Introduction

The complex interactions between predators and their prey are instrumental in shaping the composition and structure of animal assemblages1. A wide range of theory has been developed to predict the responses of predators to prey, both at the level of the population, and of individuals2,3. Although it is simpler to conceptualize predator–prey systems using mathematical modelling, interactions in nature may behave in ways not captured in models, and therefore empirical studies of such interactions are extremely valuable to tease apart the factors driving assemblage dynamics. Predation risk is, for example, a critical factor influencing the evolution of morphology, ecology, and life-history traits, of both prey and predators alike4,5,6. However, predation risk is notoriously difficult to measure in natural systems, and is typically inferred using indirect measures such as changes in habitat use or foraging behaviour of prey5,7,8 rather than empirical measures of predation risk or direct mortality9,10,11. Direct, empirical measures of predation risk in the field are useful to support the conclusions from such inferences.

Multiple factors can affect predation risk, including the abundance and density of predators. Predation risk is not, however, solely dependent on the abundance, or density of predators, as the abundance of prey populations are also important factors determining predation in multiple-prey systems12,13,14. Predators that change their prey preference, or exhibit prey-switching, consume prey disproportionally less than their availability when prey are rare or at low densities, and disproportionally more than their availability when prey are at high prey densities15. Prey-switching can be beneficial to predators, ensuring maximum food intake per unit effort, and maximizing foraging efficiency16,17,18. In addition, prey-switching can be protective for prey abundance because as a primary prey group becomes depleted, predators switch to alternative prey sources, allowing depleted primary prey populations to recover19. Further, prey-switching promotes prey species diversity, coexistence with predators, and may stabilize wildlife populations20.

Changes in predation risk in response to predators may, thus, be explained by one of two hypotheses: the shared predation hypothesis, which suggests that all prey groups are at high risk when predator populations are large12,21; or the alternative prey hypothesis, which states that predators switch prey groups when primary prey densities drop below the density of alternative prey22,23. Typically, factors influencing predation risk are examined either from the point of view of individual predator populations (i.e., examining functional and numerical responses)24,25 or of prey populations (i.e, focusing on direct or non-consumptive [indirect] behavioural effects of predators on prey)5,7,8. Few studies have directly quantified predation risk in relation to predator abundance or density; instead many assume that more predators lead to higher predation risk (but see26,27,28,29). If the extent of predation risk is not measured directly, but instead only inferred from the abundance of predators, or the behaviour of prey, it is not possible to distinguish which of these factors is more important in driving predation risk. Understanding the relative importance of these processes in determining predation risk is critical to translating the predictions of predator–prey models to explain the outcomes of real population dynamics and community processes.

Few studies incorporate both predator and prey densities and their impacts on direct measures of predation risk (but see30,31,32). We used a mensurative experiment in a model system with changing predator and prey abundances, to directly quantify predation risk and examine the relative role of predator population size and prey availability in determining predation risk. The primary objective was to empirically test two hypotheses regarding predation risk: (1) is predation risk dependent on predator densities, i.e., does predation risk increase with an increase in predator abundance; or (2) is predation risk inversely dependent on the abundance of alternative prey (alternative prey hypothesis)? In this study, we used a multi-level tropical food web with small lizards and invertebrates as prey groups, and avian and large invertebrates (e.g., huntsman spiders) as predators. We used artificial lizard models to estimate predation risk on lizards. We simultaneously quantified the abundance of predators and alternative prey to test if predation risk was proportional to predator abundance or if predation risk to lizards was more closely related to the abundance of alternative prey.

Results

Artificial lizard predation

Predators attacked control models significantly less than lizard-shaped models during the wet season (when both control and artificial lizards were present) (t = −3.394, df = 13, P = 0.004; Table 1). Our top models included invertebrate abundance, season, habitat type, and microhabitat as the best predictors of artificial lizard attack rates (Table 2). There were more attacks on artificial lizards when invertebrate prey abundance was low (Fig. 1b). Artificial lizards were attacked nearly twice as frequently in the dry season compared to the wet season, corresponding to low and high invertebrate prey abundances, respectively. There was no significant effect of microhabitat (models placed on trees vs. ground; P = 0.541) or habitat type (Reid River box vs. Silver-leaf ironbark; P = 0.260; Table 2) on predation risk. Our attack rates on model lizards likely represent a conservative estimate because all models that were stepped on (by cattle or humans; n = 2) or were missing upon collection (lizard shape: n = 41; control shape: n = 43) were removed from analysis because the fate of the model could not be determined. We realize some missing models may have been removed by predators, but without retrieving the artificial lizards for inspection, their fate could not be accurately quantified. We acknowledge that our estimate of predation are therefore conservative, but missing models only accounted for 7% of the total models deployed.

