Increased large-scale inter-network connectivity in relation to impulsivity in Parkinson’s disease

Abstract

Impulsivity is a neuropsychiatric feature of Parkinson’s disease (PD). We investigated the pathophysiology of impulsivity in PD using resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI). We investigated 45 patients with idiopathic PD and 21 healthy controls. Based on Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11) score, PD patients were classified as higher (PD-HI) or lower impulsivity (PD-LI). Functional connectivity (FC) between various large-scale brain networks were analysed using the CONN toolbox. FC between the right frontoparietal network (FPN) and medial visual network (MVN) was significantly higher in PD-HI patients than PD-LI patients (false discovery rate [FDR]-adjusted p = 0.0315). FC between the right FPN and MVN had a significant positive correlation with total BIS-11 score (FDR-adjusted p = 0.010) and the attentional impulsivity (FDR-adjusted p = 0.046) and non-planning impulsivity subscale scores (FDR-adjusted p = 0.018). On the other hand, motor impulsivity subscale score had a significant negative correlation with the FC between the default-mode and salience networks (right supramarginal gyrus, FDR-adjusted p = 0.018; anterior cingulate cortex, FDR-adjusted p = 0.027); this trend was observed in healthy controls. The attentional and non-planning impulsivity, regarded as ‘cognitive’ impulsivity, may be associated with dysfunction in integration of perceptual information and flexible cognitive control in PD.

Introduction

The estimated number of individuals with Parkinson’s disease (PD) is increasing, with 6.1 million individuals affected by PD in 20161. Patients with PD suffer from various non-motor symptoms, among which impulse control disorders (ICDs) are a common neuropsychiatric symptom. ICDs and related disorders include pathological gambling, hypersexuality, binge eating, compulsive shopping, punding, hobbyism and dopamine dysregulation syndrome. ICDs can cause unproductive, harmful and even illegal activity2. ICD risk factors include male gender, cigarette smoking history, younger age of disease onset, history of drug or alcohol abuse, novelty-seeking personality and higher dose or long-term use of dopamine agonists2,3,4. Previous brain imaging studies have consistently suggested that meso-cortico-limbic-striatal circuit dysfunction is associated with ICDs5,6,7. In addition, a hyper-dopaminergic state within the meso-limbic-cortical pathway has been found in PD patients with ICDs7,8. Furthermore, some studies have suggested that dopaminergic abnormalities may be neurobiological markers of vulnerability to the development of ICDs9,10,11,12. The association of DAT1 and OPRM1 with ICDs in PD suggests a genetic predisposition may play a role as well13.

Recently, trait impulsivity without apparent ICD in PD patients has been proposed in several studies14,15,16,17. High impulsivity (HI) is a precursor of ICDs and can also lead to severe ICDs18,19. While HI may be within the spectrum of ICDs, the underlying pathophysiological mechanism differs between trait impulsivity and ICDs20. For instance, ICD has been associated with increased connectivity between the left subthalamic nucleus and the left parietal operculum, whereas trait impulsivity has been associated with a weak connectivity between the left putamen and the right inferior temporal gyrus21. Other studies investigating cortical thickness, tractography, or functional MRI have also reported inconsistent results5,6,7,19,20,21,22. One of the causes is that impulsivity contains multifaceted constructs.

To define simply, impulsivity is the tendency to act prematurely without foresight. However, impulsivity is multifaceted like most other behaviours and involves various forms of response inhibition, reward evaluation, motivation and cognitive control23. To assess the multifaceted constructs of impulsivity, various methods have been developed, including Barratt Impulsiveness Scale 11th version (BIS-11), Stop Signal Reaction Time (SSRT), 5-choice serial reaction time task, Hayling test, go/no-go task and Iowa gambling task. Among these, BIS-11, which is the most commonly used self-reported questionnaire, can measure subdomains of attentional (Iat), motor (Im) and non-planning (Inp) impulsiveness. Among these, Iat and Inp subscales are considered ‘cognitive’ impulsivity, whereas motor impulsivity is the tendency to ‘act without thinking’. It has been pointed out that the HI of PD is caused by different pathophysiologies of motor impulsivity and cognitive impulsivity24, but few studies have investigated them individually.

