Introduction

Animal personality refers to temporally consistent individual differences in behavior1. To assess these differences, behavioral ecologists run individual animals through multiple iterations of various behavioral tests, and then take the average of each individual’s performance on these tests to assign them to a specific personality or behavioral type. Much personality research aims to parse the causes and consequences of these individual behavioral averages2,3,4,5. For instance, individual averages in traits like activity level and boldness can predict mating success, predator success, and prey survival rates6,7,8,9,10,11. However, individuals also differ in their variability or consistency in behaviors like boldness or activity level12. For instance, in hermit crabs, one individual may take risks consistently over its lifespan, while another may be daring one day and then cautious the next12,13,14. This behavioral intraindividual variability (IIV) is defined as within-individual variability in performance on a given behavioral test, corrected for systematic variation arising from habituation or fatigue12,15,16. Yet, unlike individual behavioral averages2,3,6, the ecological and evolutionary consequences of behavioral IIV remain unclear12,17,18.

This opacity stems in part from conflicting evidence on the fitness consequences of behavioral IIV. Across taxa, behavioral IIV can correspond with high success19 or low success20. For example, birds that sing variable and inconsistent songs have lower mating success21,22,23, and risk-taking IIV in fish correlates with lower foraging performance20. These studies suggest that behavioral variability reflects the lack of a cohesive behavioral strategy, perhaps akin to confusion or panic, resulting in poor performance and fitness. Conversely, other researchers have reasoned that high behavioral IIV and unpredictability in general could be adaptive13,24,25. Unpredictable movement by spiders, sometimes termed “protean behavior”, can render spiders less susceptible to being captured by Portia spiders19,26,27, and more behaviorally variable spiders have been found to be more likely to win contests with conspecifics28. Behavioral inconsistency (IIV) can be beneficial or costly depending on whether it reflects adaptive plasticity or maladaptive confusion. The benefits of variability have been examined in relation to mating success and prey survival, but never predator success.

Therefore, we tested whether behavioral inconsistency (IIV), alongside average behavior (personality) and body size, predicts the outcome of predator-prey interactions between the beach jumping spider, Terralonus californicus (Araneae, Salticidae), and the beach fly, Fucellia rufitibia. We chose these species, because we know that T. californicus eats F. rufitibia based on extensive feeding trials and carbon isotope analyses (McLaughlin & Chism, unpublished data), and because spiders are one of the most voracious predator groups on earth, consuming 400–800 million tons of prey a year, rivaling humans and whales29. Spiders have also been fruitful model organisms for the study of personality and behavioral IIV30. Studies on widow spiders (Latrodectus hesperus) have found that individuals exhibit greater behavioral IIV when they have been raised on high quality diets and are in good body condition31,32. This and other studies suggest that behavioral IIV is associated with high body condition and performance in spiders28,32,34. Based on this association between IIV and high body condition, we hypothesize that behaviorally inconsistent (high IIV) spiders are more effective predators.

Thus, we first attempted to confirm whether activity level IIV was associated with spider body condition. Body condition is often used as a proxy for fitness, and we quantify body condition as the residual distance of spider mass from a regression of body mass on carapace width33. Subsequently, we tested whether spiders with high activity level IIV ate more prey and gained more mass. We chose activity level, measured as the distance that spiders moved in five minutes in an open field test, because it characterizes the aggressive stalking hunting mode of jumping spiders9,35. We also tested whether average activity level and body size (prosoma width) were associated with predator success. We first calculated average activity level and activity level IIV in 25 T. californicus from five test iterations. Next, the spiders went through week-long mesocosm trials where they were free to consume prey in small plastic enclosures, with 12 prey items per enclosure. We evaluated the ability of spiders’ behavioral variability (IIV) to predict the outcome of this predator-prey interaction, as to clarify the ecological consequences of intraspecific behavioral variation in T. californicus.

