Introduction

Greater overall psychological stress can have adverse effects on health and is associated with higher mortality [1]. Its association with cardiovascular disease [2], upper respiratory disease [3], symptom severity of rheumatoid arthritis [4], depressive symptoms [5] and other phenotypes [6, 7] is well documented. Several pathways have been proposed to link psychological stress with morbidity. First, psychological stress has been shown to result in poor health decisions and promote impulsive decision-making [8] such as increased consumption of nicotine or alcohol [9], other substance abuse [10] and sleep deprivation leading to an increased risk for numerous diseases [11]. Second, two major endocrine response pathways mediate the physiological response to psychological stress. First, catecholamines released by the sympathetic-adrenal-medullary (SAM) system affect the cardiovascular, the pulmonary, and the immune system and prepare the body to fight or flee if threatened [12]. Second, the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis (HPA) regulates the level of glucocorticoids which have immunosuppressive and anti-inflammatory effects and promote gluconeogenesis [13, 14]. Both systems, if activated repeatedly and for long durations, are known to increase the risk for disease [15,16,17]. This is partially mediated via downregulation of glucocorticoid receptors and a chronic state of inflammation [18].

Furthermore, the reactivity of the HPA axis to psychological stress increases with age [19]. Despite poorer physiological regulation in the face of stressors in older age, older adults might have advantages in both the overall exposure as well as emotional response to stressors [20, 21]. For instance, older adults are able to evade stressful situations more successfully than younger adults by using secondary coping or avoidance strategies [21, 22]. Importantly, however, in situations where older adults are confronted with an unavoidable stressor, these emotional advantages may become unfavorable in the face of physiological vulnerability, e.g., a disabling disease [21, 23].

How psychological stress impacts physical and mental health depends on numerous modifiable and non-modifiable factors [6]. Especially poorer health seems to increase vulnerability to stress-induced disease in older age [6]. One way of objectifying age-dependent biological vulnerability is through the measurement of biomarkers of aging. One promising biomarker in this domain is the determination of DNA methylation age (DNAm age) and, in particular, its deviation from chronological age, DNAm age acceleration (DNAmAA) [24]. Both markers are estimated from epigenetic clocks that utilize data on the methylated fraction of specific cytosin-phosphat-guanine (CpG) sites. Several such epigenetic clocks are available which differ in the way they were designed [25]. Epigenetic clocks of the first generation, e.g., 7-CpG clock, Horvath clock, and Hannum clock, were trained to predict chronological age. In contrast, second-generation clocks made use of phenotypic data that were used to identify and weigh the incorporated CpG sites. Studies aiming to reveal which phenotypic domains are represented best by each of the different epigenetic clock measures yielded partly different results in the past, and are still ongoing. To further explore similarities and differences between the epigenetic clocks and their characteristics, we chose to include and directly compare five different epigenetic age measures in this work allowing a comparison. Previous work has suggested that an association between psychological stress and DNAm age appears plausible due to the fact that 85 of the 353 CpG sites of the Horvath clock (and possibly CpG subfractions of other DNAm clocks as well) are located within glucocorticoid response elements (GRE) [26, 27]. These DNA sequences represent binding sites to glucocorticoid receptors and were shown to be at or near to CpG sites that were especially affected by glucocorticoid dependent demethylation mediated by demethylating enzymes and decreased expression of DNA methyltransferase [28, 29]. Furthermore, the number of CpG sites within GRE’s exceeds the amount that would be expected by chance [29].

Additionally, epigenetic changes were suggested to be a possible link [30, 31] between adverse childhood experiences and mortality as well as higher morbidity burden in late life [32]. It was proposed that this link could be mediated by health-adverse coping mechanisms (activated as a result of high levels of anxiety and depression) that are associated with adverse childhood experiences [33]. Some of these coping strategies, such as smoking, alcohol abuse and and a high BMI resulting from unhealthy eating habits, were shown to be associated with DNAmAA in some studies [34,35,36]. However, these results were not unequivocally replicated [37,38,39] (reviewed in ref. [40]).