To elucidate the pathophysiological background of impulsivity in PD, in vivo brain function should be evaluated because abnormal function does not necessarily coincide with pathological changes. Although there are many limitations to the in vivo assessment of human brain, remarkable progress has been made in resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI), which has become more popular than conventional event-related fMRI. Metabolic changes in brain due to stimulation are < 5%, and almost all forms of brain dysfunction can be detected at a resting state25,26. Furthermore, rs-fMRI can reduce noise compared to event-related fMRI, particularly in elderly patients with PD characterised by bradykinesia or bradyphrenia. rs-fMRI is capable of creating highly reproducible maps of large-scale neural networks in the human brain27,28. Changes within these networks have been observed in many neuropsychiatric diseases29. Although several studies investigated impulsivity in PD using rs-fMRI, increased connectivity within the salience network (SN) and default-mode network (DMN) and decreased connectivity within the central executive network were associated with ICDs in PD30. On the other hand, altered frontoparietal network (FPN), which plays an important role in instantiating and flexibly modulating cognitive control31,32, has been suggested in the pathophysiology of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder33. Other studies on impulsive/compulsive disorders such as addiction and binge eating have also revealed alterations in large-scale networks34,35. Thus, changes in large-scale networks are a suggested cause of impulsivity.

Although basal ganglia is important in the regulation of impulsivity as well as ICDs, it is difficult to accurately separate the blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signals of basal ganglia subregions, such as the core and shell of the nucleus accumbens, due to the limited spatial resolution of fMRI. Furthermore, the low-frequency oscillation of BOLD signals has been shown to be slower in basal ganglia than in cerebral cortex36. Therefore, functional connectivity (FC) between basal ganglia and cortex can be underestimated during comparison with inter-cortex connectivity. On the other hand, subcortical regions regulate large-scale networks. For instance, the SN is affected by striatal disruption and SN dysfunction impacts the modulation of DMN37. Thus, disorders affecting subcortical regions are reflected in major large-scale networks. Therefore, we hypothesized that measuring the FC of large-scale networks of the cerebral cortex is an effective approach for evaluating complex psycho-cognitive functions such as impulsivity.

To test our hypothesis that the impulsivity in PD is related to alterations in large-scale networks such as the DMN, SN and FPN, we examined FC changes in various large-scale networks between PD patients grouped according to impulsivity using fully automated whole-brain FC analysis. For association with the basal ganglia, the FC of nucleus accumbens with closely related brain regions was measured separately based on the abovementioned issues. The present study, albeit exploratory, provides evidence that the change in the FC of large-scale networks is associated with the pathophysiology of impulsivity in PD.

Results

Patient characteristics

After enrolment, during the two-year follow-up period, the PD diagnosis of three patients changed: one to progressive supranuclear palsy, one to multiple system atrophy and one to psychogenic tremor; all three were excluded from analysis. Another patient was excluded because of new onset myoclonic epilepsy. We also excluded 20 patients with a mini-mental state examination (MMSE) score < 27 and three patients with < 250 valid imaging scans, for the accurate detection of FC abnormalities related to impulsivity in PD. Finally, 45 PD patients and 21 healthy controls (HCs) were included for analysis (Table 1).

Table 1 Patient characteristics.

There were no significant differences between the PD patients and HCs with respect to age, gender and education. The median BIS-11 score was 60 in the PD patients; those with BIS-11 score ≥ 60 were classified as PD-HI. The mean BIS-11 score in the PD-HI group was similar to that reported in PD patients with ICDs4. Median BIS-11 score was 57 in the HCs. There was no significant difference in Im between the PD-HI and HC groups; however, there were significant differences in other BIS-11 total and subscale scores between the PD-HI group and the other groups. The total BIS-11 score and each subscale score did not significantly differ between the PD-LI patients and HCs.

Although not significant, PD-HI patients tended to be slightly younger than PD-LI patients, and the total levodopa-equivalent daily dose (LEDD) tended to be lower in PD-HI patients. MMSE score, frontal assessment battery (FAB) score, Stroop test and Movement Disorder Society-Sponsored Revision of the Unified Parkinson’s Disease-Rating Scale (MDS-UPDRS) part 3 and part 4 scores were not significantly different. MDS-UPDRS part 1 and part 2 scores were significantly higher in the PD-HI patients. The part 1A subscale scores did not significantly differ between the PD-HI and PD-LI patients, including #1.2 hallucination/psychosis (p = 0.18) and #1.6 dopamine dysregulation syndrome (p = 0.19). Multiple regression analysis did not show any significant association between BIS-11 score and other clinical parameters.

