Abstract
Child sleep disorders are increasingly prevalent and understanding early predictors of sleep problems, starting in utero, may meaningfully guide future prevention efforts. Here, we investigated whether prenatal exposure to maternal psychological stress is associated with increased sleep problems in toddlers. We also examined whether fetal brain connectivity has direct or indirect influence on this putative association. Pregnant women underwent fetal resting-state functional connectivity MRI and completed questionnaires on stress, worry, and negative affect. At 3-year follow-up, 64 mothers reported on child sleep problems, and in the subset that have reached 5-year follow-up, actigraphy data (Nā=ā25) has also been obtained. We observe that higher maternal prenatal stress is associated with increased toddler sleep concerns, with actigraphy sleep metrics, and with decreased fetal cerebellar-insular connectivity. Specific mediating effects were not identified for the fetal brain regions examined. The search for underlying mechanisms of the link between maternal prenatal stress and child sleep problems should be continued and extended to other brain areas.
Introduction
Childhood sleep problems are increasingly prevalent. From 2003 to 2012, inadequate sleep increased from 23 to 36% among 6ā9Ā year old US children1. It is reported that 30ā40% of children experience problems falling asleep at night2 and more than a quarter of all parents of young children underestimate their childās sleep requirements as they relate to national guidelines3. Children from low socio-economic background and minority groups may be especially at risk, given data showing that minority children are more likely to go to bed after 11Ā pm and sleep fewer hours at night4. Additionally, parental knowledge about child sleep is enhanced in higher income, higher education households3.
Disturbed sleep in early childhood poses serious consequences for child outcomes and may play an important role in health disparities among minority children5. For instance, longitudinal data suggest that sleep disturbance in early childhood is associated with negative adolescent health outcomes, such as increased risk for obesity especially among minority participants of low SES6. Neurobiological frameworks also identify sleep as a key factor in emotion processing7. In line with this, sleep problems in toddlers have been found to significantly predict internalizing problems at school age8. Identifying predictors of sleep problems early in life informs the development of effective early intervention strategies that have the potential to stave off physical and mental health concerns across the lifespan.
The earliest predictors of child sleep disturbance can potentially be found in utero. Given that child hypervigilance and hyper-arousal are associated with prenatal stress exposure9,10,11,12, it follows that prenatal exposure to maternal stress and negative affect may be a risk factor for the development of early sleep problems. Indeed, a small number of studies have begun to provide evidence for this linkage, reporting sleep problems in children prenatally exposed to maternal mood disturbance13,14 and later links to adult insomnia15. Interestingly, studies also report altered sleep patterns in fetuses of anxious mothers. For example, Groome et al.16 found that fetuses of mothers with high anxiety symptoms spend more time in quiet sleep and exhibited less gross body movements in active sleep. Despite growing evidence of a relationship between the prenatal environment and sleep problems, no study to date has explored neural mechanisms by which maternal stress may affect child sleep patterns before birth.
Alterations in fetal brain structure and function are considered one of the key mechanisms of fetal programming by maternal prenatal stress and negative affect17,18,19,20. Indeed, evidence for altered brain structure and function in relation to maternal stress during pregnancy has accumulated over recent years; for a recent review see21. Recent advances in neuroimaging have made it possible to measure properties of fetal functional brain connectivity in utero22 and opened possibilities for examining the effect of maternal prenatal stress on fetal brain network development. This advancement in fetal functional brain connectivity also holds implications for predicting long term outcomes in children. Recently, Thomason, et al.23 reported the first associations between fetal functional connectivity of the motor system and infant motor development. Here, we study the effects of maternal stress during pregnancy on fetal functional brain connectivity and the mediating role of fetal brain alterations on the association between maternal prenatal stress and postnatal behavioral outcomes (i.e., sleep) in toddlerhood.
Recently, it has been found that the cerebellum plays a key role in the regulation of sleep, in addition to its well-established role in motor functions24,25. The cerebellum grows exceptionally fast in the third trimester of pregnancy and is particularly sensitive to prenatal influences at the end of gestation. Our own investigation of the fetal functional brain network identified three highly connected areas, so-called āhubsā, in the cerebellum, emphasizing the importance of this brain area for fetal functional connectivity26. Further, the cerebellum is often mentioned as one of the key components of altered functional brain networks found in patients with insomnia and sleep deprivation27,28 and research points to a significant role for the cerebellum in sleep-disordered breathing29,30. Taken collectively, these findings point to the cerebellum as a critical region of interest that must be considered in the investigation of brain mechanisms in the association between maternal stress during pregnancy and child sleep problems.
Evaluating sleep and identifying sleep problems in early childhood is of critical importance. However, quantifying child sleep behavior can be challenging. Parentās subjective sleep reports are the most common method for evaluating child sleep31, but this approach is not ideal for several reasons. First, subjective sleep reports are only as informative as the individual parentās knowledge and understanding of their childās sleep. Although they have been found useful in reporting the behavioral and contextual issues related to sleep onset and disruptions32, parent reports lack the capacity measure and track the sleepāwake patterns thought to contribute to physical33 and cognitive outcomes34 in childhood. Second, subjective measures are not well validated in diverse populations32. Given the wide range of limitations in using self-report sleep assessments, objective measures are a desirable alternative. Actigraphy, is one such alternative methodology that takes a multidimensional approach to sleep assessment, captures sleep quantity, quality, architecture and schedule through continuous and extended monitoring. Evidence suggests that actigraphy is best suited for understanding childrenās sleep stability and daily rhythms of rest and activity35. A growing body of literature has evidenced actigraphy as the golden standard for measuring sleep, especially in children36,37. For these reasons, in the present study, we implement actigraphy in a subset of our sample as cross-validation of parental report.
The aim of the current study was to investigate the relationship between maternal stress and negative affect during pregnancy, fetal cortico-cerebellar functional connectivity and toddlerās sleep problems in a predominantly African-American, high-risk sample from Detroit, MI. Our focus on the fetal cerebellum was driven by the increased interest for this brain area by developmental neuroscientists38, the fact that the cerebellum grows exceptionally fast in the third trimester of pregnancy and its extreme sensitivity to prenatal influences at the end of gestation39,40,41,42 and early adversity43. Importantly, the cerebellum is also often implicated in sleep disorders25,27,28,29,30. We first examined whether toddlers prenatally exposed to maternal psychological stress and negative affect experienced increased sleep problems. Sleep problems were reported by the mother for the full group at toddler age and objectively measured using 1-week, 24-h actigraphy measurements in a subsample at preschool age. Next, we investigated how prenatal exposure to maternal stress affected fetal cortico-cerebellar functional connectivity. We also explored sex interactions for the associations between maternal stress and mood during pregnancy, sleep, and fetal functional connectivity, since previous studies have reported sex differences in response to prenatal exposure to maternal stress44,45 and differences between males and females in cerebellum development41. Finally, we tested whether the association between toddlerās prenatal exposure to maternal stress and sleep problems was based on an indirect effect (mediation) via altered fetal cortico-cereballar functional connectivity. Building on our previous work in the fetal functional brain network26, here, we used three fetal hubs in the posterior cerebellum to compute cortico-cerebellar functional connectivity.