Results

Spiders were significantly repeatable in their activity level (R = 0.449, CIlower = 0.370, CIupper = 0.598). To screen for potential collinearity in our models we tested for correlations between activity level IIV, average activity level, and prosoma width. Prosoma width was not significantly correlated with either average activity level (R2 = 0.002, L-R Chi2 = 0.051, p = 0.821; Supplementary Figure S1a) or activity level IIV (R2 = 0.049, L-R Chi2 = 1.663, p = 0.280; Supplementary Figure S1b). Activity level IIV was not correlated with average activity level (R2 = 0.049, L-R Chi2 = 0.205, p = 0.651; Supplementary Figure S1c).

Behavioral IIV and body condition

Activity level IIV was unrelated to pre-trial body condition (R2 = 0.043, L-R Chi2 = 1.013, p = 0.314; Supplementary Figure S2a) and post-mesocosm body condition (n = 25, R2 = 0.134, L-R Chi2 = 3.319, p = 0.685; Supplementary Figure S2b).

Predator-prey interaction outcome

We tested whether spider behavioral traits predict the outcome of their interaction with flies, specifically the number of kelp flies eaten and the mass gained from week-long mesocosm trials. Spiders underwent a 63% absolute change in their mass on average by the end of the mesocosm trials. The whole model, containing activity level IIV, average activity level, and prosoma width as fixed effects, significantly predicted mass change (Table 1). Specifically, spider activity level IIV and prosoma width were related to mass change (Table 1; Fig. 1), whereas average activity level and prosoma width had no effect on mass change (Table 1). To confirm these findings, we ran identical analyses on change in body condition. Activity level IIV (GLM effect test: n = 25, L-R Chi2 = 12.309 p = 0.0005; Supplementary Figure S3a) and prosoma width (GLM effect test n = 25, L-R Chi2 = 9.384, p = 0.002; Supplementary Figure S3b) had positive effects on spider body condition gain.

Table 1 Outputs of models comparing aspects of spider behavior to metrics of predator-prey interaction outcome. The * refers to results that are significant at the α = 0.05 level. The Activity IIV and Average Activity rows refer to effect tests. Activity level refers to performance on an open field test, body condition refers to the residual definition of body condition (residual distance from predicted mass)33.
Figure 1
figure 1

Change in spider mass is correlated with (a) Activity level IIV (GLM effect test: n = 25, L-R Chi2 = 6.158, p = 0.013) and (b) Prosoma width (GLM effect test: n = 25, L-R Chi2 = 7.110 p = 0.008). Activity level refers to performance on an open field test, and IIV refers to inter-individual variation estimated by riSD. Lines represent linear best fit. Spider activity level intraindividual variability (IIV) (a) change in body condition (GLM effect test: n = 25, L-R Chi2 = 6.158 p = 0.013) but not (b) number of flies eaten (GLM effect test: L-R Chi2 = 1.102 p = 0.294). Activity level refers to performance on an open field test, body condition refers to the residual definition of body condition (residual distance from predicted mass)33. Trend lines represent best fit regressions.

The whole model containing activity level IIV, average activity level, and prosoma width had a significant effect on the number of flies consumed (Table 1). This is because spiders with greater prosoma width consumed more flies (Table 1; Fig. 2). Interestingly, the number of flies consumed was not significantly correlated with change in body condition (R2 = 0.101, L-R Chi2 = 2.545, p = 0.111).

Figure 2
figure 2

Prosoma width (mm) predicts the number of flies eaten (GLM effect test: n = 25, L-R Chi2 = 4.320 p = 0.038). Trend line represent best fit regression.

Discussion

To probe how behavioral inconsistency (IIV) relates to predator-prey interactions, we tested whether jumping spider activity level IIV was related to low20 or high31 predator mass gain and predator-prey interaction outcome. We found that spider IIV was unrelated to pre- and post-mesocosm body condition. This suggests that the variation in IIV that we observed is unrelated to the initial nutritional state of the spiders, conflicting with work on other spider species, which found that spiders in better body condition were more behaviorally variable32,36. Next, spiders with higher variability in activity levels increased more in mass and body condition (Fig. 1; Supplementary Figure S3), consistent with evidence suggesting that behavioral IIV in spiders is related to higher performance28,31,37.