Whole-brain functional connectivity analysis of large-scale networks

FC was significantly higher between the right lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFC) of the FPN and the medial visual network (MVN) in PD-HI patients than PD-LI patients (false discovery rate [FDR]-adjusted p = 0.0315, T = 3.55). Figure 1 illustrates the results of the correlation analyses between FC and the total BIS-11 and subscale scores. The total BIS-11 score and Iat and Inp subscale scores in PD patients showed a significant positive correlation with FC between the right LPFC and MVN. This finding persisted even when the MDS-UPDRS part 3 score was included as a nuisance covariate. Only the Im subscale score showed a different tendency: higher score was related to decreased FC between the DMN (medial prefrontal cortex) and SN (supramarginal gyrus and anterior cingulate cortex). Regarding the HCs, the FC between large-scale networks was not significantly correlated with the BIS-11 score or the subscale scores. However, the FC between DMN and right supramarginal gyrus, which was significantly correlated with the Im score in patients with PD, tended to be negatively correlated with the Im score in HCs (Fig. 1).

Figure 1
figure1

Comparison between BIS-11 score and inter-network functional connectivity (FC). Bivariate correlation analysis shows significant positive correlation between FC of FPN-MVN and total BIS-11 and Iat and Inp subscale scores. Higher Im subscale score is related to decreased FC between the DMN and salience networks. Regarding healthy controls, the FC between DMN and right SMG of SN tended to be negatively correlated with Im subscale score. rLPFC right lateral prefrontal cortex, FPN frontoparietal network, MVN medial visual network, ACC anterior cingulate cortex, rSMG right supramarginal gyrus, DMN default-mode network.

Functional connectivity analysis of basal ganglia

There were no significant differences in the FC between basal ganglia and large-scale networks between the PD-HI and PD-LI groups. There were also no significant differences in the FC between nucleus accumbens and other closely related regions among the three groups (Table 2).

Table 2 Functional connectivity between nucleus accumbens and other regions.

Voxel-based morphometry results

Grey matter (GM) volume in the left inferior temporal cortex was significantly different among the three groups (Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) xyz coordinates: − 54, − 12, − 29); the volume was significantly lower in the PD-HI patients compared to HCs. Multiple regression analysis detected no significant correlation between GM volume in the left inferior temporal cortex and BIS-11 score in patients with PD.

Discussion

In the present study, we showed FC differences among large-scale networks in relation to trait impulsivity of PD. While the BIS-11 total score and the Iat and Inp subscale scores were related to increased FC between the right FPN and MVN in PD patients, the Im subscale score was related to decreased FC between the DMN and SN. Since Iat and Inp are regarded as ‘cognitive’ impulsivity24, this study could successfully detect the difference in the pathophysiology between cognitive and motor impulsivity in PD. Although no study to date reported the association between ‘cognitive’ impulsivity and right FPN, the FPN plays an important role in instantiating and flexibly modulating cognitive control25,26. Therefore, our findings suggest that abnormal cognitive control due to dysfunction of FPN is related to ‘cognitive’ impulsivity in PD.

Several studies investigated changes in the FC of DMN and SN related to impulsivity38,39. In particular, a decrease in the FC of DMN has been identified in pathological gambling40, and the association between motor impulsivity and pathological gambling has been demonstrated in a meta-analysis41. Thus, the abnormalities in FC between the DMN and SN in relation to motor impulsivity were reproducible in the present study. Further, a similar tendency in the FC between the DMN and right supramarginal gyrus of SN was observed in HCs. Therefore, this finding may reflect the characteristics of individual ‘model-based’ learning rather than PD-specific changes.

The involvement of the MVN in HI remains unclear. Ide et al. also unexpectedly found that increased GM volume in the bilateral medial parietal and occipital cortices in healthy adults was associated with total BIS-11 score and Iat and Inp subscale scores42. Although they could not specify the pathophysiology, they speculated that vulnerability to distraction by visual stimuli was involved. The ventral pathway of visual information is integrated in the inferior temporal gyrus with top down control from the prefrontal cortex. Therefore, our results, albeit not conclusive, may reflect dysfunction in the integration of visual information. Alternatively, the amount of visual information might be superior to other perceptual information or the variance in BOLD signals might have been higher in the occipital lobe than in the other regions43. On the other hand, visual network alterations are also associated with visuospatial and visual hallucinations in PD44. In the present study, few patients experienced visual hallucinations, and there was no significant difference in the hallucination score between the PD-HI and PD-LI groups. We could not exclude the potential contribution of slight visuospatial impairment because we did not conduct optimal visuospatial assessment; however, none of the patients had difficulty in drawing pentagons during the MMSE. In addition, a relatively strict cut-off MMSE score of > 27 was adopted to minimise the effects of slight visuospatial cognitive impairment.