Results
Prenatal exposure to maternal prenatal NASF is related to child sleep problems
Higher maternal prenatal NASF was associated with increased mother-reported sleep problems (rā=ā0.295, pā=ā0.018). Follow-up correlations with individual stress/negative affect scales indicated that the strongest association were found for maternal perceived stress (PSST: rā=ā0.279, pā=ā0.028) and worry (PSWQ: rā=ā0.348, pā=ā0.007) during pregnancy, while no association was found for maternal prenatal depression, life satisfaction and anxiety during pregnancy and child sleep (see āSupplemental Materialsā and Table S1 for more details). Additionally, we found a significant interaction with child sex for the association between maternal NASF and child sleep problems (Bā=āāā1.265, tā=āāā2.158, pā=ā0.035). Follow-up testing revealed that the association between maternal NASF during pregnancy and child sleep problems was significant for males only (Bā=ā1.446, tā=ā3.389, pā=ā0.002). Maternal prenatal NASF explained 23.2% of the variance in sleep for males, and only 1% in females. The effect for males remained significant after controlling for gestational age at scan, gestational age and weight at birth, maternal age, and maternal postnatal anxiety at age 3Ā years. See Fig.Ā 1 for a graphical presentation of the interaction. Regarding the separate stress dimensions, we only observed a similar interactions for perceived stress. We did not observe a sex interaction for maternal satisfaction with life, worry, anxiety, or depression symptoms during pregnancy.
Graphical representation of sex interaction. Sex interaction between maternal prenatal stress and child sleep. Made with excel macro (www.jeremydawson.co.uk/slopes.htm).
Additionally, we investigated whether objective measures of child sleep were also associated with maternal prenatal NASF in a small follow-up subsample of children who wore an Actigraph wristband for a week at 5Ā years of age. We found a strong, negative correlation between the maternal prenatal NASF and the ratio between mean circadian and ultradian power (rā=āāā0.408, pā=ā0.043), indicating that objective sleep measuresāspecifically relative contributions of daily to more frequent behavior structureāalso correlated with the maternal prenatal stress factor. However, mean circadian power and mean ultradian power did not yield significant results (rā=āāā0.019, pā=ā0.928; rā=āāā0.178, pā=ā0.395, respectively). Follow-up analyses with separate analyses for different dimensions of maternal stress during pregnancy showed a similar pattern of associations, with a strong association between maternal prenatal depression and the circadian/ultradian power ratio (rā=āāā0.418, pā=ā0.047). Again, no significant association was found between circadian or ultradian power and maternal prenatal depression. To explore why the ratio of circadian/ultradian power is associated with maternal prenatal stress but not the separate measures (circadian and ultradian power), we explored out data further. We found that the standard error of childrenās circadian power increased with increasing maternal prenatal stress, especially for maternal prenatal depression (rā=ā0.612, pā=ā0.002). This association seems to indicate that higher maternal stress/depression during pregnancy increased the variability of the childrenās circadian power.
Maternal psychological stress and fetal cortico-cerebellar connectivity
In line with our hypothesis, maternal psychological stress/negative affect during pregnancy was associated with variation in fetal cortico-cerebellar connectivity. First, we performed an intersection analyses, combining the association of the three hubs with maternal prenatal NASF. We observed a robust effect of decreased cerebellar to left insula functional connectivity (See Fig.Ā 2). Correlations for separate hubs were computed (Hub1: rā=āāā0.366, pā=ā0.003; Hub2: rā=āāā0.332, pā=ā0.007; Hub3: rā=āāā0.381, pā=ā0.002) and indicated that the association existed for both left (Hub 2 and 3) and right (Hub 1) inferior cerebellum. The effects remained significant after controlling for frame count, movement during scanning, sex of the fetus, gestational age at scanning, maternal age, and maternal income. No significant sex interactions were found for the association between maternal prenatal NASF and cerebellar-insular connectivity (all pā>ā0.05). āSupplemental Materialsā include reporting of Family Wise Error (FWE) correction for significant insula effects for each hub.
Cerebellar-insular connectivity in relation to maternal prenatal stress and negative affect. Cerebellar-insular connectivity (left insula) was related to maternal psychological stress during pregnancy, presented on a fetal template at 32Ā weeks of gestational age. (A) Shows the brain results of first three images. The top three images show the results for the individual cerebellar hubs (from left to right: Hubs 1, 2, 3; at kā=ā15, pā<ā0.01), while the lower image shows the combined effect of all three hubs (intersection), revealing a robust effect. (B) Shows a scatterplot of the results for the three hubs separately. Created using xjView for SPM 12 in Matlab R2019a. NASF Negative Affect and Stress Factor, FC Functional connectivity.
Following-up analysis of linkage between cerebellar-insular connectivity and individual factors used to derive the omnibus NASF score showed that maternal prenatal perceived stress was most reliably related to cerebellar-insular connectivity (across hubs) and uniquely correlated with hub 1. Correlations between the individual prenatal stress dimensions and fetal cerebellar-insular connectivity are presented in Supplemental Table S2.
Fetal cerebellar-insular connectivity and toddlersā sleep problems
Building on the results of decreased left insula to cerebellar fetal functional connectivity in highly stressed mothers, we tested whether cerebellar-insular function connectivity was also associated with child sleep problems. We did not proceed with performing a mediation analyses due to lack of significant associations between fetal cerebellar-insula FC and maternal-reported toddler sleep problems (Hub1: rā=āāā0.179, pā=ā0.156; Hub2: rā=āāā0.075, pā=ā0.557; Hub3: rā=āāā0.196, pā=ā0.121), nor with the actigraphy data. Lack of association suggests there is not likely to be an indirect effect of cerebellar-insular functional connectivity on the association between maternal stress during pregnancy and toddler sleep problems.
Sensitivity analyses
First of all, sex differences in main predictor and outcome variables were explored by using independent-samples t-tests. We did not find any sex differences for maternal prenatal NASF scores (t(62)ā=ā0.221, pā=ā0.826) or mother-reported sleep problems (t(62)ā=āāā0.716, pā=ā0.477). Additionally, brain quality measures were not associated with our main variables, maternal prenatal NASF (motion: rā=āāā0.124, pā=ā0.331; frame count: rā=āāā0.032, pā=ā0.799) and child sleep (motion: rā=ā0.188, pā=ā0.138; frame count: rā=āāā0.072, pā=ā0.574). To ensure prematurity is not affecting our results, we reran our analyses excluding all preterm infants with a gestational age ā¤ā37Ā weeks (instead of ā¤ā32Ā weeks). This procedure excluded an additional seven infants (total Nā=ā57). The sex interaction effect for stress and sleep remained significant (Bā=āāā1.320, tā=āāā2.058, pā=ā0.046) and follow-up testing still revealed that the association between maternal NASF during pregnancy and child sleep problems was significant for males only (Bā=ā1.240, tā=ā2.069, pā=ā0.049). The effect for males remained significant after controlling for gestational age at scan, gestational age and weight at birth, maternal age, and maternal postnatal anxiety at age 3Ā years. Furthermore, correlations between maternal NASF and fetal cortico-cerebellar connectivity in the left insula still yielded similar significant correlations (for all three hubs), while the correlation between child sleep problems and fetal cortico-cerebellar connectivity remained non-significant. Taken together, all sensitivity analyses resulted in similar results, indicating robustness of our findings.
