further into 32-bits/channel cone-catch images based on the human visual system, which are required by the framework. To create the luminance channel, we averaged the long and medium wave channel, which is thought to be representative of human vision52. We modelled the spatial acuity with Gaussian Acuity Control at a viewing distance of 1300 mm and a minimum resolvable angle (MRA) of 0.01389. To increase biological accuracy, we applied a Receptor Noise Limited (RNL) filter that reduces noise and reconstructs edges in the image. The RNL filter used the Weber fractions “Human 0.05” provided by the framework (longwave 0.05, mediumwave 0.07071, shortwave 0.1657), luminance 0.1, 5 iterations, a radius of 5 pixels and a falloff of 3 pixels as specified in van den Berg et al.27 (Supplementary Fig. S1).

To test for the detectability of the outline, we used LEIA27, which is conceptually similar to the boundary strength analysis34. Boundary strength analysis requires an image with clearly delineated (clustered) colour and luminance pattern elements. However, a large degree of subthreshold details, which may be still perceived by the viewer gets lost in the clustering process. LEIA has the advantage of not requiring such a clustered input and therefore can be directly applied to RNL filtered images. LEIA measures the edge intensity (i.e. the luminance contrast) locally at each position in the image. The output image displays ΔS values in a 32-bit stack of four slices, where each slice shows the values measured in different angles (horizontal, vertical and the two diagonals, for more details, see van den Berg et al.27).

We ran LEIA on the chosen region of interest (ROI) with the same Weber fractions used for the RNL filter. The ROI was a 180 pixel-wide band that included the area of the appendages extended by 30 pixels towards the object inside and towards the outside (Fig. 3a). We log-transformed the ΔS values as recommended for natural scenes53 to make the results comparable to the natural background images used in Experiment 2 (see below). To test whether the size of the ROI affected our results, we ran an additional analysis using a 1500 × 1500 pixel-wide rectangle surrounding the object as the ROI, which included a bigger area of the background and the full object inside (Supplementary Fig. S2).

We extracted the luminance ΔS values from the four slices of the output image stack in ImageJ and stored them in separate matrices for further analysis using R version 3.5.328. ImageJ generally assigned values outside the chosen ROI to zero. Thus, we first discarded all values of zero. We then set all negative values that arose as artefacts in areas without any edges to zero, in order to make them biologically meaningful. We then identified the parallel maximum (R function pmax ()) of the four interrelated direction matrices and transferred this value to a new matrix.

High luminance and colour contrasts imply high conspicuousness34. Consequently, a lower luminance contrast leads to lower conspicuousness and therefore, better camouflage. As the outline is an important cue for predators locating and identifying a prey item7, we assumed that especially low contrasts in the outline of an object improve camouflage. Thus, a reduction of edge intensity in the object outline by the appendages indicates a camouflage improvement. To test whether the object outline became less detectable we compared the edge intensity of the outline pixels in the basic scenario without appendages (Supplementary Table S1, a) with corresponding pixels from other scenarios. The outline pixels were characterised by high edge intensity and constituted a prominent peak. They comprised 1.59% of all pixels in the analysis focused on the contour region (see “Results”, Fig. 1a). For all scenarios, we calculated the mean edge intensity of the high edge intensity pixels (HEI pixels) and identified the changes with parameter variation. Unless otherwise stated we used R28 to produce graphs and panels.

As an alternative mechanism, we tested whether appendages create a transition zone with intermediate luminance around the object (Mean Luminance Comparison (MLC), Supplementary material). We calculated the mean luminance of the object inside up to the border (object region), the area covered by appendages (appendage region) and the background (background region). We predicted that the appendage region would be characterised by intermediate luminance between object and background and therefore provide a luminance transition zone to conceal the object outline.

Experiment 2: chick photographs

Using pictures of young snowy plover chicks hiding when approached by a simulated predator, we tested if protruding neoptile feathers helped to conceal the chicks’ outline and therefore improve their camouflage.