The association of impulsivity with cognitive dysfunction in PD remains controversial. Although some reports have suggested an association between frontal lobe dysfunction and impulsivity45, others have suggested that impulsivity is independent of the cognitive state46,47. The multifaceted nature of impulsivity may explain these contradictory results. In the present study, MMSE, FAB and Stroop test did not differ between the PD-HI and PD-LI groups, which may be due to the relatively stringent criteria for MCI exclusion and the small number of cases.

Previous reports clearly show that basal ganglia play an important role in regulating impulsivity. However, the present study could not detect any association between the impulsivity and FC of basal ganglia. This negative finding may be explained by false-negative data associated with the abovementioned limitations of rs-fMRI or may indicate that the inter-cortical connectivity may be more important than basal ganglia for impulsivity in PD in the absence of ICDs. However, it is unlikely that basal ganglia are not involved in impulsivity control at all. Therefore, these findings should be considered to be associated with changes in the regulation of cortex via basal ganglia. In other words, it is highly possible that the failure to detect a direct FC abnormality in basal ganglia is a false-negative finding because of the methodology and that the FC abnormality between the cortices is regulated by basal ganglia. Future studies by other methodologies are necessary to further explore this possibility.

Although we found left inferior temporal atrophy in HI patients, consistent with one previous study5, another study did not48. Although this might be a false-positive finding, our rs-fMRI results were not influenced by inferior temporal atrophy, because the inferior temporal lobe is not included in the large-scale network regions of interest of the CONN toolbox.

In addition to these caveats, the present study has several limitations. First, MRI examinations were performed with the patient in the ‘on’ state while taking their usual medication to avoid the burden of dopamine discontinuation and reduce motion artefact, as previously reported6,49. Therefore, we cannot exclude the direct effect of dopamine replacement therapy (DRT) on FC. However, it is not feasible to eliminate the effects of DRT on FC. Although levodopa modifies FC in the short-term[50], there is no evidence of long-term influence. Thus, we attribute the current findings to changes of FC under usual conditions, considering the effect of DRT. Second, we used BIS-11 as the sole measure of impulsivity and did not include other evaluation methods such as SSRT. In the present study, we focused on examining rs-fMRI in a relatively large number of cases and excluded other methods due to feasibility concerns. The QUIP-RS, an ICD questionnaire for PD that is also used to assess compulsivity in PD51, has not yet been validated in Japanese. A more accurate understanding of the pathophysiology underlying impulsivity requires comparison of BIS-11 with other methodologies.

In conclusion, we have demonstrated that ‘cognitive’ impulsivity in PD is associated with increased large-scale inter-network FC changes between the right FPN and MVN. This new finding suggests that ‘cognitive’ impulsivity in PD may be associated with dysfunction of flexible cognitive control via the FPN. Although the involvement of the MVN is unclear, the integration of visual information might be involved. Our analyses also suggest an association between motor impulsivity and altered FC in the DMN and SN in both PD patients and HCs. Further studies are necessary to elucidate the pathophysiology of impulsivity in PD.

Methods

Participants

We recruited 72 consecutive PD patients and 21 HCs. All patients with PD fulfilled the clinical diagnostic criteria of the United Kingdom Parkinson’s Disease Society Brain Bank. Patients with apparent dementia and those receiving any cholinesterase inhibitor or antipsychotic medication were excluded from initial enrolment. The HCs were either partners of patients with PD or volunteers without PD as assessed by MDS-UPDRS part 3. We also excluded participants with ICDs based on assessment by a semi-structured interview as previously reported52: we used the diagnostic statistical manual of mental disorders IV-TR criteria for pathological gambling and binge eating53 and the currently proposed research criteria for hypersexuality, excessive buying54,55, punding, hobbyism and dopamine dysregulation syndrome[55,56,57], diagnosed by a board-certified neurologist (J. K.).

This study was conducted according to the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Wakayama Medical University Ethics Committee. All participants provided written informed consent.

Assessment of impulsivity and clinical symptoms

To assess impulsivity, we applied the Japanese version of the BIS-11[58], which is the most commonly used instrument designed to assess impulsivity59. It has also been used to assess impulsivity in patients with PD14,15. The BIS-11 contains three subscales: Iat, Im and Inp. Each patient with PD was classified as PD-HI or PD-LI, based on the median BIS-11 score (PD-HI, BIS-11 score ≥ 60; PD-LI, BIS-11 score < 60). For motor and neuropsychiatric assessment of patients with PD, we used the MDS-UPDRS, MMSE, FAB and Stroop test during the ‘on’ state, assessed by board-certified neurologists. The LEDD was calculated as previously reported60.