Discussion
This study presents investigation of the role of fetal cortico-cerebellar functional connectivity (FC) in the association between maternal prenatal negative affect and stress and toddler sleep problems. We found that high maternal negative affect and stress during pregnancy was associated with increased sleep problems in toddlerhood. When exploring sex differences, we found a significant sex interaction indicating that the association was largely driven by males. Additionally, prenatal exposure to maternal negative affect and stress was associated with decreased cerebellar-insula FC in the fetus. The predictive value of fetal cerebellar-insula FC, however, was absent, indicating that fetal cerebellar FC did not mediate the association between maternal negative affect and stress during pregnancy and later child sleep problems. Still, our results present in utero alterations of fetal functional connectivity that are associated with maternal prenatal stress exposure.
Our finding of increased sleep disturbance in toddlers prenatally exposed to maternal stress and negative affect replicates previous reports13,14. We found that maternal report of increased sleep problems was replicated in the subsample with direct observation through actigraphy. Our results on mother-reported sleep suggested that males are more at risk of developing sleep problems in response to prenatal exposure to maternal stress and mood later in life. Almost 25% of the variance in maleās mother-reported sleep problems was explained by maternal stress and negative affect during pregnancy. Previous research has reported males as being more sensitive to prenatal influences46 and some authors argue that males are more inclined to develop learning difficulties and cognitive concerns after prenatal exposure to maternal stress, while emotional disturbances are more prevalent in prenatally exposed females45,47. How these sex-specific differences are related to sleep, is an important question for future research. Moreover, the underlying mechanisms that confer differences between the sexes are largely unknown. Recent research points to several potential pathways, including sex differences in HPA-axis functioning, brain development, and sex-specific adaptation of the placenta48,49.
Regarding our objective sleep measurement, we found that children prenatally exposed to higher maternal negative affectivity and stress showed a lower ratio between mean circadian and ultradian power, as measured with actigraphy. Ultradian rhythmsārhythms occurring faster than a day, including sleep cyclesāhave recently been shown to carry important information about sleep and physiology (for a review, see50). Here we found that neither the strength of circadian nor ultradian rhythms alone correlated with early life stress. Our explorative analyses pointed out that childrenās variation in circadian power increased when mothers were more stressed during pregnancy. This may have caused the finding of a significant ratio between circadian and ultradian power, in the absence of an association for either one alone. Importantly, it has recently been found that ultradian rhythms by circadian rhythms are indicative of health51,52,53, indicating that the ratio between circadian and ultradian power is of particular interest. Consistent with this observation, we found that higher maternal prenatal stress correlated with a lower ratio between circadian and ultradian power. In this cohort, circadian power and ultradian power correlate, though circadian power rises faster than ultradian power over the population. As a result, this ratio largely reflects a combined power of the two featuresāhigher ultradian power is more likely to occur in individuals with higher circadian power, and the presence of strength in both suggests healthier biological rhythms, which we find is negatively correlated with stress. In other words, children prenatally exposed to higher levels of maternal prenatal stress and mood showed less āhealthyā biological rhythms indicative of disturbed sleep cycles. It is important to note that the data were not separated into a training and testing set, as the pool of data was too small for such machine-learning and confirmation approaches. Therefore, while this result matches our expectations about the importance of robust biological rhythms for healthy sleep, further studies are necessary to explore these interactions in a larger population.
In line with previous research (for a review see20), our results show altered functional connectivity in the offspring of prenatally stress women. Our results of fetal functional connectivity add important information about timing of this effect by showing that alterations are already observable in utero. Additionally, few studies have explored cortico-cerebellar functional connectivity even though previous researcher have called the cerebellum a āprime target for pediatric neuroimagingā41. The cerebellum has a unique protracted developmental trajectories, sexual dimorphism, preferential susceptibility to environmental influences, and its implications in childhood onset disorders such as ADHD and autism, making it a very suitable candidate for studying fetal origins of neurodevelopmental disorders. We found decreased left cerebellar-insula FC in fetuses of mothers experiencing higher negative affectivity and stress during pregnancy. In the adult brain, the insula has positive functional connections with the cerebellum54 and altered connectivity of the insula (to other cortical areas) has been related, among other functions, to emotional awareness55, saliency56, and pain perception57. Additionally, altered structural and functional properties of the insula are often reported in maltreated children and adolescents58,59,60. More importantly, thinner insula gray matter volume was found in children prenatally exposed to maternal depression61. Future studies should investigate what the implications of decreased fetal functional connectivity of the cerebellum-insula network are for later child behavioral outcomes.
In addition to examining maternal stress and negative affect during pregnancy as one factor score, we also examined the effect of the separate measures of stress. In general, we observed similar pattern of effects, but strength of individual associations varied. Perceived maternal stress during pregnancy and worry showed strongest effects, anxiety and depression showed comparatively lower effects and satisfaction with life showed little or no relationship with child sleep and fetal cerebellar functional connectivity. These findings demonstrate potential loss of detail that can result from collapsing across measures and corroborate prior evidence that varied measures of maternal prenatal stress may differently relate to child (brain) outcomes62. Maternal perceived stress and worries may exert different physical and hormonal responses in the pregnant women than depressive symptoms, leading to different effects on the developing fetusāa hypothesis that warrants further investigation in future research.
While we did find an association between fetal cerebellar functional connectivity and maternal stress during pregnancy, there was no indirect effect via cerebellar functional connectivity for the association between maternal prenatal stress and child sleep problems (no mediation). This indicates that the underlying mechanisms of the association are likely not related to the cerebellum-insula connection. One possibility is that other parts of the fetal brain have an important role in mediating sleep, instead of the cerebellum and the insula. Other areas that have been associated with prenatal exposure to maternal stress are the amygdala, hippocampus, and parietal and prefrontal areas20,21. Alternatively, maternal prenatal stress may affect child sleep problems via multiple pathways. For example, O'Connor et al.13 point to the potential role of glucocorticoids in the association between mother-reported prenatal stress and child sleep problems. Their findings suggest glucocorticoids as a major influence given several reports of a link between the establishment of a diurnal pattern in cortisol and sleeping through the night in infancy. Nevertheless, many researchers have failed to find associations between maternal cortisol and mother-reported stress during pregnancy and/or child outcomes63,64. Clearly, more research is necessary to uncover the underlying mechanisms of sleep problems in children prenatally exposed to maternal stress. Such explorations will ideally include alternative brain pathways and also biochemical measures.