Interestingly, average activity level predicted no metrics of predator success. This suggests that behavioral inconsistency (IIV) can predict outcomes of predator-prey interactions that average behavior (personality) cannot. This suggests that numerous studies probing the role of personality in spider feeding behavior38,39,40 may not have considered all relevant intraspecific behavioral variation. Notably, neither average activity level nor activity IIV predicted the number of prey that spiders consumed (Table 1), meaning that variation in activity level both within and across individuals is seemingly unrelated to prey capture success. Rather, larger spiders tended to catch more prey and gain more mass, and behaviorally variable spiders gained more mass without necessarily killing more prey (Figs 1 and 2). The effect of prosoma width on mass gain could be explained by the increased ability of large spiders to produce more digestive fluids. The increased mass gain of behaviorally variable spiders adds to growing evidence suggesting that behavioral IIV plays a role in predator-prey interactions19,20, but that the mechanism underlying its effects may be indirect. We discuss two possible explanations for the enhanced performance of behaviorally inconsistent (high IIV) spiders.

First, behaviorally consistent (low IIV) spiders could have gained less mass because they had faster metabolic rates. Spiders with greater metabolic rates would burn though their existing nutritional reserves more quickly, reducing their mass. Considerable evidence suggests that personality traits like aggressiveness, boldness, and activity level can be correlated with metabolic rate5,41,42,43. The same is potentially true for behavioral IIV. However, we found no relationship between spider IIV and pre-mesocosm body condition following an ad libitum feeding event (Supplementary Figure S2a). This result is inconsistent with a relationship between IIV and metabolic rate. Had individuals varied in metabolic rate as function of their activity level IIV, we would have predicted that individuals with a higher metabolic rate (low IIV) would have exhibited a lower body condition at a set time following an equivalent (ad libitum) feeding event. Based on this indirect evidence, we cannot definitively conclude whether or not metabolism is related to IIV in these spiders, but these data do raise some skepticism towards the possibility IIV is strongly related to metabolic rate. Factors such as nutritional history31,44, partial/differential prey consumption45,46, age/experience47,48, and hormonal state49,50 can also affect foraging decisions in spiders. These factors are beyond the scope of the present study, and at present confound our ability to more critically evaluate this hypothesis that IIV is related to metabolic rate.

Second, it is possible that behaviorally variable spiders consumed a greater proportion of the prey that they killed than behaviorally consistent spiders. Killing prey without fully consuming them is termed variably as superfluous killing, wasteful killing, or partial prey consumption, and occurs when predators kill more or larger prey than they can or will consume51. In the grass spider Agelenopsis aperta, and the comb-footed spider, Anelosimus studiosus, tendency to engage in superfluous killing or partial prey consumption varies consistently among individuals in the context of a broad boldness-aggressiveness behavioral syndrome46,52. We propose that a similar association between individual behavioral tendencies (here activity level IIV) and tendency to partially consume may occur in T. californicus. Partial or incomplete prey consumption can be adaptive or costly, depending on the situation. Partial prey consumption can reduce handling time and digestive fluid investment, because digested prey grow more viscous and difficult to ingest as they are consumed45,53. If behavioral IIV correlates with high juvenile nutrition and spider fitness8,28, then behaviorally inconsistent individuals be able to invest more in the production of digestive fluid, thus allowing them to gain more nutrients from their prey. Conversely, low IIV spiders may invest less in digestive fluid, and only consume choice prey portions before seeking out more prey.