3 MRI acquisition

A 3-T MRI scanner with a 32-channel head coil was used to scan each participant. To minimise motion artefact, the patients underwent MRI during the ‘on’ state while taking their usual medications, as in previous studies6,7,61. The following parameters were used to acquire T1-weighted structural images: TR = 7 ms, TE = 3.3 ms, field of view (FOV) = 220 mm, matrix scan = 256, slice thickness = 0.9 mm and flip angle = 10°. The following parameters were used for functional data using a gradient-echo echo-planar pulse sequence sensitive to BOLD contrast: TR = 3000 ms, TE = 30 ms, FOV = 192 mm, matrix scan = 64, slice thickness = 3 mm, number of slices 38 and flip angle = 80°, which covered the entire cerebral GM and cerebellum. Three runs, each with 105 volumes, were performed for each participant. In total, data were acquired over approximately 15 min during a resting state for each participant, as this duration was deemed the most appropriate to obtain reliable data26,62. The participants were asked to remain awake but keep their eyes closed during the acquisition.

Functional MRI data preprocessing

We used Statistical Parametric Mapping software, version 12 (SPM12) and CONN toolbox, version 17 in Matlab R2014a for MRI data preprocessing. The CONN toolbox is designed to preprocess both functional and anatomical files, including realignment, slice-timing correction, outlier identification, co-registration, segmentation, normalisation and smoothing63. Preprocessing of functional data was performed by the default preprocessing pipeline. The initial five scans were removed to eliminate equilibration effects. Images were realigned and unwarped. Slice-timing was corrected in ascending order. For functional outlier detection, we used intermediate settings (97th percentile in normative sample), by adaptive resonance theory implemented in CONN. With a series of three runs of functional imaging acquired, we analysed a total of 300 functional images per subject. For the accuracy of the analysis, we included only subjects who had more than 250 valid scans in the final analysis. Both functional data and structural data were segmented to GM, white matter (WM) and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) and normalised to MNI coordinates. Functional images were smoothed to 8 mm full width at half maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel. Subsequently, denoising was performed by default, including band-pass filtering (0.008–0.09 Hz) and linear regression.

Whole-brain functional connectivity analysis of large-scale networks

For the first-level analysis between regions of interest (ROIs), we used the 32 ROIs of large-scale networks from the CONN toolbox as defined by its independent component analyses of the Human Connectome Project dataset (497 subjects): default mode (4 ROIs), sensorimotor (3 ROIs), visual (4 ROIs), salience/cingulo-opercular (7 ROIs), dorsal attention (4 ROIs), frontoparietal/central executive (4 ROIs), language (4 ROIs) and cerebellar (2 ROIs). For the second-level analysis, the two-sample t-test was applied to evaluate the difference between PD-HI and PD-LI patients (nuisance covariates: age, gender, LEDD; with and without MDS-UPDRS part 3 scores). Bivariate correlation analysis was performed to evaluate the effect of total BIS-11 and subscale scores. All effects were evaluated using an FDR-adjusted p value of 0.05.

Functional connectivity analysis of basal ganglia

For the first-level analysis of FC of basal ganglia, we used ‘atlas’ ROIs from the CONN toolbox. For the second-level analysis, the two-sample t-test was used to evaluate the difference between PD-HI and PD-LI patients (nuisance covariates: age, gender and LEDD).

Each FC value (Z score of correlation coefficient) between the nucleus accumbens and other regions (such as putamen, amygdala and ACC) was also calculated in PD-HI, PD-LI and HCs.

Structural MRI data analysis

For voxel-based morphometry, we used SPM12 in Matlab. T1-weighted structural images were segmented to GM, WM, CSF and other non-brain portions. Spatial normalisation was performed on GM probability maps using the diffeomorphic anatomical registration with exponentiated Lie algebra algorithm. Normalised imaging was smoothed using 8 mm FWHM Gaussian kernel. Age and gender were included as nuisance covariates. We applied total brain volume for global calculation. Both implicit and explicit masking were used. To evaluate differences between the PD-HI and PD-LI patients and HCs, we performed one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) and the two-sample t-test. Tests were evaluated using family-wise error correction. p < 0.05 was considered significant for ANOVA; p < 0.05/3 was considered significant for the two-sample t-test.

Statistical analyses for patient characteristics

Patient characteristics were analysed using JMP Pro14 software. Normality of data was assessed using the Shapiro–Wilk test. Categorical variables are presented as numerals. Normally distributed continuous variables are presented as means and standard deviation. Non-normally distributed continuous variables are presented as medians and interquartile range. The PD-HI and PD-LI patients and HCs were compared using ANOVA with post-hoc Tukey-HSD test or the Kruskal–Wallis test; two-group comparisons were performed using the unpaired t-test or Wilcoxon rank-sum test. Categorical variables were compared using Fisher’s exact test. Multiple regression analysis was performed to examine association with BIS-11 score. p < 0.05 was considered significant.