Few limitations of the study should be considered. Firstly, the exploration of functional connectivity of the fetal brain in the current study was restricted to the cortico-cerebellar connectivity. We present an initial investigation in to the complex associations between maternal prenatal stress and fetal functional connectome development. Future research should extent to other brain areas, such as the fetal amygdala, hippocampus, and prefrontal cortex. Second, our main analyses was focused on mother-reported sleep problems of their child. Research has shown that current maternal mood can affect ratings of childrenās behavioral problems65. It is promising that we observed significant associations between maternal prenatal stress and child sleep problems in both subjective and objective data; however, replication of the association between prenatal stress and biological sleep measures in larger samples remains an important aim for future work. Third, it is difficult to disentangle contributions of the postnatal environment to child sleep problems. It is known that prenatal stress may affect maternal parenting66, which, in turn, can contribute to child sleep problems. However, the fact that maternal prenatal stress was found to impact functional connectivity of the fetal brain suggests that biological mechanisms are, at least, partly at play. This is corroborated by child sleep behavior measured using actigraphy, as actigraphy captures aspects of physiology, and these data also seemed to implicate biological programming.
In sum, our study has established additional evidence for the notion that child sleep problems may have fetal origins. We found evidence for in utero alterations of fetal functional connectivity in response to prenatal exposure to maternal stress in a sample of predominantly AfricanāAmerican, high-risk mothers and children. More specifically, we found decreased left cerebellar-insula functional connectivity in fetuses of mothers experiencing higher negative affect and stress during pregnancy. Additionally, maternal prenatal negative affect and stress was associated with increased sleep problems in toddlerhood in males. However, we did not observe an indirect effect via altered functional connectivity of the fetal cerebellum to the neocortex for this association. Future research should continue the search for underlying mechanisms and extend the exploration to other brain areas and mediating factors. This information holds the potential to inform innovative interventions to facilitate healthy brain development in utero and stave off deleterious health outcomes related to sleep problems in early childhood, such as increased child emotional and behavior problems and physical health concerns.
Materials and methods
Participants
Healthy pregnant women from the Metro Detroit, USA area were recruited in their second or third trimester to take part in a longitudinal fetal magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) brain study between weeks 18 and 36 of pregnancy. Inclusion criteria for participation were: maternal age >ā18Ā years, English as first language, singleton pregnancy, and typical fetal brain development (as assessed by ultrasound and anatomical MRI examination). Some cases completed more than one prenatal MRI exam. For these cases only the later GA scan was included in the current analysis.
After birth, mothers were invited to take part in the longitudinal research protocol, which included a baby research visit, a toddler visit, a preschool visit, and three remote phone assessments. At the time of the current analysis, 118 mothers met eligibility by having completed the toddler follow-up visit. Subsequent exclusions were made for cases with incomplete or missing Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) data (Nā=ā41) or maternal prenatal stress questionnaires (Nā=ā1), resulting in 76 cases with complete data. Next, cases with extremely low birthweight and/or prematurity (GAā<ā32Ā weeks and/or), birthweightā<ā1500Ā g; Nā=ā10) were excluded. Finally, two additional children were excluded due to medical complications and/or neurological diagnoses (i.e., cerebral palsy, Autism Spectrum Disorder). A flowchart of our sample selection can be found in the āSupplemental Materialsā, see Supplemental Fig. S1.
The final sample consisted of 64 motherāchild dyads (39 male). Fetal age at MRI ranged from 20.6 to 39.6Ā weeks gestational age (GA; range 32.3ā39.6, SDā=ā4.3) and were born at a mean GA of 38.4 (range 34.0ā41.7, SDā=ā1.8). Motherās age at birth was 24.7Ā years on average (range 18.3ā34.5, SDā=ā4.4). The majority of the mothers included in the study were AfricanāAmerican, which is representative of the geographic region of the Detroit area. Additionally, our sample is a high-stress, low-resource sample, which is reflected in the depression rates of our participating mothers. Clinical levels of depression (cutoffāā„ā16, for perinatal depression67) were reported in 20 out of 64 participating mothers (31.7%). All participating mothers provided written informed consent. Additionally, mothers also provided informed consent for their children to participate in the study. More detailed information of the characteristics of our study sample are included in Table 1.
Study procedure
During the MRI visit, mothers completed self-report questionnaires and reported demographic information. At age 3Ā years (meanā=ā35.8Ā months; SDā=ā1.3), mothers and their children were invited back to the laboratory for a developmental follow-up visit. During the visit, mothers completed self-report questionnaires, this time also on their childrenās behavior. Children participated in the developmental assessment tasks and engaged in interaction tasks with their mother. Additionally, actigraphy measures of sleep were obtained in a subset of study children that reached age 5Ā years at the time of this analysis (meanā=ā59.2Ā months; SDā=ā4.8). An Actigraph GT3Xā+āWristband (www.actigraphcorp.com) was worn over the course of contiguous week and weekend days for an average of 6.84Ā days (SDā=ā2.10). The wristband was placed by a research assistant during a home visit, worn on the non-dominant arm, and it was explained to the child and the mother that the wristband should not be taken off, except for swimming, bathing, and showering. The study has been approved by the Ethical Review Board of Wayne State University, Detroit, MI, and was conducted in full compliance with the Helsinki Declaration.
Questionnaires and materials
Maternal negative affect and stress factor (NASF)
Maternal stress during pregnancy was measured using five scales that assessed internalizing problems and sources of stress at the time of scanning: Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D)68, the State Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI)69, the Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ)70, the Perceived Stress Scale (PSST)71, and the Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS)72. Selected instruments were intended to comprehensively assess negative affectivity and stress in participating pregnant women. To reduce the number of variables tested and to extract the overlapping variance of these constructs, we performed explorative factor analysis (EFA) on half of the available sample. Our analyses was performed on all women enrolled in the prenatal MRI scanning protocol (Nā=ā221), including those that did not complete our follow-up protocol. The full sample matches the included sample in terms of social and ethnical background. Moreover, no mean difference was detected between our sample (Nā=ā64) and the rest of the full sample (t (217)ā=ā1.643, pā=ā0.102) on the NASF scores.
EFA established that these factors loaded best onto one factor (CFIā=ā0.98, TLIā=ā0.96, RMSEAā=ā0.08, SRMRā=ā0.03; all factor loadings, pā<ā0.001). Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) in the other random half of the sample derived a single factor of maternal negative affect and stress exposure, termed the āNegative Affect and Stress Factorā (NASF). The results indicated that the five scales showed medium to high factor loadings (i.e., correlations) and good fit to a one-factor model (nā=ā99, CFIā=ā0.98, TLIā=ā0.97, RMSEAā=ā0.06, SRMRā=ā0.03; factor loadings, pā<ā0.001). A graphical representation of the factor score, including factor loadings, is presented in Supplemental Fig. S2. For more information on construction of the maternal NASF, see āSupplemental Materialsā. The analysis was performed in Mplus vs. 7.273 statistical software. We use the term āNASFā and āmaternal prenatal stress factorā interchangeably.