Table 1 Attack frequencies from artificial lizard models.
Table 2 Results from generalized linear mixed-effects models (GLMM) indicating the terms present in the top model as the best predictor for each response variable.
Figure 1
figure 1

Expected (a) and observed (b) results for the alternative prey hypothesis, i.e., predation risk is inversely dependent on the abundance of alternative prey. The observed values for the alternative prey hypothesis (b) indicate that predation risk on lizard models (Attacks; red) was inversely related to the relative mean abundance of invertebrate (alternative) prey (Invert. Prey; blue), and proportional to the relative mean abundance of living lizards (Lizards; green), showing support for the alternative prey hypothesis. The relative means represent the mean ± SE of the responses (attacks on artificial lizards models, invertebrate prey abundance, and Gehyra dubia abundance) as a proportional representation summarized by season to scale all the data from 0–1, and compare the responses.

Predatory birds were responsible for a majority of overall artificial lizard attacks (84.6%), followed by large invertebrates (12.8%), and other (unidentifiable) predators (2.6%). While some of the unidentifiable attacks or missing models could have come from small predatory mammals, this study system has remarkably low numbers of small mammals. Neilly and Schwarzkopf (unpublished data) report low capture rates of small mammals at these sites, with only 39 captures from 20,160 trap nights over 3 years. This trend was similar to other dry savanna woodlands across northeast Queensland33. We suspect that most of the bird attacks on artificial lizards came from large predatory birds abundant at our sites, including grey butcherbirds (Cracticus torquatus), pied butcherbirds (Cracticus nigrogularis), Australian magpies (Cracticus tibicen), blue-winged kookaburras (Dacelo leachii), laughing kookaburra (Dacelo novaeguineae), Torresian crows (Corvus orru), and Australian ravens (Corvus coronoides). We found no significant difference in the rate of attacks on artificial lizards from birds during the day (58%) or night (42%) (t = 0.478, df = 11, P = 0.641), likely because many of these birds are crepuscular foragers. Further, we found no difference in the overall attack rates on artificial lizards during the day (47%) or night (53%) (t = 0.289, df = 13, P = 0.776). Invertebrate predators were likely huntsman spiders (Sparassidae) and centipedes (Scolopendromorpha) based on bite mark indentations left on artificial lizards and the high abundance of these invertebrates throughout the study area. Most of the attacks by invertebrates occurred at night (90%). We did not detect any attacks on artificial lizards from snakes or other reptiles. Predation risk was not correlated to the abundance of either predator group (birds: rho = −0.017, P = 0.949; invertebrates: rho = 0.049, P = 0.854).

Lizard and predator abundance

Our top models (ΔAICc < 2) indicated that invertebrate prey abundance and season were the best predictor of gecko abundance (Table 2). Lizards were more abundant in the dry season than the wet season, a trend opposite to invertebrate prey abundance (Fig. 1b, Table 3). We conducted 13.3 hrs of dawn point-count surveys for diurnal predatory birds, and 13.3 hrs of nocturnal spotlight surveys for nocturnal predators, including large invertebrates, nocturnal birds, and snakes. We identified 14 species of predatory birds, two groups of predatory invertebrates, and one species of snake (Table 4). Magpies, butcherbirds, and corvids made up 71.8% of all the predatory birds detected. Pale-headed snakes (Hoplochephalus bitorquatus) were the only snakes we encountered, but are nocturnal, arboreal, and a likely a predator of small lizards34. Huntsman spiders were the most abundant invertebrate predator and made up 73.7% of the invertebrate predator abundance.

Table 3 Mean (±SE) abundance counts for Gehyra dubia (calculated from a combination of nocturnal spotlighting surveys and captures under artificial cover boards (ACBs)) and invertebrate prey abundance (calculated from under ACB surveys).
Table 4 Total counts of predator groups.

Predatory bird abundance

Our top models for the best predictors of predatory bird abundance included a season x habitat interaction and a non-significant negative relationship with gecko abundance (Table 2). Predatory birds were more abundant in the wet season than the dry season, and in the ironbark than the box habitat (Fig. 2; Table 4).