Data availability

The datasets analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

References

  1. 1.

    Dorsey, E. R. et al. Global, regional, and national burden of Parkinson’s disease, 1990–2016: a systematic analysis for the global burden of disease study 2016. Lancet Neurol. 17, 939–953 (2018).

    Google Scholar 

  2. 2.

    Weintraub, D. et al. Impulse control disorders in Parkinson disease: a cross-sectional study of 3090 patients. Arch. Neurol. 67, 589–595 (2010).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. 3.

    Corvol, J. C. et al. Longitudinal analysis of impulse control disorders in Parkinson disease. Neurology 91, e189–e201 (2018).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. 4.

    Voon, V. et al. Factors associated with dopaminergic drug-related pathological gambling in Parkinson disease. Arch. Neurol. 64, 212–216 (2007).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. 5.

    Biundo, R. et al. Patterns of cortical thickness associated with impulse control disorders in Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 30, 688–695 (2015).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. 6.

    Carriere, N., Lopes, R., Defebvre, L., Delmaire, C. & Dujardin, K. Impaired corticostriatal connectivity in impulse control disorders in Parkinson disease. Neurology 84, 2116–2123 (2015).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. 7.

    Cilia, R. et al. Pathological gambling in patients with Parkinson’s disease is associated with fronto-striatal disconnection: a path modeling analysis. Mov. Disord. 26, 225–233 (2011).

    ADS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. 8.

    O’Sullivan, S. S. et al. Cue-induced striatal dopamine release in Parkinson’s disease-associated impulsive–compulsive behaviours. Brain 134, 969–978 (2011).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. 9.

    Buckholtz, J. W. et al. Mesolimbic dopamine reward system hypersensitivity in individuals with psychopathic traits. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 419–421 (2010).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. 10.

    Buckholtz, J. W. et al. Dopaminergic network differences in human impulsivity. Science 329, 532 (2010).

    ADS  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. 11.

    Payer, D. E. et al. [11C]-(+)-PHNO PET imaging of dopamine D(2/3) Receptors in Parkinson’s disease with impulse control disorders. Mov. Disord. 30, 160–166 (2015).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. 12.

    Steeves, T. D. et al. Increased striatal dopamine release in Parkinsonian patients with pathological gambling: a [11C] raclopride PET study. Brain 132, 1376–1385 (2009).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. 13.

    Cormier-Dequaire, F. et al. Suggestive association between OPRM1 and impulse control disorders in Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 33, 1878–1886 (2018).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. 14.

    Antonini, A. et al. Impulsivity and compulsivity in drug-naïve patients with Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 26, 464–468 (2011).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. 15.

    Isaias, I. U. et al. The relationship between impulsivity and impulse control disorders in Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 23, 411–415 (2008).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. 16.

    Nombela, C., Rittman, T., Robbins, T. W. & Rowe, J. B. Multiple modes of impulsivity in Parkinson’s disease. PLoS ONE 9, e85747 (2014).

    ADS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. 17.

    Riley, M. et al. Demographic and clinical predictors of trait impulsivity in Parkinson’s disease patients. Parkinsons Dis. 2018, 9472120 (2018).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. 18.

    Marín-Lahoz, J. et al. Parkinson’s disease: impulsivity does not cause impulse control disorders but boosts their severity. Front. Psychiatry 9, 465 (2018).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. 19.

    Hlavatá, P. et al. Behavioral and neuroanatomical account of impulsivity in Parkinson’s disease. Front. Neurol. 10, 1338. https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2019.01338 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. 20.

    Mosley, P. E. et al. The structural connectivity of discrete networks underlies impulsivity and gambling in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 142, 3917–3935. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awz327 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. 21.

    Ruitenberg, M. F. L. et al. Impulsivity in Parkinson’s disease is associated with alterations in affective and sensorimotor striatal networks. Front. Neurol. 9, 279. https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2018.00279 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. 22.

    Kubera, K. M. et al. A search for cortical correlates of trait impulsivity in Parkinson’s disease. Behav. Brain Res. 369, 111911 (2019).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. 23.

    Dalley, J. W., Everitt, B. J. & Robbins, T. W. Impulsivity, compulsivity, and top-down cognitive control. Neuron 69, 680–694 (2011).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. 24.