Child sleep problems
To measure child sleep problems, mothers completed the 7-item sleep subscale of the Child Behavioral Checklist (CBCL)74. The CBCL sleep problems scale is a widely used and well-validated scale in toddlers75, which assesses the most commonly reported sleep complaints presented in pediatric sleep clinics and primary care practices. Specifically, the scale includes the following items: resists going to bed at night; does not want to sleep alone; has trouble getting to sleep; wakes up often at night; sleeps less than most children; nightmares; talks or cries out in sleep. Items are rated using a 3-point scale (0ā=ānot true, 1ā=āsomewhat or sometimes true, 2ā=āvery true or often true). The Sleep Problems scale demonstrated adequate internal reliability in the present sample (αā=ā0.74).
Functional brain data acquisition and preprocessing
Fetal brain resting-state fMRI data were collected using a Siemens Verio 70-cm open-bore 3-T and a Siemens Flex Abdominal Coil. Images were collected with the following parameters: echo planar imaging (EPI) BOLD, TR/TE 2000/30Ā ms, axial 4-mm slice thickness, estimated specific absorption rate (SAR)ā=ā0.2 (SDā=ā0.1); repeated twice. The voxel size was 3.4āĆā3.4āĆā4Ā mm3. Between 12 and 24Ā min of fetal resting-state fMRI data were collected per participant.
Resting state fMRI preprocessing followed methods previously described76,77,78,79. In brief, periods of fetal quiescence were manually identified using FSL image viewer80, in which individual segments must consist of at least 20Ā s (ten frames) of low motion. To ensure that the resulting fetal motion did not contribute to our study outcomes, we checked whether the resulting fetal motion was associated with maternal NASF and/or child sleep outcomes (see below). Brainsuite81 was used to manually draw 3D masks for a reference frame from each period of fetal movement quiescence. Masks were then binarized and applied only to frames corresponding to their select segment, and only those data were retained for further analyses. Data quality criteria for fMRI in these analyses required cases have at least 80 volumes of low motion data. For these cases, approximately 50% of data were discarded due to high fetal motion; after motion censoring, an average of 162 frames (Mā=ā5.40, SDā=ā1.87Ā min) were retained. As a point of reference, prior fetal fMRI studies have reported censoring 39ā56% of total number of acquired fMRI frames26,78,82,83.
Subsequent within-segment preprocessing steps included reorientation, realignment and normalization to an average 32Ā weeks gestational age fetal anatomical template84 using Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM8) software implemented in Matlab (http://icatb.sourceforge.net). To correct for variation in normalization across segments, within-participant normalized images were then concatenated, realigned to the mean BOLD volume, and smoothed using a 4Ā mm FWHM Gaussian kernel across runs. Further preprocessing was performed in CONN functional connectivity toolbox (v14n)85 in MATLAB including linear detrending, nuisance regression using aCompCor of five principal components extracted from a 32-week fetal atlas white matter and CSF mask, six head motion parameters, and band-pass filtering at 0.008ā0.09Ā Hz.
Seed-based functional connectivity analyses
Functional connectivity analyses were carried out with the CONN toolbox for MATLAB (http://www.nitrc.org/projects/conn)85. To examine the effect of maternal prenatal stress on fetal functional connectivity of the cerebellum with the cortex, we conducted a seed-to-voxel analysis with the three cerebellar hubs as seed regions. Following prior work, we seeded three hub regions of the fetal functional connectome, identified by graph theoretical analysis26. All three hubs are located in the inferior (posterior) part of the cerebellum, which is the largest part of the cerebellum and known to be highly connected with cerebral regions, including parietal and prefrontal cortices41. One hub is located in the right hemisphere of the cerebellum, while the other two hubs are located in the left hemisphere. Temporal associations between the mean time courses of all voxels in the seeds with the rest of the brain were estimated using regression analyses. See Table 2 for details on cerebellar hub locations and Fig.Ā 3 for an anatomical representation of the cerebellar hub locations on a fetal template. Note that our previous work on hubs in the human fetal brain network 26 also yielded hubs in other areas of the fetal brain, not located in the cerebellum, such as the inferior temporal gyrus, precentral gyrus, angular gyrus, medial temporal lobe, and the primary visual cortex. Here, however, our focus is on the cerebellar hubs.
Actigraphy data analyses
Actigraphy data were analyzed in Matlab 2018a using custom processing routines. Data were transformed using wavelets as previously described51,86,87. Briefly, wavelets are functions with frequency and location, here, Morse wavelet, that are compared to the data to generate information about frequency composition and stability across the recording window. They are similar to Fourier transforms, but are sensitive to changes in frequency composition across time (non-stationary signals). Daily (circadian) frequencies were isolated as a band between 23 and 25Ā h periodicity, and ultradian frequencies were isolated as a band between 1 and 3Ā h periodicities. Features were generated from each individualsā band characteristics, including: mean circadian power (average strength of the circadian band across the entire window), mean ultradian power (average strength of within-a-day ultradian band across the entire window), and the ratio of the mean circadian/ultradian power. As activity becomes more consolidated to the day time, and decreases in the night time, circadian power increase as a reflection of this dichotomy; in this way circadian power serves as a combined statistical proxy for both sleep consolidation and daily activity. Ultradian characteristics serve as a proxy for biological rhythms associated with hormonal dynamics and sleep cycles, whereas the ratio of the two serves to identify individuals with alterations in one but not both of these measures.
Statistical approach
To address our research questions, we tested whether maternal NASF was associated with mother-reported child sleep problems at age 3Ā years by performing a hierarchical regression analysis. The first model included child sleep values as dependent variable and the maternal prenatal stress factor as predictor. We then added an interaction effect of child sex and child sleep problem score (sex*sleep) into the next model and controlled the model for the following confounders in the final model: gestational age at scan, gestational age at birth, birth weight, maternal age, and maternal anxiety at age 3Ā years postnatal (concurrent maternal anxiety symptoms). Controlling for maternal postnatal anxiety was done to make sure the child sleep problems are not biased by maternal concurrent anxiety symptoms. In addition to our analyses on the mother-reported sleep scores, we explored whether associations existed between maternal NASF and objective measures of child sleep, as measured with actigraphy. To this aim, we performed Pearsonās correlations in the subsample with actigraphy sleep data. Actigraphy measures of interest were mean circadian power (average strength of the circadian band across the entire window), mean ultradian power (average strength of within-a-day ultradian band across the entire window), and the ratio of the mean circadian/ultradian power.