Figure 2
figure 2

Expected (a) and observed (b) results for the predator density-dependent predation hypothesis, i.e., predation risk increases with an increase in predator abundance (blue line; a). The observed values for the predator density-dependent predation risk (b) indicate that predation risk was not predator density-dependent, as the relative mean number of attacks on lizard models (Attacks; red) and the relative mean abundance of predatory birds (Pred. Birds; blue) show an inverse relationship to each other. The relative means represents a proportional response of attacks on artificial lizard models and predatory bird abundance summarized by season to scale the data from 0–1 for direct comparisons of trends.

Snake and Invertebrate predators

Snake abundances were too low throughout the duration of this study to adequately produce models using snake abundance, although they are likely potential predators (see Appendix S1 in Supporting Information). We only found two snakes, both pale-headed snakes (Hoplocephalus bitorquatus) in box habitat in the wet season. Invertebrates (predominantly huntsman spiders) were responsible for 13% of attacks on artificial lizards. Large predatory invertebrates may be important predators of small vertebrates, such as lizards, even though invertebrates are often overlooked in this capacity35 (see Appendix S1). A season x habitat interaction was the best predictor of predatory invertebrate abundance (Table 2). There was a trend for the ironbark habitat to support higher abundances of predatory invertebrates than the box habitat in both the dry and wet season (see Fig. S1).

Invertebrate prey

Our top models included season and gecko abundance as the best predictors of invertebrate prey abundance (Table 2). Invertebrate prey abundance was highest in the wet season and lowest in the dry season (Fig. 1b) and were significantly negatively related to gecko abundance (P = 0.002).

Discussion

In our study, predation risk on lizards was not proportional to the abundance of predatory birds, lending no support to the hypothesis that predator density drives predation risk. Instead, we found an inverse relationship between predator abundance and predation risk, with higher predation rates at low bird abundances. We found the abundance of alternative prey (invertebrates), was also inversely related to lizard predation; i.e., when the abundance of alternative prey was low, predation risk was greater for lizards, lending strong support to the alternative prey hypothesis. Predation risk on small lizards oscillated over time in opposition to the rise and fall of invertebrate prey populations, consistent with the alternative prey hypothesis, and opposing the predator density-dependence hypothesis. Our study constitutes one of the few empirical studies of the factors driving predation risk, directly, rather than inferring predation risk from indirect measures (such as predator presence or behavioural changes in prey behaviour).

Few studies present data on predator abundances and densities when discussing predator–prey interactions, although predator abundance and density may be major contributing factors for both consumptive and non-consumptive effects on prey populations. Liebezeit and Zack (2008) monitored nests of ground-dwelling birds in the Arctic using remote cameras, and identified potential nest predators from timed point-count surveys. They found more avian predators in the environment (80%), yet Arctic foxes were responsible for over 80% of the predation events captured on remote cameras. Although there were high densities of avian predators, there was a weak association between predation risk and avian predation on nests36. Similarly, high density of predators does not necessarily mean they are responsible for increased predation. For example, DeGregorio et al.37 found high predation of passerine bird nests near powerlines and at habitat edges where raptors were abundant, yet video surveillance of nests indicated that snakes were responsible for the majority of nest predation events. Predator groups can often be misrepresented without confirmation of predation attempts from remote cameras36,37, artificial/clay models (e.g. teeth indentations11,38, or visual confirmation. Without confirmation of predation events, abundant ‘potential’ predators may be erroneously classified as important predators in an ecosystem, inferred directly from their abundance37.

Birds are the most common predators of small herpetofauna in many ecosystems (frogs11,39,40; lizards39,41; snakes38,42). We identified predator groups from indentations on artificial lizards after predation attempts, enabling us to conclude predatory birds were responsible for a majority of attacks on artificial lizards (84.6%). Both grey (Cracticus torquatus) and pied butcherbirds (Cracticus nigrogularis) were common in our study area (21.2% of total bird abundance) and often foraged around and under loose or peeling bark43. Butcherbirds consume G. dubia, which are vulnerable while basking in early morning and late afternoon44, but may also be dislodged from diurnal refugia under loose or peeling bark. We observed predation by butcherbirds as two geckos fitted with radio transmitters were predated and impaled on dead branches during another study44, which is a common behaviour for butcherbirds. Despite having little overlap in activity and foraging time with our nocturnal model species (G. dubia), diurnal birds were a major contributor to the predation on our models, especially at dawn and dusk, when many birds were foraging and lizards theromoregulating.