We studied snowy plovers in their natural environment at Bahía de Ceuta, Sinaloa, Mexico. Fieldwork permits were granted by the Secretaría de Medio Ambiente y Recursos Naturales (SEMARNAT). All field activities were performed in accordance with the approved ethical guidelines outlined by SEMARNAT. The breeding site consists of salt flats that are sparsely vegetated and surrounded by mangroves54. The predators of chicks are not well described but likely similar to the egg predator community that includes several mammalian predators such as racoon, opossum, coyote, bob cat, avian predators such as crested caracara Caracara cheriway and reptiles17. General field methodology is provided elsewhere55,56. In 2017, we took photographs of young (one to 3 days old) chicks hiding on the ground, that had already left the nest scrape. To photograph the chicks, two observers approached free-roaming families with two mobile hides within the period one hour after sunrise and one hour before sunset. At a distance of 100–200 m, one observer acted as ‘predator’, left the hide and openly approached the brood while the second observer kept watching the chicks. The chicks responded by crouching to the ground and staying motionless while the parents were alarming. The second observer directed the ‘predator’ to the approximate hiding place. When searching for the chicks, we took great care to reduce the number of steps to avoid modification of the ground through our tracks.

Once the first chick had been found, the second observer joined the ‘predator’ and took the chick photographs. We used a Nikon D7000 camera converted to full spectrum including the UV range (Optic Makario GmbH, Germany) and a Nikkor macro 105 mm lens that allows transmission of light at low wavebands. The equipment was chosen because calibration data were available for this combination50. Each hiding background was photographed with and without the chick using a UV pass filter for the UV spectrum and a UV/IR blocking filter (“IR-Neutralisationsfilter NG”, Optic Makario GmbH, Germany) for the visible spectrum. The camera was set to an aperture of f/8, ISO 400 and the pictures were stored in “RAW” file format. We used exposure bracketing to produce three images to ensure that at least one picture was not over or underexposed. A 25% reflectance standard (Zenith Polymer Diffuse Reflectance Standard provided by SPHEREOPTICS, Germany) placed in the corner of each picture enabled a subsequent standardizing of light conditions.

In total, we took pictures of 32 chicks from 15 families. For 21 chicks we obtained photographs suitable for further analyses with an unobstructed view to the entire chick and only one chick per photograph. Of these, we randomly selected pictures of 15 chicks. Unfortunately, it was not possible to obtain proper alignment of visual and UV pictures in ImageJ as either chick or camera moved slightly in the break between changing filters for the two settings. Therefore, we restricted our analyses to human colour vision and discarded the UV pictures for further analysis.

In each picture, we manually selected the chick outline and the feather-boundary as a basis for the ROIs (Fig. 2a–c). The chick outline included bill, legs, rings and all areas densely covered by feathers without background shining through. We then marked the feather-boundary, i.e., the smoothened line created by the protruding neoptile feather tips. In the next step, we transferred images of chicks with or without protruding feathers, i.e. cropped at feather-boundary or chick outline, respectively, and inserted them into a uniform or the natural background. First, we cropped the chick without protruding feathers and transferred it into a uniform black background. Second, we cropped the chick including all feathers and inserted it into exactly the same hiding spot on the picture of the natural background (Fig. 2b). Third, we cropped the chick excluding the protruding feathers and transferred it into the natural background (Fig. 2c).

We then proceeded with LEIA following the protocol of experiment 1 with the following changes. Again, the selected ROI was the contour region ranging from the chick outline extended by 30 pixels towards the chick inside to the feather-boundary extended by 30 pixels towards the outside. We excluded all areas of the ROI that showed a shadow of the chick as the chicks’ shadow was missing on the empty natural background images to which the cropped chicks were transferred to (Fig. 2a–c). We used the images of the cropped chicks on the black background to determine the threshold of the HEI pixels according to the protocol of experiment 1 for each chick separately. For each cropped chick that was transferred to the picture with the natural background, we compared the mean edge intensity of the HEI pixels provided by LEIA with and without protruding feathers (Fig. 2b,c) using a two-sided paired t-test.

We also calculated mean luminance differences for chick photographs. Details for this MLC are given in the supplementary material.