The data herein add to growing evidence54,55,56 that intraspecific behavioral variation can predict individual performance in staged mesocosm. An interesting next step might be to probe the fitness consequences of behavioral IIV in unpredictable environments, or to manipulate juvenile environment to observe the cascading consequences it has on adult IIV. Most studies relating behavioral variation to foraging performance ignore IIV in favor of focusing on behavioral averages (behavioral types/personality). For instance, many have found that spider average activity level and aggressiveness predict the number and species of prey animals they consume9,57,58. Had we done this, we would have mistakenly concluded that intraspecific behavioral variation was unrelated to predator performance. However, evaluating IIV is more time consuming, and requires far more data than evaluating average behaviors, which may help to explain why few studies aim to address its effects. Nonetheless, the available evidence suggests that IIV can have sizable and contrasting effects compared to average behavior, and may enhance the predictability of species interaction modules19,20. Therefore, under ideal circumstance, both personality and behavioral IIV should be accounted for when probing the causes and consequences of intraspecific behavioral variation.

Methods

Subject species and collection

We collected T. californicus from sandy beach habitat adjacent to the University of California, Santa Barbara (34.406290°N, 119.847680°W) from August to October 2016. Individuals were collected with an inhalation aspirator apparatus (Bioquip 1135 A, 1135Y), and then transported to the University of California Santa Barbara, where they were stored individually in 12 ml (12 cm × 3 cm) plaster containers (Bioquip 8912) containing approximately 10 g of kelp (Macrocystis pyrifera) wrack. We selected only female spiders from a size range of 0.8 mm to 1.5 mm to ensure that they could subdue the prey we provided. They weighed on average 8.4 ± 4.4 mg SD (Supplementary Figure 4a). We collected specimens of the kelp fly Fucellia rufitibia (identified by GC) from beaches surrounding UCSB campus. We caught these phototactic flies by illuminating containers at night on kelp wracks and closing the containers when approximately 50 flies had entered. We chose this species pair because we know that T. californicus consumes F. rufitibia in situ based on extensive feeding trials, stable isotope analyses, and natural observations (McLaughlin & Chism, unpublished data).

Activity level assays

We began spider behavioral testing four days after collection. Immediately after the spiders arrived in the lab, we fed them all to satiation with five Bledius fenyesi (Staphylinidae), a beetle species that these spiders prey upon in the wild. We began open field assays forty-eight hours after this feeding event.

Activity assays took place in a 120 mm × 120 mm square petri-dish with 1 cm grid paper fastened below it. We gave the spiders 30 seconds to acclimate to the dish before we started the trial. We then counted the number of squares the spiders crossed with their cephalothoraxes during the following 300 seconds. We chose the open field test for two reasons. First, it is a well vetted metric of activity level related to fitness and predatory behavior in numerous other species59,60,61. Second, this test is neither left nor right censored, which can bias behavioral IIV estimates12. The distribution of spiders’ performances on this test does not differ significantly from a normal distribution (Shapiro-Wilk W test: W = 0.99, p = 0.16). We performed this assay every other day for ten days (5 trials per spider).

Predation experiment

After the activity level assays, we gave all spiders a 3-day rest period. We next measured the number of prey they killed and the change in their mass and body condition in a staged mesocosm experiment. We used two chambered mesocosms to create habitat complexity for the spiders and to allow them, and their prey, to move between habitat patches/refuges. The two-chamber setup mimics the natural habitat of these spiders, which is characterized by a series of discrete patches (clumps of decaying kelp) that the spiders move between. We constructed mesocosms using two 750 ml deli containers that we connected by a 15 cm section of 25 mm plastic tubing, and sealed the joints with silicone. We put 250 mg of sieved beach sand (using a 2 mm sieve) in each chamber, along with 30 ml of filtered water and ~10 g of cleaned kelp wrack to each patch (obtained fresh from the surf).