Previous studies that examined the relationship between DNAmAA and stress operationalized stress as low socioeconomic status (SES) [41, 42], (childhood) trauma [26, 43,44,45], racial discrimination [46], or exposure to violence [47]. Many previous studies on the topic focused on changes in DNAm age during childhood as this period is known to be particularly prone to stress-related epigenetic changes [29].

In contrast, in this work we focus on older age which was shown to be the second most vulnerable phase in a person’s life in terms of epigenetics [29]. As epigenetic modifications remain even after the psychological stimulus has ceased there is the possibility of cumulating effects on the epigenome exerted by repeated psychological stressors [29]. Specifically, we analyzed the association between the amount of experienced stress (measured by Cohen’s Perceived Stress Scale [PSS] [48]) and several DNAm age estimators (i.e. the 7-CpG clock [49], Horvath’s clock [50], Hannum’s clock [51], PhenoAge [34], GrimAge [52]) in 1100 older adults. While the PSS represents a well-established marker of perceived stress [48], to our knowledge it has not been investigated in the context of epigenetic aging before. While we were able to replicate well-established associations with perceived stress, none of the five epigenetic clocks analyzed in the current study were associated with the perception of stress.

Methods

BASE-II/GendAge study

BASE-II is a longitudinal study that aims to identify factors that promote healthy aging. Participants were recruited through advertisements in local newspapers and on public transport in the greater Berlin area, Germany. At baseline examination (2009–2014), 2171 participants were medically examined (75% aged 60–84 years and 25% aged 20–37 years; this latter, younger group was not considered in the present work). In this study, we focus on the cross-sectional analysis of 1083 BASE-II participants of the older age group who were reexamined on average 7.4 years after baseline as part of the GendAge study. Seventeen additional BASE-II participants were available for follow-up that were not included in the medical baseline examination. For a more detailed cohort information at baseline and follow-up, please refer to Bertram et al. [53], Gerstorf et al. [54], and Demuth et al. [55].

All participants gave written informed consent. The medical assessments at baseline and follow-up were conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Charité—Universitätsmedizin Berlin (approval numbers EA2/029/09 and EA2/144/16). They were registered in the German Clinical Trials Registry as DRKS00009277 and DRKS00016157.

Measures

Perceived stress

Stress was assessed by eight items of the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS) that was developed by Cohen, Kamarck and Mermelstein in 1983 (items 1, 2, 3, 7, 8, 10, 11, and 14 of the original publication [48]). Participants answered the questions on a scale from 1 (“never”) to 5 (“very often”). The answers were averaged and z-transformed with R’s “scale” function for the linear regression analyses. Data on PSS was available for 1006 participants of the GendAge study.

DNA methylation age (DNAm age)

DNAm age was estimated by five epigenetic clocks. The 7-CpG clock was developed from methylation data obtained through methylation-sensitive single nucleotide primer extension (MS-SNuPE) from samples collected at baseline examination of the participants analyzed in this study [49] and replicated in separate cohorts [56, 57]. For a more detailed description of the methods used see ref. [58].

Additionally, DNAm age was estimated using Horvath’s clock [50], Hannum’s clock [51], PhenoAge [34] and GrimAge [52] from methylation data determined with the “Infinium MethylationEPIC” array (Illumina, Inc., USA). All functions to load and process DNAm data are from the R-package “Bigmelon” [59]. Briefly, outliers were identified with the outlyx function with a threshold of 0.15 for the myP parameter [60]. Additionally, samples with a bisulfite conversion efficiency below 80% (as estimated by the bscon function) were removed. Subsequently, the samples were reloaded with outliers excluded and normalized with the function dasen. The function qual was used to determine the extent of change in beta values in each sample due to normalization. Samples with a root-mean-square deviation of 0.1 or larger were removed and loading and normalization were repeated with the new sample set with removed outliers. The raw (not normalized) DNAm beta-values were uploaded to the DNAm age estimation website (https://horvath.genetics.ucla.edu/html/dnamage/) according to the instructions in the manual. On the EPIC array only 512 of the original 513 CpG sites and 64 of the original 71 CpG sites were available for the estimation of PhenoAge and Hannum’s clock, respectively. For more information on the protocol used to obtain DNAm data at follow-up, see ref. [58].

DNAm age acceleration (DNAmAA)

To correct for the well documented association between blood cell type composition and chronological age, we employed a blood cell count adjusted model to calculate DNAmAA [37, 61]. It was calculated as unstandardized residuals of a linear regression analysis of DNAm age on chronological age and leukocyte cell distribution (neutrophils, monocytes, lymphocytes, and eosinophils in G/l). Blood cell composition was measured by an accredited clinical biochemistry laboratory (MVZ Labor 28 GmbH, Berlin, Germany) using automated standard methods (flow cytometry).

Morbidity, depressive symptoms, and frailty measure

Morbidity burden was assessed using a modified version [62] of Charlson’s morbidity index [63]. Symptoms of depression were recorded with the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) [64]. A score of 16 or more points on the 0 to 60 point scale is used to identify individuals at risk for clinical depression [65] but the full scale was used in this study to make use of more subtle differences in depressive symptoms as well. Frailty was measured using Fried’s frailty phenotype [66] that incorporates unintentional weight loss, self-reported exhaustion, weakness (grip strength), slow walking speed (timed-up-and-go test), and low physical activity [67].

Covariates

We included the following covariates in all statistical models to account for potential confounding: Differences between sexes with respect to aging [68] and DNAmAA [38, 69], and the effect of psychological stress on disease [13] are well documented. Therefore, sex was included as covariate in all regression analyses. In addition, we performed sex-stratified analyses for all tests. Other covariates included were information on alcohol consumption (“yes”/“no”) and smoking behavior (packyears) which were assessed in one-to-one interviews by trained study personnel. The body mass index (BMI) was calculated using electronic height and weight measurements (via a “seca 763” measuring station, SECA, GERMANY). Educational attainment was assessed as education years until highest degree [70]; this information was available for 994 participants. Lastly, we controlled for genetic ancestry by using the first four principal components from a principal component analysis on genome-wide SNP genotyping data generated in the same individuals. In brief, these SNP genotype data were generated using the “Global Screening Array” (GSA) with shared custom content (Illumina, Inc.). These data were processed and QC’ed using the same workflows and criteria outlined in ref. [71]. This entailed processing of the raw data, i.e. clustering and genotype calling from raw intensity data (idat format), which was performed in GenomeStudio software (v2.0.4; Illumina, Inc.) using the genotyping module (version 2.0.2). Samples with call rate <0.95 and p50GC < 0.7 were excluded at this stage. We then used PLINK software (v1.9) to perform additional QC filtering, i.e. sex checks (--check-sex 0.25 0.75), strand check (--flip), missing genotype rate (--geno 0.02; --mind 0.05), Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (HWE) tests (--hwe 0.000005), and minor allele frequency (MAF) filtering (--maf 0.01). For determining pairwise allele sharing (to identify cryptic relatedness), we used an LD-pruned set of markers (--indep-pairwise 1500 150 0.2). Pairwise allele-sharing IBD/IBS was determined using (--Z-genome --min 0.1). The LD-pruned dataset was also used for principal component analysis (PCA; using PLINK command ‘--pca’) along with the reference dataset of the 1000 Genome Project Consortium Phase 3 to assign ethnic descent groups using the five 1000 G super-populations by k-nearest neighbor (k-NN; k = 9) classification (using R-package ‘class’ in R 2.3.2). Only “European descent” samples were used in the subsequent statistical analyses; these corrected for the first four ancestry depending principal components as covariates.

Statistical analyses

All statistical analyses were executed in R 3.6.2 [72] and code is available from the authors upon request. Linear regression analyses were performed using the “lm” function, and all figures were produced with the “ggplot2” package [73].

Participants were only excluded from an analysis if they were missing a variable required for the respective analysis (available case analysis). We indicate the number of observations for each analysis individually. A p-value below 0.05 was considered statistically significant.

Results

Sample characteristics

Cross-sectional data on 1100 participants were available. Included participants were between 64.9 and 94.1 years old (mean age: 75.6 years, SD = 3.8 years, 52.1% female, Table 1). Perception of stress, assessed as averaged answer on eight items of Cohen’s Perceived Stress Scale (PSS), was normally distributed (Fig. 1) and no sex-difference was found (t-test, p = 0.08, Supplementary Table 1). Men had statistically significant higher DNAmAA in all five available epigenetic clocks (t-test, p ≤ 0.001, Supplementary Table 1). This sex-difference was reported before in this dataset [74] as well as in others [38, 69].

Table 1 Cohort characteristics of 1100 BASE-II participants of the GendAge study.
Fig. 1: Distribution of Cohen’s PSS in GendAge participants (n = 1006).
figure 1

Please note that items four and six (respectively items seven and ten of original publication by Cohen and colleagues) were reversed before inclusion in the final PSS. PSS Perceived Stress Scale.

Association between Cohen’s PSS and depressive symptoms, morbidity, and frailty

In a first step, we tested for known associations between psychological stress and clinical phenotypes. To improve the interpretability of the PSS, it was z-transformed prior to the inclusion in linear regression models. The known associations between stress and depressive symptoms, morbidity and frailty were also observed in this dataset (Table 2). Specifically, an increase of one standard deviation on the PSS was associated with 0.3 points higher morbidity index, 0.2 points higher frailty score, and 0.8 points higher scores on the CES-D, after adjustment for all covariates. These associations persisted in sex-stratified analyses and seemed to be particularly pronounced in women, as the morbidity index and the CES-D showed a higher effect size in this subgroup compared to men (Supplementary Table 2).

Table 2 Multiple linear regression analyses of morbidity index, frailty score or CES-D on Cohen’s PSS in older BASE-II participants of the GendAge study.

Relationship between DNAmAA and Cohen’s PSS

The potential relationship between Cohen’s PSS and the various DNAmAA parameters was assessed by multiple linear regression models. The fully adjusted model included sex, smoking, alcohol, BMI, education, and genetic ancestry as covariates (Model 3).

While weak associations were observed in the unadjusted (Model 1) and sex-adjusted model (Model 2) between PSS and Horvath’s DNAmAA (ß = –0.27, SE = 0.13, p = 0.04, n = 976, Model 2) and PSS and PhenoAge DNAmAA (ß = −0.37, SE = 0.17, p = 0.04, n = 976, Model 2), these were no longer significant in the full model (Model 3, p > 0.2, Table 3). Although no statistically significant association between PSS and 7-CpG, Hannum’s and GrimAge DNAmAA were found in unadjusted linear regression models, the coefficients pointed in the same negative direction. This was true for the sex-adjusted model (Model 2) as well, except for the relationship between PSS and GrimAge DNAmAA, were the beta-coefficient was slightly positive (ß = 0.013, p = 0.899, Model 2). Like the results reported for the full dataset, sex-stratified subgroup analyses revealed no statistically significant associations after adjustment for covariates (Supplementary Table 3).

Table 3 Multiple linear regression analyses of Cohen’s PSS on DNAmAA of five epigentic clocks and covariates.

Discussion

In this study, we report data on perceived stress as assessed by the Cohen’s PSS and the biomarker DNAmAA as derived from five different epigenetic clocks in a comparatively large sample of older adults. Overall, we found no noteworthy associations between our marker of psychological stress and DNAmAA estimated by any of the five employed epigenetic clocks.

While our study is not the first on the topic, comparability with previous work is limited due to substantial differences in cohort characteristics and in quantification of stress as well as DNAmAA. Most previous studies focused on associations between retrospectively assessed life adversities during childhood and DNAmAA in comparatively young cohorts (with a mean age of 50 years or younger, reviewed in ref. [32]). A particular impact of psychological stress that was (retrospectively remembered as having been) experienced during childhood and adolescence on epigenetic changes was shown and explained by an high vulnerability to epigenetic changes during early age [29].