Many of our predatory bird species were generalists and opportunistic feeders43,45. In regions where food availability fluctuates seasonally, it is advantageous to consume abundant and easy-to-obtain resources. We suspect that birds in our study alter their diet and foraging strategy to take advantage of the most abundant food sources. Our results indicate a strong seasonal shift in the attack rates on lizards. In the dry season, lizard abundances were the highest, as were predation rates, indicating a positive correlation between predation risk and prey abundance.

The presence of alternative prey (invertebrates) helps explain the fluctuation in predation risk on lizards. When examining the trends in an alternative food source (invertebrates) for predatory birds, we found that they followed the opposite pattern from lizard abundance; invertebrates were most abundant in the wet season, and least abundant in the dry season. The pattern in predator and alternative prey abundances can best be explained by the alternative prey hypothesis, in which predation rates are reduced on lizards when the abundance of alternative prey (invertebrates) increases. We suggest predators took advantage of the most abundant prey source, and switched to an alternative prey when lizard prey populations were depleted. Our estimations of predation risk (highest in the dry season and lowest in the wet season) were consistent with this hypothesis. Similarly, other studies have found greater frequency of vertebrates (lizards and frogs) in the diet of birds during the dry season39 and reduced invertebrate abundances during the driest parts of the year46,47,48,49, although none have also examined predation rates.

The community dynamics in our system of small lizards, predatory birds, and invertebrates make for an interesting predator-prey dynamic. Gehyra dubia are an insectivorous lizard, preying on a variety of arthropod prey items, including spiders50, but large predatory huntsman spiders have also been known to be predators of juvenile and even adult lizards and frogs35. Huntsman spiders would be potential prey items for geckos when they are small, competitors when they are equivalent size, and predators when the spiders grow larger than the geckos. One of the major complexities of this system is in how lizards and spiders are both predators, prey, and competitors of each other. High predation rates on invertebrates by geckos or predatory spiders, may indirectly influence prey-switching in birds.

Many bird species exhibit seasonal shifts in prey preference13,51,52. American dippers (Cinclus mexicanus) exhibit a seasonal diet shift with a greater proportion of fish in their diet than aquatic invertebrates prior to egg laying53. A diet of fish has more calories, lipids, and protein, essential for eggshell formation53. The major predatory birds in our study all breed and lay eggs in the dry season54. Predatory birds switched from invertebrate to lizard prey based on their relative abundance, but additionally, we hypothesize birds may switch from primarily invertebrate prey to a larger proportion of vertebrates (lizards) during the dry season to acquire additional nutrients prior to breeding and egg laying, although this should be experimentally tested.

We did not find a difference in the attack rates on artificial lizards in relation to microhabitat, although Steffen (2009) found that artificial lizards in the canopy of Costa Rican tropical forests were attacked more frequently than those on the trunk-ground level. We did not examine height differences on the same scale as Steffen (2009); 10 m = canopy vs. 0.5 m = trunk/ground; whereas our height differences were 0 m = ground vs. 2 m = trunk. A majority of the predatory birds in our study foraged on or near the ground43,45, which may explain why we didn’t find a difference in the attack rates among the microhabitat types. Although, various factors including geographic location, species composition, and foraging behaviors can lead to dubious comparisons between studies.

In summary, predator–prey dynamics are among the more complicated ecological processes studied by ecologists. Understanding the relationships between predators and prey are even more complex in multi-prey systems. We used a multi-level (bird–lizard–invertebrate) system, and a mensurative experiment to test common, but largely untested, assumptions about the relationship between predator abundance and predation risk. We demonstrated that predator density of the primary predator, birds, was not driving predation risk, and found no support for predator density-dependent predation risk. Abundances of both predators and prey varied between seasons, and we found that predation rates on lizards were greatest in the dry season, at the time we observed the lowest abundances of predatory birds. Thus, although predation risk is often thought to be driven by the number of predators, our results indicated that the relative abundances among multiple prey groups may be more important in determining predation. Predation risk fluctuated inversely with the abundance of alternative prey, suggesting that predation risk was closely tied to alternative prey populations. Predation risk varied seasonally, probably as predatory birds shifted their diet in response to the abundance of alternative (invertebrate) prey, and potentially in relation to nutritional requirements prior to egg laying. Our study directly quantified predation risk in the context of predator–prey dynamics, and determined that in this system, a likely source of increased predation was reduced availability of alternative prey groups, not the abundance of predators. It is difficult to experimentally manipulate the relative abundances of predators and of several prey species on a landscape scale, therefore, experimental tests of the factors driving predation risk could use enclosures stocked with varying densities of predators and prey, or attack rates on artificial models to further test our conclusions.

