Introduction

Genetic information is increasingly used in different life contexts, such as health [1], law [2] and reproductive choices [3]. This increased application of genetics marks the genomic era, which has brought multiple potential benefits and risks for individuals. In justice, the benefits of genetic information include DNA identification in criminal investigations and the exoneration of the wrongly convicted [4]. Genetics have also provided insights into origins of individual differences in human behaviour, including for traits that are relevant to criminal justice, such as impulsivity, aggressiveness and ‘free’ will/control over behaviour. For example, an aetiological approach conceptualises human will (to act and to control actions) as a trait. Therefore, as with virtually all human traits, individual differences in will result from complex gene-environment processes [5, 6]. This contrasts with the well-established view of ‘free’ will as each individual’s ability to control their actions/decisions and the belief that the will of all ‘sane (mentally healthy)’ people is free to the same extent [7]. Many genes/genetic markers have already been linked to traits such as psychiatric disorders, criminal behaviour, physical illness and learning disabilities, and many more will be discovered in the future [8,9,10,11]. This information may be used to aid justice, for example, in taking decisions on the best interventions for criminal behaviour. Conversely, the same information might be used to harm individuals, including through limiting access to health insurance and education, or through surveillance [12].

The justice system has been slow to utilise advances in genetic knowledge (GK). Current laws are not designed to protect from misuses of the vast amounts of information that can be extracted from whole genome sequencing. For example, laws do not provide sufficient protection from misuses of predictive genetic information on health risks (e.g. for determining insurance premiums); on criminal behaviour (e.g. for crime prevention); on intelligence (e.g. for selection or streaming in education); and on career specific skills (e.g. for the military). Updating relevant laws requires that the judiciary has the necessary expertise, as they play a ‘steering’ role in development of the law for novel matters. Moreover, even before laws are updated, the judiciary faces challenges of deciding cases involving genetic information. An increasing number of cases involve genetic information, including in the US [13, 14]; England and WalesFootnote 1[15]; Italy [16]; and the Netherlands [17]. Taking decisions in such cases without sufficient knowledge of genetics has serious consequences for justice, because these first decisions set precedent—influencing the development of the law.

Considering this important role of the judiciary, it is necessary to explore their knowledge of fundamental genetic concepts. It is also important to explore how prepared judges are to embrace relevant training in genetics. To our knowledge, no previous study explored GK of judges. The findings from several recent studies with non-judges suggest that levels of GK are generally low [18,19,20,21,22]. For example, only 34% of 62 respondents, recruited through a random digit dialling method in the continental United States, knew that genes are stored in every cell of the body [19]. Another study, which utilised the same measure used here, identified that participants struggled to answer questions designed to evaluate a basic and functional level of GK, with only 1.2% of 5404 participants answering all questions correctly [22].

If judges’ GK is similarly insufficient, their decisions in cases involving evaluation of genetic information may be affected. Moreover, variability in GK among judges may contribute to the wide variability in sentencing decisions [23], for example via affecting views on aetiology of behaviour, immutability of ‘genetically determined’ traits, and ‘free will’ [24,25,26].

Several studies that explored judges’ views on the use of genetic information in sentencing found widely mixed views on how this information should be used [24,25,26]. For example, one study explored through phone interviews how knowledge of genetic influences on mental disorders would affect judges’ views on sentencing. The study used a sample of 59 Pennsylvania State Court judges, with a response rate of 7.4% out of a sample of 800 judges approached. The judges were asked to describe their thoughts about sentencing in cases where (a) the offender had mental disorders and (b) the offender’s mental disorder was known to be genetically influenced [27]. The qualitative analyses showed that a high proportion of judges held essentialist beliefs (e.g. that traits are genetically determined); as well as stigmatisation biases associated with such beliefs (e.g. that a person with genetic risk poses a threat to society). The study also suggested that sentencing partly depended on the judges’ personal experiences with genetics, personal experiences with mental disorders and other factors. For example, the stereotyping led to more restrictive sentences and support for deterrence and incapacitation in the absence of personal experience with genetics or mental health illness.

Overall, the results of the limited previous research suggest that genetic information can affect the views of judges, and that these effects may differ as a function of multiple factors, such as held beliefs, knowledge and experience with genetics; and jurisdiction.

A key limitation of previous studies is that they did not specifically evaluate judges’ knowledge on genetics. Another limitation of most previous studies is the potential selection bias, as participation rates were either very low, or unknown. This may have decreased representativeness of the samples, for example with participants having particular interest in issues related to sentencing of people with psychopathy. Another limitation is that most previous research explored judges’ views only in a few jurisdictions (e.g. US, Germany) and more research is needed to understand whether judiciaries’ attitudes differ across jurisdictions. In addition, most previous research focused on mental disorders, specifically psychopathy—a diagnosis associated with much stigma. Previous research also used single items or very few items, mostly addressing only what judges would do in a scenario under the current laws, procedure and guidance, and not judges’ views on how genetic data should be used in court. Finally, most research employed qualitative analyses, quantitative research is needed to compare judges’ knowledge with that of other groups.