Before we put spiders into the mesocosms, we weighed each spider to the 10−4 gram (Denver Instruments Pinnacle balance) and measured their prosoma widths to the nearest millimeter (Bioquip 4828 M mini-scale). Next, we placed spiders in freshly provisioned mesocosms along with 12 prey items - Fucellia rufitibia (Anthomyiidae, Fucellidae). Twelve flies were more than a single spider could eat but still within the range of densities at which these flies occur in situ (Chism & Lichtenstein, pers. obs.). We left the mesocosms at 18 °C for six days exposed to natural light. Once the trial was completed, we recorded how many flies were killed by predators. Flies killed by predators were easily distinguishable from other deaths, because the corpses exhibited clear bite and feeding point marks, a lack of internal organs, or were mangled from predator handling. To determine changes in body condition, we reweighed and measured the prosoma widths of spiders immediately following the trial. We found no evidence that the spiders had molted during the trials. Thus, we recorded three metrics of predator success: change in body condition, the number of flies consumed, and change in mass. Spiders were then released within 10 m of their original collection site.

Statistical methods

To assess IIV in activity level, we calculated the residual individual standard deviation (riSD) of each spider. The riSD approach is the most widely accepted estimation of IIV, because it accounts for habituation and other systematic sources of performance variation across days12,14,19,62. It is analogous to a standard deviation calculation, but instead of measuring deviation from the average, riSD is calculated with deviation from the expected performance for each day12,63,64. To calculate activity level riSD, we first made a linear model for each spider with “test day” as a predictor variable and “activity level” as a response variable. For each day, we used the estimated slope and intercept of this model to calculate an expected value. Next, we subtracted the expected value from the observed value, and then divided this by the expected value to get average corrected deviation (residuals) of their activity level scores from the expected value for each day. Activity level IIV is the average corrected residual for each spider, and the average IIV was 0.249 ± 0.115 SD (Supplementary Figure S4d) for our spiders. We calculated these values using the lm function in R version 3.3.1. Then, to test whether activity level was significantly repeatable we fit a GLMM with “spider ID’ as a random effect, “testing day” as a fixed effect, and activity level as a response variable. We assessed whether activity level was significantly repeatable by observing whether the confidence intervals of the spider ID random effect overlapped zero, after Nakagawa and Schielzeth65. We fit this model using the rptR package66 in R version 3.3.1.

We estimated the body condition of our spiders as the residuals of a regression of mass against prosoma width33. The residual distance method characterizes body condition as the deviation of spider’s mass from their expected mass for their given body size (prosoma width). We fit two GLMs with normal distributions, prosoma width as the predictor variable and spider mass as the response variable for before and after experiment values. Prosoma width was correlated with mass before (n = 25, R2 = 0.283, L-R Chi2 = 7.978, p = 0.047) and after (n = 25, R2 = 0.637, L-R Chi2 = 24.309, p > 0.0001) the experiment (Supplementary Figure S5). The residuals of these models conformed to normal distributions, implying that Gaussian distributions are good fits for the data. This metric of body condition was not correlated with prosoma width before (n = 25, R2 < 0.001, L-R Chi2 = 0.011, p = 0.917) but it was after the experiment (n = 25, R2 = 0.292, L-R Chi2 = 8.279, p = 0.004). We calculated pre-mesocosm body condition as the residual distance of spiders’ pre-mesocosm mass from the pre-mesocosm prosoma/mass regression and post-mesocosm body condition as the distance of their post-mesocosm mass from this same regression.

Next, to assess the effects of spider behavior on predator success, we used a series of GLMs. We fit two GLMs with normal distributions, with activity level IIV, average activity level, and prosoma width as predictor variables. One model had mass change as the response variable, and the other had change in body condition as the response variable. We fit a similar model for the number of flies consumed with average activity, activity IIV, and prosoma width as predictor variables, but instead fit the data with a GLM using a Poisson distribution. Once again, the residuals of all of these models conformed to normal distributions. To ensure that multicollinearity did not compromise these models, we tested to see whether activity level IIV, average activity level, and prosoma width were correlated using three linear models. We performed all statistics, excepting riSD and repeatability calculations, with JMP 13.0 Pro.