Methods

Study system and site

This study was conducted on at Wambiana Station, a cattle grazing property southwest of Charters Towers, Queensland, Australia (−20.542790, 146.132204, datum = WGS84). The study area is a tropical savanna woodland, containing two major open eucalyptus forest types: Reid River box (Eucalyptus brownii) and Silver-leaf ironbark (Eucalyptus melanophloia). A total of eight sites (1 ha. each) were distributed across both habitat types, four in each. Sites were 1.52 ± 0.12 km (mean ± SE) apart and were not spatially autocorrelated (Mantel permutation test; r = −0.134, P = 0.735). We sampled all sites for one-week in the dry season (August 2015) and the wet season (January 2016). During each one-week sampling period, we quantified lizard, predator, and alternative prey abundances, and placed the artificial lizard models to minimize short-term temporal fluctuations in abundance or prey preference.

In this study, we use an empirical, multi-level food web system (birds, lizards, and invertebrates) and a mensurative experiment (seasonal fluctuations in abundance) to test two competing hypotheses regarding predation risk. We selected a locally abundant lizard, the Australian native house gecko (Gehyra dubia) as our model to measure predation risk in relation to their predators and the abundance of alternative prey (invertebrates). Native house geckos are small arboreal, insectivorous geckos found throughout eastern Australia34,50. While G. dubia are primarily nocturnal, they thermoregulate (bask) in the late evening and early morning sun44, making them susceptible to predation by many crepuscular predators.

Lizard model construction

We constructed life-sized artificial lizard models (hereinafter as “artificial lizards”) to simulate attack rates and predation attempts on lizards. Plasticine and clay models of small vertebrates, especially herpetofauna, have been widely used to assess predation rates across various habitat types41,55, morphological traits such as color pattern11,40,56, and body sizes57,58. Artificial lizards (Fig. 3) were formed with Blu-Tack adhesive putty (Blu-Tack, Bostik Australia Pty Ltd., Thomastown, VIC, Australia) and were shaped by hand to form lizards with similar dimensions (model snout-vent length = 40–60 mm; mass = 4–6 g) as Gehyra dubia (snout-vent length = 47.8 ± 0.65 mm; mass = 3.0 ± 0.10 g; mean ± SE). Prior to shaping artificial lizards, small amounts of graphite powder (Pressol Graphite, Hordern and Company Pty Ltd, Artarmon, NSW, Australia) were added and worked into the Blu-Tack by hand to make models more life-like in coloration. We compared spectral reflectance of artificial lizards and the skin of G. dubia using a spectrophotometer to verify that our models produced similar reflectance as actual lizards (Nordberg and Schwarzkopf, unpublished data). We used a graphite pencil to create darker dorsal patterns commonly found in our population of geckos to make lizard models more realistic in appearance. Once the artificial lizards were formed, we placed each model on a transparent plastic sheet cut into lizard shapes (Lowell Laminating Pouches, Officeworks Ltd, Bentleigh, VIC, Australia) with an exposed tab for attaching it to a substrate. A small 20 mm tack was used to secure the model to different microhabitats (e.g., trunks of trees, or the ground).

Figure 3
figure 3

The native house gecko, Gehyra dubia (a), is an arboreal, nocturnal gecko found throughout northeast Australia. We used Blu-Tack to make physical models of G. dubia for deployment in various macro- and microhabitats to test predation risk in lizards (b,c). Due to its pliable nature, attacks on models can be inferred from indentations remaining after predation events (c; indentations from a bird beak). All photographs taken by Eric Nordberg.

Due to the pliable nature of Blu-Tack, predation attempts left indentations on the artificial lizards (Fig. 3c). We identified attacking predators by inspecting indentations on the models, and classified predators into categories (e.g., bird, reptile, invertebrate, etc.). The indentations from birds left a deep “V” shape in the model, which could be distinguished from large invertebrates which left two fang or pincer punctures. We validated the indentations created by birds from visual observations of attacks on models. We presented artificial lizards to large huntsman spiders (Sparassidae) and coerced them to bite a lizard model to validate the bite mark indentations from large predatory invertebrates.