The current research addresses some of these limitations. First, 91% of the judges approached in the current study completed the survey. Second, this study is the first to collect data from a non-western sample of judges. Third, the study is the most comprehensive to date—exploring the judiciary’s knowledge on a wide range of genetic concepts; and comparing them with those of non-judge lawyers (from here, ‘lawyers’) and non-lawyers (others). We used the iGLAS—International Genetic Literacy and Attitudes Survey—to collect data on 17 key GK items and heritability estimates for 8 traits [28]. In addition, we evaluated confidence in own GK, and the relationship between knowledge and confidence. Finally, we explored the judges’ views on whether genetics should be included in their training.

We expect that: (a) the judges’ GK will be higher than other groups’ as previous research found that GK is correlated with education; (b) the judges’ confidence in their GK would be higher than that of other groups; (c) knowledge and confidence would be only moderately correlated, as previous research has indicated that people have poor knowledge calibration in many areas [29]; (d) many judges will endorse including genetic information in their training. This prediction is based on our previous sessions with lawyers and judges as part of seminars and working groups; e.g. the Legal, Ethical and Societal Implications of Genetics Working Group [30].

Participants and methods

The study was approved by the Goldsmiths Department of Psychology Ethics Committee (PSY10.10.2016).

This study was conducted as part of an ongoing large scale project by The Accessible Genetics Consortium (TAGC) to collect data from a range of jurisdictions (tagc.world). The iGLAS was used to collect the data. Detailed information on validation, construction and use of iGLAS can be found in a previous publication [28]. iGLAS is a dynamic instrument which is currently in its 10th version. iGLAS can be completed in seven languages, including Romanian. All items in iGLAS were developed in English; then the items were translated and back translated—for each of the languages currently available. The translations were additionally checked by a team of experts and piloted with small sample of participants. Translation documentation is available from the authors. Over 13,000 participants have completed iGLAS to date.

Participants

The total sample included 283 participants (73 judges, 94 lawyers and 116 non-lawyers). All the judges were supreme court judges and were recruited specifically for this study, with data collection facilitated by two members of the supreme court. The Supreme Court is the highest court in Romania, with ~120 judges. Participation rate in this study was 91% (80 judges were approached, 73 provided full data). The samples of lawyers and non-lawyers were composed of all Romanian participants who completed the general iGLAS on-line collection at approximately the same time (August 2017—January 2018). Non-lawyers included participants who identified themselves as students or working in professions other than law.

The number of participants slightly varied across different analyses as not all questions were answered by every participant. Participants were all at least 18 years old (M = 42.08, SD = 11.09). In total, 178 females and 102 males completed this study; two participants identified as non-binary; and two did not disclose their sex. Further descriptive statistics for each group can be found in Table 1. All participants were educated and living in Romania.

Table 1 Descriptive statistics (age and gender) for each group of participants.

Measures and procedure

Data were collected via Qualtrics software. Informed consent was gathered before the beginning of the survey.

Participants first provided demographic information. They then rated how confident they were in their GK and completed 17 GK items (See Table 3 for the list of items). The 17 items were summed to give total GK scores for each participant. Questions were formatted in various ways: yes/no, Likert scales, dropdowns and multiple choice.

Participants also rated on a scale of 0–100 (zero to full genetic influence) the heritability of eight human traits: height, weight, IQ, eye colour, clinical depression, motivation, school achievement, sexual orientation. Participants also completed a large number of opinion and attitudes items. Of these, only one is analysed in the current study: ‘Information about gene-environment processes should be included in judges’ training (strongly disagree to strongly agree on a 1–7 scale).

Results

Table 2 presents the results for GK and confidence for the three groups (judges, lawyers, non-lawyers). The results of an ANCOVA showed that the judges’ GK was significantly higher than that of the other two groups, after controlling for age and education level F(2,269) = 5.24, p = 0.006 (Partial Eta Squared 0.037). The judges’ greater knowledge was not related to differences across the groups in terms of education, F(1,269) = 0.50, p = 0.481 (η20.002). However, their greater knowledge was partially related to age, F(1,269) = 4.18, p = 0.042 (η2 0.015), with older participants scoring on average higher. The judges also showed significantly narrower distribution of scores (Levene’s test = F(2,271) = 18.75, p < 0.001).

Table 2 Overall average genetic knowledge and confidence for the judges, lawyers and non-lawyers.

We also compared the judges’ results with the results obtained from a large sample of unselected participants (N = 5310) reported in Chapman et al., which also collected data using iGLAS [22]. The overall GK score of the judges (73%) was higher than that of the sample in Chapman et al. (65.5% correct) [22]. In Chapman, data collection was not targeted to a specific group; however, 87.6% of the participants had completed or were working towards at least degree level education (93.8% in the current study). Participants tended to be younger (M = 32.5, SD = 12.8) however, the gender split was very similar (61.1% female) to the present study (62.9% female). The frequencies of responses for the judges and this unselected sample are presented in SOM Table 1.

The proportions for each response option for the knowledge items for the three groups are provided in Table 3. Focusing on the judges, the judges’ knowledge of different concepts could be split into three categories: excellent knowledge; relatively good knowledge, and poor knowledge.

Table 3 Number of participants and proportions of responses for each of the multiple-choice option in the 17 genetic knowledge items.

Excellent knowledge was demonstrated for 8 of 17 items. For example, over 90% of the judges knew that: each cell contains two copies of each gene; that there are ~20,000 genes in the human genome; that siblings share on average 50% of the variable DNA (DNA that can differ across people); and that DNA sequence in two different cells is identical. They also correctly stated that Autism results from many (rather than one) genes. Slightly fewer judges (87.5%) answered correctly a similar question about Schizophrenia.

Participants were overall accurate in knowing that we cannot fully predict someone’s behaviour from looking at their DNA (Table 3: Item 16) and that infants are tested for certain genetic traits in many countries (Table 3: item 17). The results of a Chi-squared analyses, comparing the three groups, showed that significantly more judges (95.9%) and lawyers (93.6%) than non-lawyers (83.6%) knew that in many countries infants are tested for certain genetic traits (χ2 (2, N = 283) = 9.51, p = 0.009). Significantly more judges (95.90%) than lawyers (83%) and non-lawyers (85.20%) also correctly identified that we cannot fully predict a person’s behaviour from examining their DNA sequence (χ2 (2, N = 282) = 6.86, p = 0.032).

Relatively good knowledge was demonstrated on 3 of the 17 items. A substantial proportion of judges (65.3%) knew that an epigenetic change is a change in gene expression; that polymorphisms are points of genetic variation (77.8%); and that genetic modification includes both selective breeding and genetic engineering (74%).

Poor knowledge was found for the five remaining items that tapped into fundamental genetic concepts. First, the question ‘what is a genome?’ was correctly answered only by 39.7% of the judges; the majority (60.30%) selected an incorrect answer: ‘all the genes in DNA’. The responses for this item were better for lawyers (74.8%) and non-lawyers (67.2%).

Second, only 57.5% chose the correct combination of the four letter groups that represent the base units of the DNA. The rest of the responses were distributed across the other three incorrect response options—demonstrating a high degree of guessing. The response rates for this item were similar for lawyers (57.6%) and better for non-lawyers (73.4%).

Third, fewer than 25% of the judges answered correctly that the main function of all genes is storing information for protein synthesis. Almost 50% chose the incorrect answer ‘To provide energy to the cell’. A greater proportion of lawyers (52.7%) and non-lawyers (67.5%) than judges chose the correct response for this item.

Fourth, only 38% of the judges knew that on average more than 99% of the total DNA is the same in two people selected at random. 48% selected an incorrect response of ‘less than 50%’. The response rates for this item were similar for lawyers (41.4%) and non-lawyers (50%).

Fifth, only 28.8% of the judges knew that “non-coding” DNA describes DNA that does not lead to the production of protein. A higher proportion of lawyers (40.2%) and non-lawyers (46.9%) than judges chose the correct response for this item. The judges’ answers were distributed across all 4 response options—suggesting high degree of guessing.

Table 2 in SOM presents participants’ estimates of genetic influence (heritability) for eight complex traits. Results show that the judges were overall relatively accurate in estimating the heritability of these traits, providing similar estimates to those reported in the scientific studies [22]. The estimates were also overall accurate for the other two groups, with a tendency to underestimate in some cases (e.g. height; weight).

Confidence

The average judges’ confidence in their GK was 55.6 out of 100, ranging from 30 to 76 (see Table 2). This is significantly higher than that found in the other two groups (non-lawyers, showing the lowest). It is also higher than the confidence level (35.95 out of 100) found in a group of undergraduate Psychology students in the UK (N = 153) [31]. The ANCOVA on GK confidence, indicated significant group differences between the judges, lawyers and non-lawyers when controlling for education level and age, F(2,266) = 6.39, p = 0.002, η2 = 0.046. Age did not have a significant effect on confidence F(1,266) = 1.55, p = 0.215, η2 = 0.006. Education level also did not have a significant effect F(1,266) = 0.201, p = 0.654, η2 = 0.001. Levene’s test indicated unequal variances, F (2268) = 19.73, p < 0.001, with judges showing a narrower distribution than the other two groups.

Moderate positive correlations between GK and confidence were found in the samples of judges (r = 0.43, N = 72, p < 0.001); and lawyers (r = 0.34, N = 93, p < 0.001). There was no significant correlation for non-lawyers (r = 0.12, N = 115, p = 0.206).

Training

100% of the judges agreed that information about gene-environment processes should be included in judges’ training: 74%—agreed, 16.4%—somewhat agreed, and 9.6%—strongly agreed.

Discussion

The judges’ average GK was relatively good but uneven. For some items, the judges’ average knowledge was much higher than that of the lawyers and non-lawyers; and for other items, substantially lower (see Table 2). The observed pattern of results shows that the average good knowledge shown by the judges reflects partly their ability to apply common sense to the multiple-choice options—selecting the most plausible answers based on their general knowledge and perhaps school biology. It also shows that their knowledge of some of the key genetic concepts is poor.

When compared to the results of Chapman et al. [22], the judges particularly underperformed on knowledge items related to molecular genetics, especially: the function of genes, the definition of the genome, the nature of non-coding DNA and the constituent units of DNA. For example, over 60% of the judges selected an incorrect answer for the question of ‘what is a genome’—selecting ‘all the genes in DNA’. Understanding the difference between genes and genome is important, for example, for decisions to undergo genetic testing or interpreting the results of such testing. In sentencing, ‘predictive’ information (from single genes vs. polygenic risk scores vs. epigenetic profiles) may be incorrectly interpreted or given an inappropriate weight.

Less than 40% of the judges knew that on average more than 99% of the total DNA is the same in two people selected at random. At the same time, the judges correctly identified sibling genetic similarity. These results suggest that many judges do not distinguish between ‘total DNA’ and ‘variable/differing/segregating’ DNA—the portion (less than 1%) of the total DNA that differs across people. These misconceptions may lead to other misconceptions, such as viewing individuals from different groups as being genetically very different; or having deterministic and essentialist biases.

The judges were overall accurate in estimating heritability. In fact, they did not show a pattern of overestimation and underestimation previously found in Chapman et al. with the same items [22]. In that study, heritability of traits viewed as ‘fixed’, such as height, eye colour and IQ were overestimated; and heritability of traits viewed as ‘malleable’, such as motivation, school achievement and weight, were underestimated.

Confidence (self-evaluation of their knowledge) was significantly higher in the group of judges, compared with the other two groups, as well as to the results of a study that utilised this same measure with a cohort of undergraduate psychology students in the UK [31]. This was expected as the judge’s knowledge was also higher than that of the other groups. However, the correlations between knowledge and confidence for judges (r = 0.43) and lawyers (r = 0.34) were only moderate; and correlation was not significant for non-lawyers. This suggests that people, including judges, are not precise in the estimates of their own GK. For example, many judges whose knowledge was high did not think so, and some of those whose knowledge was very poor reported high knowledge.

Summary and conclusion

Overall, the results demonstrate that without specific training even most highly educated people, at the top of their profession lack much of the essential GK. This is of course not surprising as genetic science is rapidly developing, amassing vast amounts of complex continuously updated information. The 100% endorsement by the judges of the need for gene-environment training attests to their recognition of the importance of such knowledge and is a positive outcome for justice.

The three main findings of this study are:

  1. 1.

    Judges showed higher GK than other groups. This relatively good overall GK masked poor knowledge of five of the most fundamental genetic concepts that are important for successful application of GK in the genomic era.

  2. 2.

    The judges’ confidence in their GK, although higher than that of the other groups, was quite low and showed only a moderate correlation with their knowledge.

  3. 3.

    100% of the judges agreed that information about gene-environment processes should be included in judges’ training—endorsing the importance of GK for justice.

The study addressed some limitations of previous research, exploring knowledge on a wide range of topics in a sample of the judiciary from a non-western country with a very high response rate. However, a range of socio-cultural factors and country-specific context of the legal system may mean that the results are not fully applicable across all jurisdictions. For example, Romanian judges participate at least once every 3 years in continuous training programmes organised by National Institute of Magistracy or other institutions. The continuous training might have contributed to their unanimous endorsement of the need for training in gene-environment processes. The representativeness of the non-selected sample (non-lawyers) is likely to have been limited by the method of data collection. Participants needed the economic and educational resources to access the internet in order to engage with this study. This is supported by the fact that 79.7% of the non-lawyers were either working towards or had completed degree level studies. Further work is underway to collect data from judiciary and non-lawyers in other countries to examine generalisability of the results.

To conclude, judges have significant weight in the development of laws. It is clear that the first step towards achieving adequate laws in this area is to increase genetic expertise of the justice stakeholders. The study identified specific weaknesses in judges’ knowledge, and this information can be used in designing training programmes. Over the past 10 years genetics has begun to penetrate curricula across the social sciences, including degrees in psychology, sociology, anthropology and economics. We hope that the findings of the present study will encourage this trend in training of lawyers.