    Antonelli, F., Ray, N. & Strafella, A. P. Impulsivity and Parkinson’s disease: more than just disinhibition. J. Neurol. Sci. 310, 202–207. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jns.2011.06.006 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. 25.

    Raichle, M. E. & Mintun, M. A. Brain work and brain imaging. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 29, 449–476. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.112819 (2006).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. 26.

    Donishi, T., Terada, M. & Kaneoke, Y. Effects of gender, digit ratio, and menstrual cycle on intrinsic brain functional connectivity: a whole-brain, voxel-wise exploratory study using simultaneous local and global functional connectivity mapping. Brain Behav. 8, e00890. https://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.890 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. 27.

    Heine, L. et al. Resting state networks and consciousness: alterations of multiple resting state network connectivity in physiological, pharmacological, and pathological consciousness states. Front. Psychol. 3, 295 (2012).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. 28.

    Seeley, W. W., Crawford, R. K., Zhou, J., Miller, B. L. & Greicius, M. D. Neurodegenerative diseases target large-scale human brain networks. Neuron 62, 42–52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2009.03.024 (2009).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. 29.

    Menon, V. Large-scale brain networks and psychopathology: a unifying triple network model. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 483–506 (2011).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. 30.

    Tessitore, A. et al. Resting-state brain networks in patients with Parkinson’s disease and impulse control disorders. Cortex 94, 63–72 (2017).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. 31.

    Marek, S. & Dosenbach, N. U. F. The frontoparietal network: function, electrophysiology, and importance of individual precision mapping. Dial. Clin. Neurosci. 20, 133–140 (2018).

    Google Scholar 

  32. 32.

    Zanto, T. P. & Gazzaley, A. Fronto-parietal network: flexible hub of cognitive control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 602–603 (2013).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. 33.

    Lin, H. Y., Tseng, W. Y., Lai, M. C., Matsuo, K. & Gau, S. S. Altered resting-state frontoparietal control network in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 21, 271–284. https://doi.org/10.1017/S135561771500020X (2015).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. 34.

    Hobkirk, A. L., Bell, R. P., Utevsky, A. V., Huettel, S. & Meade, C. S. Reward and executive control network resting-state functional connectivity is associated with impulsivity during reward-based decision making for cocaine users. Drug Alcohol Depend. 194, 32–39. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2018.09.013 (2019).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. 35.

    Oliva, R., Morys, F., Horstmann, A., Castiello, U. & Begliomini, C. Characterizing impulsivity and resting-state functional connectivity in normal-weight binge eaters. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 53, 478–488. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.23212 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. 36.

    Zuo, X. N. et al. The oscillating brain: complex and reliable. Neuroimage 49, 1432–1445 (2010).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. 37.

    Putcha, D., Ross, R. S., Cronin-Golomb, A., Janes, A. C. & Stern, C. E. Salience and default mode network coupling predicts cognition in aging and Parkinson’s disease. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 22, 205–215. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617715000892 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. 38.

    Zhang, R. & Volkow, N. D. Brain default-mode network dysfunction in addiction. Neuroimage 200, 313–331. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.06.036 (2019).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. 39.

    Zilverstand, A., Huang, A. S., Alia-Klein, N. & Goldstein, R. Z. neuroimaging impaired response inhibition and salience attribution in human drug addiction: a systematic review. Neuron 98, 886–903. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2018.03.048 (2018).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. 40.

    Jung, M. H. et al. Decreased connectivity of the default mode network in pathological gambling: a resting state functional MRI study. Neurosci. Lett. 583, 120–125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2014.09.025 (2014).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. 41.

    Chowdhury, N. S., Livesey, E. J., Blaszczynski, A. & Harris, J. A. Pathological gambling and motor impulsivity: a systematic review with meta-analysis. J. Gambl. Stud. 33, 1213–1239. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-017-9683-5 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. 42.

    Ide, J. S., Tung, H. C., Yang, C. T., Tseng, Y. C. & Li, C. R. Barratt impulsivity in healthy adults is associated with higher gray matter concentration in the parietal occipital cortex that represents peripheral visual field. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11, 222 (2017).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. 43.

    Kiviniemi, V., Kantola, J. H., Jauhiainen, J., Hyvärinen, A. & Tervonen, O. Independent component analysis of nondeterministic fMRI signal sources. Neuroimage 19, 253–260 (2003).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. 44.

    Hepp, D. H. et al. Loss of functional connectivity in patients with Parkinson disease and visual hallucinations. Radiology 285, 896–903 (2017).

    ADS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. 45.