Lizard model placement

In the dry season (August 2015) we placed 400 artificial lizards among our eight sites. We placed models in two microhabitats used by lizards, on the trunks of trees and on the ground. At each site, 25 artificial lizards were placed on the trunks of trees approximately 2 m from the ground and 25 artificial lizards were placed on the ground on various substrata (e.g. bare ground, in leaf litter, under Carissa ovata bushes, or on woody debris). Artificial lizards were collected after six days and scored as “not attacked” or “attacked”. Models were only scored as “attacked” if they had bite-mark indentations from a potential lethal predator; bite marks from ants and other small invertebrates were not scored as a potentially lethal attack. Attacked artificial lizards were categorized by predator type. In the wet season (January 2016) we implemented the same procedure for model placement but with the addition of 400 control models (patternless Blu-Tack rolled by hand into spheres) to test if predators could distinguish foreign objects from lizard-shaped models. We hypothesized that if control models were attacked at the same rate as artificial lizards, then attack rates were possibly a result of predators inspecting novel objects and may not accurately depict predation. Further, in the wet season, we checked the fate of models twice daily, just after dawn to check for nocturnal attacks, and just before sunset to check for diurnal attacks to test for differences in attack rates from diurnal vs. nocturnal predators.

Invertebrate predator and prey abundance

Invertebrate predator and prey abundances were monitored using arboreal cover boards (ACBs)59. Closed-cell foam cover boards (50 × 50 × 1 cm) were strapped to the main trunks of trees using elastic bungee cords approximately 1.5 m from the ground. Cover boards were used as area-constrained surveys to quantify the relative abundance of invertebrate prey between seasons. All cover boards were removed every day during morning surveys (between 0800–1000 hrs) to quantify the abundance of invertebrate prey but also to survey for sheltering lizards. Cover boards remained on a particular tree for two consecutive days before being moved and replaced on different trees throughout each site. Twenty-four trees were sampled at each site, for a total of 192 trees or 384 trap-nights. During each season, relative invertebrate prey abundances were calculated from area-constrained searches (each ACB represents 0.25 m2 (50 cm × 50 cm) multiplied by 24 trees = 6 m2 of search area per site). This method has been used to monitor small invertebrate prey groups, such as beetles, ants, crickets, and spiders, as well as large invertebrate predators such as centipedes and huntsman spiders50,59.

Nocturnal surveys: geckos and predator abundance

Gecko abundances were monitored via timed spotlight surveys and the use of arboreal cover boards (see description above). Nocturnal surveys were conducted every night, consisting of time-constrained spotlight surveys for geckos and nocturnal predators (i.e., birds, snakes, invertebrates). Each spotlight survey consisted of two observers searching all trees, logs, and the forest floor for 20 min in a “U” shaped transect (to avoid overlap in search area) within each 1 ha. site. All spotlight surveys were completed within the first 3 hours after sunset.

Diurnal surveys: predatory birds

Diurnal predators (mainly carnivorous or omnivorous birds) were monitored using timed point-count dawn bird surveys. Each morning, in both seasons, a survey was conducted by two observers for 10 min between 0530–0700 hrs. We recorded all birds seen or heard during each survey, and later removed any non-predatory birds (i.e., birds that are not known to consume small lizards43,45 from the data).

Data analysis

We used generalized linear mixed-models (GLMMs; R package lme4)60 and model selection to identify the best predictor variables for a series of response variables: predation/attack rate, predatory bird abundance, predatory invertebrate abundance, invertebrate prey abundance, and lizard abundance. We constructed a correlation matrix to identify and remove any variables that showed collinearity. Prior to fitting models, we explored variables for collinearity, checked normality of residuals, heterogeneity of variance, and checked our model distribution to avoid overdispersion61. Two sets of models were used, “environmental models” used season, habitat type, and microhabitat as predictor variables to our response variables listed above, while our “biological models” used the abundance of other organisms as predictor variables (predatory bird abundance, predatory invertebrate abundance, lizard abundance, and invertebrate prey abundance; Table 2). All models included site as a random factor. Attack frequencies on artificial lizards were analysed using a binomial distribution due to the nature of the response variable (model fate: attacked or not attacked). In all other models dealing with count data we used a Poisson distribution. We conducted model selection with the function ‘dredge’ in the R package MuMIn62 using the Akaike Information Criterion to identify optimal models (with a ΔAICc < 2). Model averaging was used when no optimal model could be identified (i.e., there were multiple top models with ΔAICc < 2). Final models were validated by examining deviance residual plots. TukeyHSD tests using lsmeans63 were conducted for post-hoc comparisons. To test differences in attack frequencies between artificial lizards and control (sphere-shaped) models we used a Student’s t-test. All analyses were conducted using the program R64. All experimental protocols were approved by James Cook University under animal ethics approval A2050 and fieldwork was conducted under the Queensland Department of Environment and Heritage Protection research permit WISP14656614 and were performed in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations.