Abstract
Background
Cancer studies reported mixed results on benefit finding (BF) and posttraumatic growth (PTG) prevalence and few were focused on long-term survivors.
Methods
BF and PTG were assessed in a multi-regional population-based study in Germany with 6952 breast, colorectal and prostate cancer survivors, using the Benefit Finding Scale and Posttraumatic Growth Inventory. We calculated the age-adjusted prevalence, stratified by demographical and clinical characteristics.
Results
Overall, 66.0% of cancer survivors indicated moderate-to-high BF, and 20.5% moderate-to-high PTG. Age-adjusted prevalence of BF and PTG differed according to cancer type (breast > colorectal > prostate) and sex (female > male). BF and PTG prevalence were higher in younger than in older respondents; the age-adjusted prevalence was higher in respondents who survived more years after diagnosis. The strength and direction of associations of age-adjusted prevalence with cancer stage, disease recurrence, and time since diagnosis varied according to cancer type and sex.
Conclusions
A substantial proportion of long-term cancer survivors reported moderate-to-high BF and PTG. However, the prevalence was lower in older and male cancer survivors, and during the earlier years after cancer diagnosis. Further longitudinal studies on PTG and BF in cancer survivors are warranted to address heterogeneity in survivors’ experience after cancer diagnosis.
Background
Cancer, as a life-threatening illness, has been recognised as a traumatic event [1]. However, over half of cancer survivors report at least one beneficial change or personal growth in cancer experience [2]. Derived from such positive perceptions, benefit finding (BF) [3] and posttraumatic growth (PTG) [4] have been conceptualised and widely researched. Both terms are often used synonymously in publications, but they differ. BF is a form of cognitive adaptation to adversity in which survivors positively evaluate their circumstance [3, 5]. PTG is defined as the positive changes experienced from a traumatic event [4]. BF assesses the broader and less specific positive changes compared to PTG, and the adversity is not necessary to be traumatic [6]. The experience of BF can start immediately after cancer diagnosis, while that of PTG can take a longer time to initiate due to essential processes (e.g. self-disclosure and rumination) that need to be first worked through [4]. For decades, researchers have found that cancer survivors with more BF and PTG reported higher health-related quality of life [7, 8]. Hence, clinicians recognise the importance of BF and PTG in cancer survivors [9, 10].
A challenging/traumatic event like cancer is crucial to trigger BF or PTG [4, 5]. Cancer trajectories vary depending on cancer type, stage, treatment and care protocols. Individuals’ approaches to post-diagnosis appraisal and coping (e.g. gain a sense of mastery) and the perception of BF and PTG in cancer experience vary as well [11]. Reported prevalence rates of BF and PTG range from 10% for PTG [12] to 99% for BF [13] in single-type cancer samples. Age and sex could affect feelings about cancer experience. A meta-analysis [14] showed that survivors of different trauma younger than 60 years were more likely than older survivors to report moderate-to-high PTG. On the other hand, older females have been considered to be cognitively more mature to comprehend the meaning of a cancer diagnosis and to experience personal growth than younger females [15]. Females are considered to be more emotionally sensitive to perceive the positive changes/growth from cancer experience than males [16]. In addition, research suggests that time is associated with the development of PTG and BF. However, the results are conflicting. On the one hand, studies suggested that time is needed [13, 17], and on the other hand, the recall of traumatic experiences reduces with increasing time since diagnosis [16]. Results from systematic reviews regarding the factors associated with prevalence of BF and PTG among cancer survivors are inconsistent [15, 18, 19]. Moreover, few studies focused on long-term (≥5 years post-diagnosis) cancer survivors [14].
Due to the increasing prevalence of cancer, it is crucial to better understand the outcomes of cancer-related stress and coping, such as BF and PTG. Most of the pertinent studies on the prevalence of BF and PTG have focused on heterogeneous cancer types and had mixed results [14, 20]. There is a lack of studies reporting the prevalence stratified by cancer type and further potential covariates. To fill these gaps, this study aims to describe age-specific and age-adjusted prevalence of BF and PTG in long-term cancer survivors according to cancer type, sex, cancer stage, experience of recurrence during follow-up, and time since cancer diagnosis. The findings might be useful for clinicians to understand and support cancer survivors’ recall of positive feelings from cancer experience in the long run.
Methods
Study participants
The study population came from the CAESAR study (Cancer survivorship—a multi-regional population-based study) which was conducted by the German Cancer Research Center (Deutsches Krebsforschungszentrum, DKFZ) in cooperation with population-based cancer registries in Germany. Participants in this study were registered in one of six population-based cancer registries in Germany (including the federal states of Schleswig-Holstein, Hamburg, Bremen, North Rhine-Westphalia (administrative district of Münster), Rhineland-Palatinate, and Saarland) [21].
Details of the recruitment of study participants including data collection have been described elsewhere [21]. In brief, the study included 20–75 years old (age at diagnosis) breast, prostate and colorectal cancer survivors diagnosed during 1994–2004. Data collection was conducted from August 2009 until April 2011 by postal questionnaires [22]. Of the 15,674 cancer survivors eligible for the study, 8631 did not participate, 91 completed a short questionnaire without BF or PTG items. Of the 6952 survivors who completed the long questionnaire (response rate: 44%), 3045 were breast cancer survivors, 1504 were colorectal cancer survivors (627 females, 877 males), and 2403 were prostate cancer survivors.
Outcomes and measurements
Demographics and clinical data
The questionnaire used in CAESAR study included questions regarding demographic and clinical factors. Clinical data such as cancer type, cancer stage, year of birth, and year of diagnosis were provided by the cancer registries. Disease recurrence was self-reported and is defined as any recurrence, metastases or new cancer after index cancer. Respondents’ age was calculated by deducting year of birth from year of survey; years since diagnosis equate to the year of survey minus the year of diagnosis.
Benefit finding
Benefit finding (BF) was measured by the German short form of the Benefit Finding Scale (BFS) [23]. The original [24] and German version [23] are valid and reliable. The 10-item BFS is scored on five-point Likert scales ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely). Items are classed into four subscales (acceptance/sensitive to others/improving coping/ new purpose of life).
Posttraumatic growth
Posttraumatic growth (PTG) was assessed by three subscales (appreciation of life/spiritual change/new possibilities) of the German version of the Posttraumatic Growth Inventory (PTGI) [25]. Two other scales, ‘personal strength’ and ‘relationship to others’ were not included [13], given their overlaps with BFS and also to limit the length of the total questionnaire to reduce participant burden [6]. The original [26] and German version [25] are valid and reliable. The 10-item PTGI in this study uses six-point Likert scales ranging from 0 (I did not experience this change as a result of my cancer) to 5 (I experienced this change to a very great degree as a result of my cancer).
Intensity of BF and PTG
The intensity levels of BF and PTG were determined by the mean item score. The mean item score was calculated as the mean of non-missing items if at least half of the items had been completed, which was then dichotomised using a cut-off of 3 (indicated as ‘moderate’ according to a previous report [27]) into ‘none-to-low’ and ‘moderate-to-high’ (Supplementary Table 1).
Statistical analysis
Multiple imputation with 25 repetitions was employed to handle missing values by using the Markov chain Monte Carlo method to reduce possible bias and increase precision of the replaced values [28]. Before analysing prevalence, we examined the reliability and validity [29] within each individual instrument (Supplementary Table 2-1) and across instruments (Supplementary Table 2-2) of the BFS and PTGI used in this study. Within instruments, BFS and PTGI demonstrated acceptable internal consistency (Cronbach’s α > 0.70) and validity (Supplementary Table 2-1). BFS and PTG were highly correlated (odds ratio = 23.3, co-prevalence of BF and PTG are presented in Supplementary Table 3) but the reliability and validity of the two instruments differed. Although there were overlaps between BFS and PTGI (especially for the subscale “new purpose of life”, which showed high convergent and low discriminant validity with PTGI, see Supplementary Table 2-2), differences existed in the subscales between BFS and PTGI. For BFS, the acceptance subscale showed low convergent and high discriminant validity with PTGI; and for PTGI, the spiritual change subscale showed low convergent and high discriminant validity with BFS.
In the description of characteristics of the total study population, and by cancer type and sex, respondents’ were grouped according to age at survey: <60 years, 60–69 years, 70–79 years, or 80–89 years; and to their time since diagnosis: long-term cancer survivors (LTCS; 5–9 years post-diagnosis) or very long-term cancer survivors (VLTCS; 10 or more years post-diagnosis). Age-adjusted prevalence of moderate-to-high BF and PTG by cancer type and sex were calculated according to the direct method using the age-specific proportions from the total sample described previously as standard population weights. Distributions of moderate-to-high BF and PTG prevalence according to age groups in cancer survivors overall and by cancer type and sex were also estimated. Detailed age-adjusted prevalence of moderate-to-high BF and PTG by characteristics of the study population on overall scale level were calculated accordingly, stratified by cancer type and sex. Chi2-tests were used to evaluate subgroup differences. A two-tailed P-value <0.05 was considered statistically significant.
Results
Sample characteristics
The imputed data was comparable to original data (Table 1). From the imputed data, mean respondents’ age was 69.1 ± 8.9 years, and mean time since diagnosis was 8.0 ± 2.2 years. Nearly three quarters of the survivors were diagnosed with cancer at UICC stage I/II, and 12.9% had experienced cancer recurrence.
Age-adjusted prevalence of moderate-to-high BF and PTG by cancer type and sex
Overall, 66.0% of all survivors indicated moderate-to-high BF (95%CI: 65.8%–66.2%) and 20.5% moderate-to-high PTG (95%CI: 20.3%–20.7%) (Fig. 1). Similar patterns of moderate-to-high BF and PTG were observed across cancer type and sex after adjusting for respondents’ age. The age-adjusted prevalence of moderate-to-high BF, as well as PTG, was significantly different between cancer types and sex (P < 0.0001). Stratifying all survivors by sex, the age-adjusted prevalence was statistically significantly higher in female than in male cancer survivors, both for moderate-to-high BF (69.8% females: 61.0% males, P < 0.0001) and moderate-to-high PTG (23.5% females: 16.4% males, P < 0.0001).
Age-specific and age-adjusted prevalence of moderate-to-high BF and PTG by sample characteristics, stratified by cancer type and sex
Pattern of age-specific prevalence: a significant trend by age was noted in which older survivors reported lower prevalence of moderate-to-high BF and PTG. When stratified by cancer type, the same pattern was observed in breast cancer survivors (Figs. 2 and 3). Comparing all subgroups stratified by cancer type and sex, the highest prevalence of moderate-to-high BF and PTG was observed in the younger than 60 years old female colorectal cancer survivors group (Figs. 2 and 3).
Age-adjusted prevalence by other characteristics: Overall, survivors of stage III/IV cancer reported lower prevalence of moderate-to-high BF (P < 0.0001) and PTG (P < 0.0001) as compared to stage I/II survivors (Figs. 2 and 3). When stratified by cancer type and sex, reverse patterns and general heterogeneity were found. Statistically significant differences were detected when stratified by cancer type and sex, except for prostate cancer survivors on the prevalence of moderate-to-high BF (Fig. 2), and male colorectal cancer survivors on the prevalence of moderate-to-high BF and PTG (Figs. 2 and 3).
In general, survivors having had an experience of cancer recurrence indicated less BF than survivors without recurrence or metastasis (P < 0.0001). This pattern was found in all tumour groups but reversely in prostate cancer survivors (Fig. 2). For PTG, no significant differences in the prevalence of moderate-to-high PTG were found between survivors with and without recurrence of disease in the overall survivors, breast cancer survivors, and female colorectal cancer survivors, but contradicting patterns were found for male colorectal cancer and prostate cancer (Fig. 3).
Regarding time since diagnosis, LTCS showed a significantly lower age-adjusted prevalence of moderate-to-high BF (Fig. 2) and moderate-to-high PTG than VLTCS (Fig. 3). However, this pattern varied according to cancer type and sex. Time since diagnosis was inversely associated with the prevalence of moderate-to-high BF in prostate cancer survivors (P < 0.0001) but positively associated with the prevalence of moderate-to-high BF in female colorectal cancer survivors. Time since diagnosis was positively associated with the prevalence of moderate-to-high PTG in breast and female colorectal cancer survivors but inversely associated in male colorectal cancer survivors (P = 0.0005).
Discussion
Based on data from a multi-regional population-based study, our study contributes detailed information on the prevalence of BF and PTG according to demographic and disease characteristics. When adjusted for respondents’ age, our results suggest that the prevalence of BF and PTG differed by cancer type (breast > colorectal > prostate) and sex (female > male). Generally, the prevalence of moderate-to-high BF and PTG is lower in older cancer survivors compared to younger age groups; the age-adjusted prevalence of moderate-to-high BF and PTG was higher in VLTCS than in LTCS. Cancer stage, disease recurrence and time since diagnosis were also associated with the prevalence of BF and PTG, although the strength and direction of the association varied according to cancer type and sex. To our knowledge, our study is the largest one to address the role of time since diagnosis on the prevalence of moderate-to-high BF and PTG in cancer survivors 5–16 years after diagnosis, with sufficient power to describe the prevalence by cancer type and sex in detail.
Age, in general, had an inverse relationship with PTG and BF, which is comparable to previous studies [14, 30]. The results suggest that being confronted with a cancer diagnosis at a younger age is more threatening than at an older age, thereby inducing the greater potential for initiation of the BF and PTG process. One further explanation is that age can be associated with life cycle events, e.g. retirement or bereavement, experienced by cancer survivors beyond their cancer; aging may also correspond to a decline in health and function [30]. The greater the number and severity of experienced life and health-related events, the higher the traumatic stress level an individual may be able to endure. Stanko and colleagues [31] suggested another explanation that the older (age > 65) lost hope in having positive changes occur after a trauma. While lower life expectations would reduce the discrepancy between one’s original beliefs (towards the world, self, and self-in-world) and trauma meaning; then less distress would be perceived and less BF and PTG would be experienced [32]. Most respondents in this study were older than 60 years when they were diagnosed with cancer. Therefore, the prevalence of moderate-to-high PTG in this study was lower compared to other studies with younger samples [14, 33].
In our study, the prevalence of moderate-to-high PTG ranged between 16 and 24%. These rates are lower compared to previous studies in a recent review [14]. It is possible that these differences with previous studies might be related to cultural differences, e.g. 25% of breast cancer survivors in our sample reported moderate-to-high PTG. This is in contrast to 60–73% in a Chinese sample, 66% in a Polish sample, and 56% in a Portuguese sample [14]. However, due to the heterogeneity of the sample characteristics and different cut-off values used in previous studies, it is difficult to directly compare the prevalence among the studies using samples from other cultures. Furthermore, our sample is older than those from previous studies [14], which might also be related to the low prevalence. Future studies could address potential cross-cultural differences of BF and PTG.
Following adjustment by respondents’ age, our study found prevalence differences in BF and in PTG according to sex and cancer type. Reflecting the sex distribution of the cancer types, the highest prevalence rates of BF and PTG were noted in breast cancer survivors, and the lowest prevalence in prostate cancer survivors in this study. The sex difference (higher prevalence was found in females than in males) also existed in colorectal cancer survivors. Life cycle events, physical changes, and developmental maturity associated with aging could also differ by sex. Females experience more stressful life events with physical changes, like pregnancy, giving birth or (peri)menopause [34, 35]. They may also be more sensitive to perceive a situation as threatening and stressful, and experience more psychological and biological responses to trauma [36]. Existing meta-analyses regarding BF and PTG indicated that females reported more BF and PTG than males [2, 30], and that this trend increases with increasing mean age of the sample.
Cancer stage may also contribute to differences in the prevalence of PTG and BF. The lower chance of cancer-free survival associated with more advanced cancer stage could increase the sense of hopelessness and perception of illness in a negative way [37]. As a result, cancer survivors with more advanced cancer stage might have reported lower BF and PTG. This association of cancer stage with BF and PTG is confirmed in this study, even when stratified by sex or cancer types, which is comparable to previous studies that only focused on a single cancer (e.g. breast cancer [38], colorectal cancer [13]).
We found that long-term cancer survivors, with or without recurrence, reported comparable prevalence of PTG, but those with cancer recurrence/metastasis generally had a higher chance to perceive BF. Previous studies also reported that long-term survivors with adolescent cancer who had recurrence/metastasis did not significantly differ in terms of their amount of PTG from those who did not experience such an episode [13, 39]. However, these results are based on cross-sectional data. Longitudinal research design could better describe the trajectory of PTG before and after recurrence. Cancer recurrence/metastasis is also a challenging or traumatic event. As PTG needs time to get through the initiation processes (e.g. self-disclosure and rumination) [4], and BF could be experienced soon after the event, time since recurrence could be the potential confounder that influence the recurrence and PTG/BF. It is also possible that personality traits (e.g. optimism, positive reframing) could play a role [2, 18]. However, this study did not include personality traits. Further studies could investigate the associations between perceived illness severity and personality traits on BF after disease recurrence in long-term cancer survivors.
While previous research only included short-term survivors (within 5 years) [13, 40], we included long-term cancer survivors (5–16 years post-diagnosis) in this study and generally found that longer time since diagnosis was associated with more moderate-to-high BF and PTG in this study. As the recall of traumatic experience could be reduced and trauma-related stress could decline with time, the relationship between PTG and the passage of time since diagnosis may be more positive and stronger in the immediate years following diagnosis and treatment than after several years of survivorship [41]. Our results support the assumption that time since diagnosis has positive relationships with BF and with PTG, and these relationships may last for more than 5 years. However, the cross-sectional samples in this study were 5–16 years past diagnosis, which suggests selection bias (healthy survivor bias) could exist. Those who survive from cancer for over 5 years may be more grateful and report higher BF/PTG. Taken together, further evidence from longitudinal studies with repeated BF/PTG assessments is needed to conclude on the actual association between time since diagnosis and BF/PTG.
Limitations
Although our reports were from a population-based study, which allowed the detailed stratification according to several factors, several limitations still need to be noted. Firstly, we acknowledge that potential response bias could exist because of an over-selection of ‘fitter’ survivors who were able to complete the questionnaire or were more interested/motivated to give feedback. Secondly, there is a limited generalisation with regard to sex differences as we only looked at selected cancer types. Nevertheless, these three cancers are the most common cancers for males and females (apart from lung cancer, for which the proportion of long-term survivors is very low). Another factor related to cancer type is that the age distribution of samples differed between the cancers. To address this age difference, we reported age-adjusted prevalence when we stratified the groups by cancer type and sex. Thirdly, we did not use full instruments because we wanted to reduce participant burden by excluding overlapping subscales. However, the mean ratings of two excluded subscales of PTGI reported in other studies [42, 43] were almost the same as the subscales included in our study. As such, the result that a higher prevalence of BF compared to PTG was reliable even though we only used three subscales of PTGI. Fourthly, there are no universal cut-offs for BFS and PTGI. We chose a published criterion previously used in a German sample as this minimises the potential bias of using a shortened version of the BFS and PTGI. Our results could reflect the difference between the two concepts; BF describes broader and less specific positive changes compared to PTG. However, the difference in prevalence between BF and PTG is intriguing and should be investigated in future studies [13].
Future studies on the prevalence of BF/PTG in cancer survivors need to address the abovementioned limitations. For example, they could use full instruments and include more types of cancers to verify the results. Future prospective studies should ideally assess baseline BF/PTG before the onset of cancer or at least at diagnosis and longitudinal studies with repeated BF/PTG assessments could verify the association between time since diagnosis and BF/PTG. Additional studies are also needed to test cognitive perception of illness severity in processing of BF and PTG.
Conclusion
We found that moderate-to-high BF and PTG are common in cancer survivors 5–16 years after diagnosis and that the prevalence varies with survivors’ age, sex, cancer type, stage of disease, recurrence/metastasis, and time since diagnosis. Our results highlight the heterogeneity in survivors’ experience after cancer diagnosis. Prevalence of BF and PTG was lower in older and male cancer survivors, during the earlier years after diagnosis and in survivors with more advanced cancer diagnosis. Further longitudinal studies on PTG and BF in cancer survivors are warranted to address heterogeneity in survivors’ experience after cancer diagnosis. Better identification of vulnerable survivors, e.g. taking demographic and clinical factors into consideration, could assist in tailoring interventions appropriate to their needs.
Data availability
Not applicable.
References
Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®). American Psychiatric Association. 5th ed. American Psychiatric Pub: Arlington, VA, USA: 2013..
Helgeson VS, Reynolds KA, Tomich PL. A meta-analytic review of benefit finding and growth. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2006;74:797.
Tennen, H, Affleck, G. Benefit-finding and benefit-reminding. In: Snyder, CR, Lopez, SJ, editors. Handbook of positive psychologydn. (Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2002) pp. 584–97.
Tedeschi RG, Calhoun LG. Posttraumatic growth: conceptual foundations and empirical evidence. Psychol Inq. 2004;15:1–18.
Taylor SE. Adjustment to threatening events: a theory of cognitive adaptation. Am Psycho. 1983;38:1161–73.
Applebaum, AJ, Marziliano, A, Schofield, E, Breitbart, W, Rosenfeld, B. Measuring positive psychosocial sequelae in patients with advanced cancer. Psycho Trauma. https://doi.org/10.1037/tra0000944 (2020).
Brand C, Barry L, Gallagher S. Social support mediates the association between benefit finding and quality of life in caregivers. J Health Psychol. 2016;21:1126–36.
Liu Z, Doege D, Thong M, Arndt V. The relationship between posttraumatic growth and health-related quality of life in adult cancer survivors: a systematic review. J Affect Disord. 2020;276:159–68.
Currier JM, Holland JM, Neimeyer RA. Making sense of loss: a content analysis of end-of-life practitioners’ therapeutic approaches. Omega 2008;57:121–41.
Tedeschi, RG, Calhoun, LG, Groleau, JM. Clinical applications of posttraumatic growth. In: Joseph, S, editor. Positive psychology in practice: Promoting human flourishing in work, health, education and everyday life, 2nd edn. (John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2015) pp. 503–18.
Franks HM, Roesch SC. Appraisals and coping in people living with cancer: a meta‐analysis. Psychooncology 2006;15:1027–37.
Holtmaat K, van der Spek N, Cuijpers P, Leemans CR, Verdonck-de Leeuw IM. Posttraumatic growth among head and neck cancer survivors with psychological distress. Psychooncology 2017;26:96–101.
Jansen L, Hoffmeister M, Chang-Claude J, Brenner H, Arndt V. Benefit finding and post-traumatic growth in long-term colorectal cancer survivors: prevalence, determinants, and associations with quality of life. Br J Cancer. 2011;105:1158–65.
Wu X, Kaminga AC, Dai W, Deng J, Wang Z, Pan X, et al. The prevalence of moderate-to-high posttraumatic growth: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. 2019;243:408–15.
Koutrouli N, Anagnostopoulos F, Potamianos G. Posttraumatic stress disorder and posttraumatic growth in breast cancer patients: a systematic review. Women health. 2012;52:503–16.
Jin Y, Xu J, Liu D. The relationship between post traumatic stress disorder and post traumatic growth: gender differences in PTG and PTSD subgroups. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2014;49:1903–10.
Bourdon M, Blanchin M, Campone M, Quéreux G, Dravet F, Sébille V, et al. A comparison of posttraumatic growth changes in breast cancer and melanoma. Health Psychol. 2019;38:878–87.
Shand LK, Cowlishaw S, Brooker JE, Burney S, Ricciardelli LA. Correlates of post-traumatic stress symptoms and growth in cancer patients: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychooncology. 2015;24:624–34.
Casellas-Grau A, Ochoa C, Ruini C. Psychological and clinical correlates of posttraumatic growth in cancer: a systematic and critical review. Psychooncology. 2017;26:2007–18.
Casellas-Grau A, Sumalla EC, Lleras M, Vives J, Sirgo A, Leon C, et al. The role of posttraumatic stress and posttraumatic growth on online information use in breast cancer survivors. Psychooncology 2018;27:1971–8.
Arndt V, Koch-Gallenkamp L, Bertram H, Eberle A, Holleczek B, Pritzkuleit R, et al. Return to work after cancer. a multi-regional population-based study from Germany. Acta Oncol. 2019;58:811–8.
Koch-Gallenkamp L, Bertram H, Eberle A, Holleczek B, Schmid-Höpfner S, Waldmann A, et al. Fear of recurrence in long-term cancer survivors—Do cancer type, sex, time since diagnosis, and social support matter? Health Psychol. 2016;35:1329–33.
Mohamed NE, Böhmer S. Die deutsche Version der Benefit Finding Skala: Ihre psychometrischen Eigenschaften bei Tumorpatienten. Z Med Psychol. 2004;13:85–91.
Antoni MH, Lehman JM, Kilbourn KM, Boyers AE, Culver JL, Alferi SM, et al. Cognitive-behavioral stress management intervention decreases the prevalence of depression and enhances benefit finding among women under treatment for early-stage breast cancer. Health Psychol. 2001;20:20–32.
Maercker A, Langner R. Posttraumatic personal growth: Validation of German versions of 2 questionnaires. Diagnostica 2001;47:153–62.
Tedeschi RG, Calhoun LG. The Posttraumatic Growth Inventory: Measuring the positive legacy of trauma. J Trauma Stress. 1996;9:455–71.
Schroevers MJ, Teo I. The report of posttraumatic growth in Malaysian cancer patients: relationships with psychological distress and coping strategies. Psychooncology. 2008;17:1239–46.
Cummings P. Missing data and multiple imputation. JAMA Pediatrics. 2013;167:656–61.
Iravani K, Jafari P, Akhlaghi A, Khademi B. Assessing whether EORTC QLQ-30 and FACT-G measure the same constructs of quality of life in patients with total laryngectomy. Health Qual Life Out. 2018;16:183.
Bombardier CH, Ehde DM, Stoelb B, Molton IR. The relationship of age-related factors to psychological functioning among people with disabilities. Phys Med Rehabil Clin N Am. 2010;21:281–97.
Stanko KE, Cherry KE, Ryker KS, Mughal F, Marks LD, Brown JS, et al. Looking for the silver lining: Benefit finding after Hurricanes Katrina and Rita in middle-aged, older, and oldest-old adults. Curr Psychol. 2015;34:564–75.
Park CL. Making sense of the meaning literature: an integrative review of meaning making and its effects on adjustment to stressful life events. Psychol Bull. 2010;136:257.
Silva SM, Moreira HC, Canavarro MC. Examining the links between perceived impact of breast cancer and psychosocial adjustment: the buffering role of posttraumatic growth. Psychooncology. 2012;21:409–18.
Steiner M, Dunn E, Born L. Hormones and mood: from menarche to menopause and beyond. J Affect Disord. 2003;74:67–83.
Cohen LS, Soares CN, Vitonis AF, Otto MW, Harlow BL. Risk for new onset of depression during the menopausal transition: the Harvard study of moods and cycles. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63:385–90.
Olff M, Langeland W, Draijer N, Gersons BP. Gender differences in posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychol Bull. 2007;133:183.
Nehir S, Tavşanli NG, Özdemir Ç, Akyol T. A determination of hopelessness and the perception of illness in cancer. Omega 2019;79:115–31.
Bellizzi KM, Smith AW, Reeve BB, Alfano CM, Bernstein L, Meeske K, et al. Posttraumatic growth and health-related quality of life in a racially diverse cohort of breast cancer survivors. J Health Psychol. 2010;15:615–26.
Gunst DCM, Kaatsch P, Goldbeck L. Seeing the good in the bad: which factors are associated with posttraumatic growth in long-term survivors of adolescent cancer? Supportive Care Cancer. 2016;24:4607–15.
Sears SR, Stanton AL, Danoff-Burg S. The yellow brick road and the emerald city: benefit finding, positive reappraisal coping and posttraumatic growth in women with early-stage breast cancer. Health Psychol. 2003;22:487–97.
Stanton, AL, Bower, JE, Low, CA. Posttraumatic growth after cancer. (eds) Handbook of posttraumatic growth: Research and practicedn. (2006) pp. 138–75.
Taku K, Cann A, Calhoun LG, Tedeschi RG. The factor structure of the posttraumatic growth inventory: a comparison of five models using confirmatory factor analysis. J. Trauma. Stress. 2008;21:158–64.
Brunet J, McDonough MH, Hadd V, Crocker PR, Sabiston CM. The posttraumatic growth inventory: an examination of the factor structure and invariance among breast cancer survivors. Psychooncology 2010;19:830–8.
Acknowledgements
Authors would like to express the special thanks to Dr. Lina Jansen for helping to improve the clarity of the manuscript.
Funding
This work was supported by a grant from the German Cancer Aid (No. 108262). ZL was supported by the China local research projects (No. QNRC052) and China Scholarship Council PhD program (ID number: 201808320419). The funding source was not involved in the collection, interpretation, or data analysis, or in the decision for the writing and submission of this report for publication. Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
ZL analysed the data and wrote the draft. MT and DD, reviewed and edited the paper. LKG was the study manager of the CAESAR study. HBe, AE, BH, AW, SRZ and RP contributed to recruitment of study participants and data collection. HBr and VA are the PIs of the CAESAR study. All authors read and approved the paper.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
This study was approved by the ethics committees of the University of Heidelberg and the cancer registries. All participants provided written informed consent.
Consent to publish
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Liu, Z., Thong, M.S.Y., Doege, D. et al. Prevalence of benefit finding and posttraumatic growth in long-term cancer survivors: results from a multi-regional population-based survey in Germany. Br J Cancer 125, 877–883 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41416-021-01473-z
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41416-021-01473-z
This article is cited by
-
Psychische Langzeitfolgen von Krebserkrankungen
Bundesgesundheitsblatt - Gesundheitsforschung - Gesundheitsschutz (2022)