In contrast, only very few studies examined psychological stress during adulthood and its impact on epigenetic aging. In these studies, stress was most often operationalized as low socioeconomic status (SES) (overview in Supplementary Table 4). Similar to childhood and adolescence, older adults were reported to be especially prone to stress-related epigenetic changes, mostly due to a decline of the epigenetic maintenance system [29]. For instance, low income was associated with higher DNAmAA (using Hannum’s clock estimate) in a cohort of 100 black women (mean age 48.5 years) [39]. In a different study, Fiorito and colleagues found several measures for low socioeconomic status in a meta-analysis of three cohorts from Italy, Australia and Ireland (n = 5111, mean age: 57.28 years) to be associated with Horvath’s and Hannum’s DNAmAA [75]. In contrast to these results, Hughes and colleagues found no association between current SES and Horvath’s or Hannum’s DNAmAA in a cohort of 1099 participants with a mean age of 58.4 years [76]. The same was true for women assessed in two waves of the ALSPAC study (mean age: 28.7 and 47.4 years) and the NSHD study (mean age: 53.4 years) and Horvath DNAmAA [77]. To our knowledge, the oldest cohort analyzed in this context consisted of 490 women and men between 50 and 87 years of age (mean age: 62.2 years) [42]. This study by McCrory and colleagues is the only one that employs not only first-generation clocks (trained to predict chronological age) but also examines PhenoAge, a second-generation clock that aims to predict biological (phenotypic) age measures [34]. Still, no association was observed between SES (assessed as life course social class trajectory, education, and income) and DNAmAA derived from PhenoAge, Horvath’s clock or Hannum’s clock and SES in this study, either [42].

The lack of a statistically significant association between PSS and epigenetic aging in this study might be the result of several factors. First, the well-established stress marker employed here assesses perceived stress over the course of the last month before the examination. Although cortisol-mediated short-term changes in the epigenome are known, they might not be distinctive enough to translate into a detectable change in DNAmAA. It is unclear how the PSS corresponds with chronic stress in our cohort, which is often made responsible for the stress-associated adverse effects on physical and mental health [26, 27]. However, we were able to show that the PSS is associated with several relevant clinical phenotypes, such as morbidity burden, frailty, and symptoms of depression. Therefore, it seems likely that the stress marker used here does serve as a proxy of more longterm psychological stress of our participants. Second, we cannot rule out that we may have missed covariates of relevance in our regression analyses. However, this is a limitation applicable to most studies examining epigenetic markers. Furthermore, we note that we performed a detailed literature search on the topic and did not identify any additional covariates of relevance in the screened papers. Third, we cannot rule out the presence of selection/recruitment bias. The sample analyzed here is characterized by its above-average health status at baseline [53, 74]. Similarly, the average PSS scores reflected a generally low stress level, which might has impacted our results. Although we can only speculate on the reasons for these findings, a high stress level seems to be among the plausible reasons that would prevent one from voluntarily participating in a study. Finally, the lack of statistically significant findings could be the result of our sample size. However, it is unlikely that a higher number of analyzed participants would reveal clinically relevant associations as our sample size was shown to be sufficient to detect even small effect sizes in a power analysis (f2 = 0.02, power = 0.8, alpha = 0.05). Nevertheless, it would be of interest to repeat this analysis in an even larger sample of older participants who perceive a higher level of stress.

Strengths of this study include the usage of a well-established instrument to measure perceived stress (PSS), and the application of five different DNAm algorithms (both first- and second-generation) based on two molecular methods (MS-SNuPE and EPIC array). Despite the lack of a significant association here, further studies using individuals in a comparable age range are needed to better understand the short- and long-term consequences of acute and chronic psychological stress on biological and epigenetic age. In addition, it may be interesting to analyze biological and epigenetic age as a potential risk factor for stronger stress responses in daily life. This could help explain individual differences among participants which we observe as association between perceived stress and several health-relevant clinical outcomes.

Conclusion

Although previous studies suggest an effect of childhood trauma on DNAmAA, the situation is less clear on the potential association between psychological stress and DNAmAA during adulthood and advanced age. In the nearly 1000 individuals aged 64.9 years and above, we did not observe evidence for a noteworthy association between psychological stress and epigenetic aging as measured by five different epigenetic clocks.