We then investigated the effect of maternal prenatal stress on fetal cortico-cerebellar functional connectivity. Second-level analyses were performed in SPM12 by subjecting the connectivity maps into a regression model with the maternal prenatal NASF scores as the predictor, including gestational age at scan as a covariate. Resultant cerebellar connectivity maps were set to a pā<ā0.05 (uncorrected) threshold and k-minā=ā15 and binarized in order to derive a single binary map representing common effects across cerebellar connectivity models (intersection of three sets Aāā©āBāā©āC). Intersection of the three models at this threshold yielded a single region of significance in the left insula that became the focus of all subsequent statistical modelling. Functional connectivity values were then extracted from three peaks within the insula conjunction map, for each fetus, in order to test associations with potential confounds, and to assess whether altered fetal functional connectivity mediated the association between maternal stress during pregnancy and child sleep problems. A total of three models were tested in SPSS version 22 (IBM), one for each cerebellar hub. Potential confounds tested in each model included frame count, mean translational and rotational movement, sex of the fetus, gestational age at scanning, maternal age, and maternal income. Models also included an exploratory sex interaction term. While tests of mediation were planned, these were not pursued due to lack of observed significant association between child sleep problems and fetal cerebellar connectivity.
Recent research has shown that different dimensions of maternal distress during pregnancy (anxiety, depression, perceived stress) may have both shared and unique effects on the developing brain in utero62. We therefore conducted follow-up analyses for all our tests including the NASF factor, exploring the unique effects of the five individual scales contributing to the omnibus NASF factor score. Results of these follow-up analyses are reported in the āSupplemental Resultā section (see āSupplemental Materialsā).
Finally, several sensitivity analyses were conducted, to test the robustness of our findings. First of all, we tested whether there were any sex differences in our main predictors with two sampled t-tests. Next, we checked whether motion differences were confounding observed effects. Fetal movement effects are kept to a minimum by only including frames of low motion. However, some motion may remain. In order to check confounding motion effects, we computed Pearsonās correlations between brain quality measures (average XYZ mean and the frame count) and our outcome variables (prenatal NASF and child sleep problems).
References
Hawkins, S. S. & Takeuchi, D. T. Social determinants of inadequate sleep in US children and adolescents. Public Health 138, 119ā126. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.puhe.2016.03.036 (2016).
Fricke-Oerkermann, L. et al. Prevalence and course of sleep problems in childhood. Sleep 30, 1371ā1377 (2007).
McDowall, P. S., Campbell, A. C. & Elder, D. E. Parent knowledge of child sleep: A pilot study in a childrenās hospital cohort. Sleep Med. 21, 57ā62. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2016.01.014 (2016).
Spilsbury, J. C. et al. Sleep behavior in an urban US sample of school-aged children. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 158, 988ā994 (2004).
Laposky, A. D., Van Cauter, E. & Diez-Roux, A. V. Reducing health disparities: The role of sleep deficiency and sleep disorders. Sleep Med. 18, 3ā6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2015.01.007 (2016).
Anderson, S. E., Andridge, R. & Whitaker, R. C. Bedtime in preschool-aged children and risk for adolescent obesity. J. Pediatr. 176, 17ā22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2016.06.005 (2016).
Goldstein, A. N. & Walker, M. P. The role of sleep in emotional brain function. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 10, 679ā708. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032813-153716 (2014).
Troxel, W. M., Trentacosta, C. J., Forbes, E. E. & Campbell, S. B. Negative emotionality moderates associations among attachment, toddler sleep, and later problem behaviors. J. Family Psychol. JFP J. Div Family Psychol. Am. Psychol. Assoc. (Div. 43) 27, 127ā136. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031149 (2013).
van den Heuvel, M. I., Henrichs, J., Donkers, F. C. L. & Van den Bergh, B. R. H. Children prenatally exposed to maternal anxiety devote more attentional resources to neutral pictures. Develop. Sci. 21, e12612. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12612 (2018).
van den Heuvel, M. I., Donkers, F. C., Winkler, I., Otte, R. A. & Van den Bergh, B. R. Maternal mindfulness and anxiety during pregnancy affect infantsā neural responses to sounds. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 10, 453ā460. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsu075 (2015).
Frankenhuis, W. E. & Del Giudice, M. When do adaptive developmental mechanisms yield maladaptive outcomes?. Dev. Psychol. 48, 628ā642. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025629 (2012).
Glover, V. Annual research review: Prenatal stress and the origins of psychopathology: An evolutionary perspective. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 52, 356ā367 (2011).
OāConnor, T. G. et al. Prenatal mood disturbance predicts sleep problems in infancy and toddlerhood. Early Hum. Dev. 83, 451ā458. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2006.08.006 (2007).
Toffol, E. et al. Maternal depressive symptoms during and after pregnancy are associated with poorer sleep quantity and quality and sleep disorders in 3.5-year-old offspring. Sleep Med. 56, 201ā210. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2018.10.042 (2019).
Palagini, L., Drake, C. L., Gehrman, P., Meerlo, P. & Riemann, D. Early-life origin of adult insomnia: Does prenatalāearly-life stress play a role?. Sleep Med. 16, 446ā456. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2014.10.013 (2015).
Groome, L. J., Swiber, M. J., Bentz, L. S., Holland, S. B. & Atterbury, J. L. Maternal anxiety during pregnancy: Effect on fetal behavior at 38 to 40 weeks of gestation. J. Develop. Behav. Pediatr. 16, 391ā396 (1995).
Scheinost, D. et al. Does prenatal stress alter the developing connectome?. Pediatr. Res. 81, 214ā226. https://doi.org/10.1038/pr.2016.197 (2017).
Van den Bergh, B. R. H. et al. Prenatal developmental origins of behavior and mental health: The influence of maternal stress in pregnancy. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.07.003 (2017).
Monk, C., Lugo-Candelas, C. & Trumpff, C. Prenatal developmental origins of future psychopathology: Mechanisms and pathways. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050718-095539 (2019).
van den Bergh, B. R. H., Dahnke, R. & Mennes, M. Prenatal stress and the developing brain: Risks for neurodevelopmental disorders. Develop. Psychopathol. 30, 743ā762. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579418000342 (2018).
Adamson, B., Letourneau, N. & Lebel, C. Prenatal maternal anxiety and childrenās brain structure and function: A systematic review of neuroimaging studies. J. Affect. Disord. 241, 117ā126. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2018.08.029 (2018).
van den Heuvel, M. I. & Thomason, M. E. Functional connectivity of the human brain in utero. Trends Cognit. Sci. 20, 931ā939. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.10.001 (2016).
Thomason, M. E. et al. Prenatal neural origins of infant motor development: Associations between fetal brain and infant motor development. Dev. Psychopathol. 30, 763ā772. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457941800072X (2018).
DelRosso, L. M. & Hoque, R. The cerebellum and sleep. Neurol. Clin. 32, 893ā900. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ncl.2014.07.003 (2014).
Canto, C. B., Onuki, Y., Bruinsma, B., van der Werf, Y. D. & De Zeeuw, C. I. The sleeping cerebellum. Trends Neurosci. 40, 309ā323. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2017.03.001 (2017).
van den Heuvel, M. I. et al. Hubs in the human fetal brain network. Develop. Cognit. Neurosci. 30, 108ā115. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2018.02.001 (2018).
Bell-McGinty, S. et al. Identification and differential vulnerability of a neural network in sleep deprivation. Cereb. Cortex 14, 496ā502. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhh011 (2004).
Li, C. et al. Abnormal whole-brain functional connectivity in patients with primary insomnia. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 13, 427ā435. https://doi.org/10.2147/NDT.S128811 (2017).
Macey, P. M. et al. Brain morphology associated with obstructive sleep apnea. Am. J. Respir. Crit. Care Med. 166, 1382ā1387. https://doi.org/10.1164/rccm.200201-050OC (2002).
Harper, R. M., Kumar, R., Ogren, J. A. & Macey, P. M. Sleep-disordered breathing: Effects on brain structure and function. Respir. Physiol. Neurobiol. 188, 383ā391. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.resp.2013.04.021 (2013).
Sadeh, A., Raviv, A. & Gruber, R. Sleep patterns and sleep disruptions in school-age children. Dev. Psychol. 36, 291ā301. https://doi.org/10.1037//0012-1649.36.3.291 (2000).
Lewandowski, A. S., Toliver-Sokol, M. & Palermo, T. M. Evidence-based review of subjective pediatric sleep measures. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 36, 780ā793. https://doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jsq119 (2011).
Stores, G. Childrenās sleep disorders: Modern approaches, developmental effects, and children at special risk. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 41, 568ā573. https://doi.org/10.1017/s001216229900119x (1999).
Bernier, A., Beauchamp, M. H., Bouvette-Turcot, A. A., Carlson, S. M. & Carrier, J. Sleep and cognition in preschool years: Specific links to executive functioning. Child Dev. 84, 1542ā1553. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12063 (2013).
Sadeh, A. Sleep assessment methods. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 80, 33ā48. https://doi.org/10.1111/mono.12143 (2015).
Meltzer, L. J., Montgomery-Downs, H. E., Insana, S. P. & Walsh, C. M. Use of actigraphy for assessment in pediatric sleep research. Sleep Med. Rev. 16, 463ā475. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2011.10.002 (2012).
Sadeh, A. The role and validity of actigraphy in sleep medicine: An update. Sleep Med. Rev. 15, 259ā267. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2010.10.001 (2011).
Fair, D. A. The big reveal: Precision mapping shines a gigantic floodlight on the cerebellum. Neuron 100, 773ā776. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2018.11.013 (2018).
Matthews, L. G. et al. Brain growth in the NICU: Critical periods of tissue-specific expansion. Pediatr. Res. 83, 976ā981. https://doi.org/10.1038/pr.2018.4 (2018).
Herzmann, C. S. et al. Cerebellar functional connectivity in term- and very preterm-born infants. Cereb. Cortex https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhy023 (2018).
Tiemeier, H. et al. Cerebellum development during childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal morphometric MRI study. NeuroImage 49, 63ā70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.016 (2010).
Volpe, J. J. Cerebellum of the premature infant: Rapidly developing, vulnerable, clinically important. J. Child Neurol. 24, 1085ā1104. https://doi.org/10.1177/0883073809338067 (2009).
Bauer, P. M., Hanson, J. L., Pierson, R. K., Davidson, R. J. & Pollak, S. D. Cerebellar volume and cognitive functioning in children who experienced early deprivation. Biol. Psychiat. 66, 1100ā1106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.06.014 (2009).
Bale, T. L. & Epperson, C. N. Sex differences and stress across the lifespan. Nat. Neurosci. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4112 (2015).
Weinstock, M. Gender differences in the effects of prenatal stress on brain development and behaviour. Neurochem. Res. 32, 1730ā1740. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11064-007-9339-4 (2007).
Weinstock, M. Sex-dependent changes induced by prenatal stress in cortical and hippocampal morphology and behaviour in rats: An update. Stress 14, 604ā613. https://doi.org/10.3109/10253890.2011.588294 (2011).
Wang, X. et al. Gender-specific effect of pregnancy-related anxiety on preschoolerās emotional and behavioral development: A population-based cohort study. J. Affect. Disord. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2020.10.014 (2020).
Hicks, L. M., Swales, D. A., Garcia, S. E., Driver, C. & Davis, E. P. Does prenatal maternal distress contribute to sex differences in child psychopathology?. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 21, 7. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-019-0992-5 (2019).
Clifton, V. L. Review: Sex and the human placenta: mediating differential strategies of fetal growth and survival. Placenta 31(Suppl), S33-39. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.placenta.2009.11.010 (2010).
Grant, A. D., Wilsterman, K., Smarr, B. L. & Kriegsfeld, L. J. Evidence for a coupled oscillator model of endocrine ultradian rhythms. J. Biol. Rhythms 33, 475ā496. https://doi.org/10.1177/0748730418791423 (2018).
Smarr, B. L., Grant, A. D., Zucker, I., Prendergast, B. J. & Kriegsfeld, L. J. Sex differences in variability across timescales in BALB/c mice. Biol. Sex Diff. 8, 7ā7. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13293-016-0125-3 (2017).
Smarr, B. et al. Circadian dysfunction in the Q175 model of Huntingtonās disease: Network analysis. J. Neurosci. Res. 97, 1606ā1623. https://doi.org/10.1002/jnr.24505 (2019).
Smarr, B. L., Zucker, I. & Kriegsfeld, L. J. Detection of successful and unsuccessful pregnancies in mice within hours of pairing through frequency analysis of high temporal resolution core body temperature data. PLoS ONE 11, e0160127. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0160127 (2016).
Cauda, F. et al. Functional connectivity of the insula in the resting brain. NeuroImage 55, 8ā23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.049 (2011).
Simmons, W. K. et al. Keeping the body in mind: Insula functional organization and functional connectivity integrate interoceptive, exteroceptive, and emotional awareness. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 2944ā2958. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22113 (2013).
Menon, V. & Uddin, L. Q. Saliency, switching, attention and control: A network model of insula function. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 655ā667. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0 (2010).
Segerdahl, A. R., Mezue, M., Okell, T. W., Farrar, J. T. & Tracey, I. The dorsal posterior insula subserves a fundamental role in human pain. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 499. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3969 (2015).
Hein, T. C. & Monk, C. S. Neural response to threat in children, adolescents, and adults after child maltreatmentāA quantitative meta-analysis. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 58, 222ā230. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12651 (2017).
McLaughlin, K. A., Peverill, M., Gold, A. L., Alves, S. & Sheridan, M. A. Child maltreatment and neural systems underlying emotion regulation. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 54, 753ā762. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2015.06.010 (2015).
Dannlowski, U. et al. Limbic scars: Long-term consequences of childhood maltreatment revealed by functional and structural magnetic resonance imaging. Biol. Psychiat. 71, 286ā293. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.10.021 (2012).
Sandman, C. A., Buss, C., Head, K. & Davis, E. P. Fetal exposure to maternal depressive symptoms is associated with cortical thickness in late childhood. Biol. Psychiat. 77, 324ā334. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.06.025 (2015).
Scheinost, D., Spann, M. N., McDonough, L., Peterson, B. S. & Monk, C. Associations between different dimensions of prenatal distress, neonatal hippocampal connectivity, and infant memory. Neuropsychopharmacology https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-020-0677-0 (2020).