    Almeida, B. A. & Hamdan, A. C. Impulsiveness and executive functions in Parkinson’s disease. Dement. Neuropsychol. 13, 410–414 (2019).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. 46.

    Martini, A., Dal Lago, D., Edelstyn, N. M. J., Grange, J. A. & Tamburin, S. Impulse control disorder in Parkinson’s disease: a meta-analysis of cognitive, affective, and motivational correlates. Front. Neurol. 9, 654 (2018).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. 47.

    Jeyadevan, A. et al. Trait impulsivity is independent of mild cognitive impairment in a Parkinson’s disease cohort. Parkinsons Dis. 2019, 2672075 (2019).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. 48.

    Tessitore, A. et al. Cortical thickness changes in patients with Parkinson’s disease and impulse control disorders. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 24, 119–125 (2016).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. 49.

    Hacker, C. D., Perlmutter, J. S., Criswell, S. R., Ances, B. M. & Snyder, A. Z. Resting state functional connectivity of the striatum in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 135, 3699–3711 (2012).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. 50.

    Szewczyk-Krolikowski, K. et al. Functional connectivity in the basal ganglia network differentiates PD patients from controls. Neurology 83, 208–214 (2014).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. 51.

    Stark, A. J. et al. Nigrostriatal and mesolimbic D. J. Neurosci. 38, 3230–3239. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3082-17.2018 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. 52.

    Probst, C. C. et al. Validation of the questionnaire for impulsive–compulsive disorders in Parkinson’s disease (QUIP) and the QUIP-rating scale in a German speaking sample. J. Neurol. 261, 936–942. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00415-014-7299-6 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. 53.

    Grant, J. E., Steinberg, M. A., Kim, S. W., Rounsaville, B. J. & Potenza, M. N. Preliminary validity and reliability testing of a structured clinical interview for pathological gambling. Psychiatry. Res. 128, 79–88. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2004.05.006 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. 54.

    McElroy, S. L., Keck, P. E., Pope, H. G., Smith, J. M. & Strakowski, S. M. Compulsive buying: a report of 20 cases. J. Clin. Psychiatry 55, 242–248 (1994).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. 55.

    Voon, V. & Fox, S. H. Medication-related impulse control and repetitive behaviors in Parkinson disease. Arch. Neurol. 64, 1089–1096. https://doi.org/10.1001/archneur.64.8.1089 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. 56.

    Voon, V. et al. Prevalence of repetitive and reward-seeking behaviors in Parkinson disease. Neurology 67, 1254–1257. https://doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000238503.20816.13 (2006).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. 57.

    Giovannoni, G., O’Sullivan, J. D., Turner, K., Manson, A. J. & Lees, A. J. Hedonistic homeostatic dysregulation in patients with Parkinson’s disease on dopamine replacement therapies. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 68, 423–428. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.68.4.423 (2000).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. 58.

    Someya, T. et al. The Japanese version of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. 11th version 111–114, (BIS-11): its reliability and validity. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 55, 111–114 (2001).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. 59.

    Stanford, M. S. et al. Fifty years of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale: an update and review. Pers. Individ. Differ. 47, 385–395 (2009).

    Google Scholar 

  60. 60.

    Tomlinson, C. L. et al. Systematic review of levodopa dose equivalency reporting in Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 25, 2649–2653 (2010).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. 61.

    Ruitenberg, M. F. L. et al. Impulsivity in Parkinson’s disease is associated with alterations in affective and sensorimotor striatal networks. Front. Neurol. 9, 279 (2018).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  62. 62.

    Birn, R. M. et al. The effect of scan length on the reliability of resting-state fMRI connectivity estimates. Neuroimage 83, 550–558 (2013).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. 63.

    Whitfield-Gabrieli, S. & Nieto-Castanon, A. C. Conn: a functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks. Brain Connect. 2, 125–141 (2012).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was supported in part by a grant-in-aid for scientific research from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science [Grant Number 18H02743] and by a grant from the 2016 Wakayama Medical Award for Young Researchers and by a Takeda Japan Medical Office Funded Research Grant 2018 [Grant Number N95].

Author information

Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

JK, YK, MY, HI, MH, KM, MT and HI designed the study; JK, YK, TD and TI analysed and interpreted the data; JK, MS, YH and YN performed the data collection; JK and YK performed the statistical analyses and wrote the manuscript. All authors read and approved the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jinsoo Koh.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Koh, J., Kaneoke, Y., Donishi, T. et al. Increased large-scale inter-network connectivity in relation to impulsivity in Parkinson’s disease. Sci Rep 10, 11418 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-68266-x

Download citation

Comments

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.