Zijlmans, M. A., Riksen-Walraven, J. M. & de Weerth, C. Associations between maternal prenatal cortisol concentrations and child outcomes: A systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 53, 1ā24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.02.015 (2015).
van den Heuvel, M. I., van Assen, M. A. L. M., Glover, V., Claes, S. & Van den Bergh, B. R. H. Associations between maternal psychological distress and salivary cortisol during pregnancy: A mixed-models approach. Psychoneuroendocrinology 96, 52ā60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.06.005 (2018).
Maoz, H. et al. The effects of parental mood on reports of their childrenās psychopathology. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2014.07.005 (2014).
Henrichs, J., van den Heuvel, M. I., Witteveen, A. B., Wilschut, J. & Van den Bergh, B. R. H. Does mindful parenting mediate the association between maternal anxiety during pregnancy and child behavioral/emotional problems?. Mindfulness https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-019-01115-9 (2019).
Orr, S. T., Blazer, D. G., James, S. A. & Reiter, J. P. Depressive symptoms and indicators of maternal health status during pregnancy. J. Womenās Health 16, 535ā542. https://doi.org/10.1089/jwh.2006.0116 (2007).
Radloff, L. S. The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl. Psychol. Meas. 1, 385ā401 (1977).
Spielberger, C. D. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory: A Comprehensive Bibliography (Consulting Psychologists Press, Palo Alto, 1984).
Meyer, T. J., Miller, M. L., Metzger, R. L. & Borkovec, T. D. Development and validation of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire. Behav. Res. Ther. 28, 487ā495. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(90)90135-6 (1990).
Cohen, S., Kamarck, T. & Mermelstein, R. A global measure of perceived stress. J. Health Soc. Behav. 24, 385ā396 (1983).
Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J. & Griffin, S. The satisfaction with life scale. J. Pers. Assess. 49, 71ā75. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13 (1985).
MuthĆ©n, L. K. & MuthĆ©n, B. O. Mplus Userās Guide. 7th edn (MuthĆ©n & MuthĆ©n, Los Angeles, CA, 1998ā2012).
Achenbach, T. M. & Rescorla, L. A. Manual for the ASEBA School-Age Forms & Profiles: An Integrated System of Mult-informant Assessment (University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth & Families, Burlington, 2001).
Koot, H. M., Van Den Oord, E. J. C. G., Verhulst, F. C. & Boomsma, D. I. Behavioral and emotional problems in young preschoolers: Cross-cultural testing of the validity of the child behavior checklist/2-3. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 25, 183ā196. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025791814893 (1997).
Thomason, M. E. et al. Cross-hemispheric functional connectivity in the human fetal brain. Sci. Transl. Med. 5, 173ra124 (2013).
Thomason, M. E. et al. Intrinsic functional brain architecture derived from graph theoretical analysis in the human fetus. PLoS ONE 9, e94423. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0094423 (2014).
Thomason, M. E. et al. Weak functional connectivity in the human fetal brain prior to preterm birth. Sci. Rep. 7, 39286. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep39286 (2017).
van Heuvel, M. I. et al. Hubs in the human fetal brain network. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 30, 108ā115. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2018.02.001 (2018).
Jenkinson, M., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens, T. E., Woolrich, M. W. & Smith, S. M. FSL. NeuroImage 62, 782ā790 (2012).
Shattuck, D. W. & Leahy, R. M. BrainSuite: An automated cortical surface identification tool. Med. Image Anal. 6, 129ā142 (2002).
Thomason, M. E. et al. Age-related increases in long-range connectivity in fetal functional neural connectivity networks in utero. Develop. Cognit. Neurosci. 11, 96ā104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2014.09.001 (2015).
Thomason, M. E. et al. Intrinsic functional brain architecture derived from graph theoretical analysis in the human fetus. PLoS ONE 9, 10. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0094423 (2014).
Serag, A. et al. Construction of a consistent high-definition spatio-temporal atlas of the developing brain using adaptive kernel regression. Neuroimage 59, 2255ā2265. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.062 (2012).
Whitfield-Gabrieli, S. & Nieto-Castanon, A. Conn: A functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks. Brain Connect. 2, 125ā141. https://doi.org/10.1089/brain.2012.0073 (2012).
Palesh, O. et al. Acupuncture to improve circadian health in breast cancer survivors (BCS): An RCT. J. Clin. Oncol. 34, 10066ā10066. https://doi.org/10.1200/JCO.2016.34.15_suppl.10066 (2016).
Leise, T. L. Wavelet analysis of circadian and ultradian behavioral rhythms. J. Circadian Rhythms 11, 5ā5. https://doi.org/10.1186/1740-3391-11-5 (2013).
Acknowledgements
This project was supported by awards to M.E.T. from the National Institutes of Health, MH110793, ES026022, and ES020957, andĀ MH122447. MvdH is supported by the Dutch Research Council (NWO), Veni.VI.191G.025. The authors thank Toni Lewis, Amanpreet Bhogal, Sarah Wilhoit, Abdulmohsen Ghuloum, Rosemary Joseph, Houda Ajrouche, Zainab Altarjoman, Mohamed Hussain Mussa, and Joshua Hammond for their assistance in data acquisition and analyses. Special thanks to Dr. Rebecca Waller for construction of the stress factor score used in this study. The authors also thank participant families who generously shared their time.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
MvdH. wrote the initial draft of the manuscript, B.L.S., L.J.K., J.B., and M.E.T. contributed to writing. J.L.H. preprocessed and analyzed brain data, and B.L.S. preprocessed the actigraphy data. MvdH. performed the statistical analyses of the data, B.L.S. contributed to the analyses. MvdH, J.L.H., B.L.S., L.J.K., J.B., and M.E.T. all reviewed the results and contributed to interpretation. Recruitment of participants was conducted by T.Q. and MvdH., J.L.H., T.Q., and K.E.H. contributed to data collection. M.E.T. and MvdH. conceived the original idea and M.E.T. supervised the project. All authors reviewed the final draft of the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
van den Heuvel, M.I., Hect, J.L., Smarr, B.L. et al. Maternal stress during pregnancy alters fetal cortico-cerebellar connectivity in utero and increases child sleep problems after birth. Sci Rep 11, 2228 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-81681-y
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-81681-y
This article is cited by
-
The sleeping brainās connectivity and family environment: characterizing sleep EEG coherence in an infant cohort
Scientific Reports (2023)
-
Maternal and early life exposures and their potential to influence development of the microbiome
Genome Medicine (2022)
-
Maternal psychological distress during the COVID-19 pandemic and structural changes of the human fetal brain
Communications Medicine (2022)
-
Association of maternal prenatal depression and anxiety with toddler sleep: the China-Anhui Birth Cohort study
Archives of Women's Mental Health (2022)
-
Interactive relations between maternal prenatal stress, fetal brain connectivity, and gestational age at delivery
Neuropsychopharmacology (2021)
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.