Introduction

Psychotic experiences (also called “psychotic-like experiences”) resemble positive symptoms of psychotic disorders, such as paranoia, hallucinations, and cognitive symptoms, and have a median prevalence rate of ~7%–8% in the general population1,2. Negative symptom traits in the community resemble negative symptoms of psychotic disorders such as apathy, anhedonia, and social withdrawal. Schizotypy3 is a related, older personality-based construct compared with psychotic experiences and negative symptoms (PENS). Positive psychotic experiences during adolescence or adulthood, especially when persistent, are associated with an increased risk of developing psychotic disorders4,5,6,7,8,9 and, to a lesser extent, with other psychiatric disorders10,11,12. Likewise, schizotypy is associated with subsequently developing psychotic disorders13.

Twin studies suggest heritability accounts for a third to a half of variation in PENS during adolescence14,15,16,17,18,19. Genome-wide association studies (GWAS) indicate modest single-nucleotide polymorphism heritability (SNP-h2) for some PENS in mid-adolescence (3%–9%) and for schizotypy in adults (20%–27%)20,21,22. Psychotic experiences share genome-wide genetic influences with schizophrenia and major depression20,23,24,25,26, although not all studies found this, particularly those that used comparatively smaller samples or polygenic scores (PGS) from less well-powered GWAS23,24,27,28,29,30,31. Schizophrenia PGS has been associated with schizotypy in adults assessed using semi-structured interviews, but not with self-rated PENS32. Studies have mainly focussed on adolescents and young adults rather than older adults and have not reported on the genetic overlap of psychotic experiences across age, despite this being an important topic for understanding the etiology and development of mental illness. It is also not known whether psychotic experiences and schizotypy share genetic influences.

This study investigates the genetic overlap between psychotic experiences across age using the largest current GWAS summary statistics available for adolescent PENS and for schizotypy and positive psychotic experiences measured in adulthood. We evaluate genome-wide genetic correlations and overlapping associated genes between these trait measures and with schizophrenia, depression, and bipolar disorder. For traits and disorders that share common additive genetic influences, we further explore the nature of these associations using Mendelian randomization (MR).

Methods and materials

Samples and measures

PENS traits during adolescence

Summary statistics for adolescent PENS came from a mega-GWAS of three European community samples (N = 6297–10,098)20: Twins Early Developmental Study (TEDS)33, a community sample born between 1994–1996 in England and Wales (mean age 16.32 years); Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC)34,35, a birth cohort from the United Kingdom (mean age 16.76 years) born in 1991–1992; and Child and Adolescent Twin Study in Sweden (CATSS)36 that recruited twins born in Sweden since 1992 (mean age 18.31 years).

In TEDS, PENS items came from the Specific Psychotic Experiences Questionnaire37 and were matched by a team of clinicians to similar items from psychopathology questionnaires available in CATSS and ALSPAC20. After harmonization, PENS included four continuous subscales that assessed the frequency or severity of paranoia and hallucinations, cognitive disorganization, anhedonia, and parent-rated negative symptoms.

Schizotypy during adulthood

Schizotypy was assessed in the Northern Finland Birth Cohort 1996 (NFBC)38 when participants were aged 31 years. GWAS summary statistics22 of four continuous schizotypy scales were included (N = 3967–4057): perceptual aberrations were assessed with the Perceptual Aberration Scale39 and included experiences that resemble clinical features of schizophrenia. Hypomania was from the Hypomanic Personality Scale40, devised to assess hypomania, gregariousness, grandiosity, and euphoria. Two scales from Chapman’s Schizotypia Scales were employed: the Revised Social Anhedonia Scale and the Revised Physical Anhedonia Scale41, devised to assess the inability to take pleasure from physical and social stimuli, respectively.

Positive psychotic experiences assessed in adults

GWAS summary statistics of four dichotomous items from the UK Biobank were obtained from Neale Lab (http://www.nealelab.is/uk-biobank) for individuals of European ancestry (N = 116,787–117,794). Items assessed psychotic experiences in adults aged 40–69 years: whether participants ever experienced auditory hallucinations, visual hallucinations, delusions of persecution, and delusions of reference.

Additional information on psychotic experiences items are provided in the Supplementary Note.

Psychiatric disorders

GWAS summary statistics for schizophrenia42, bipolar disorder43, and major depressive disorder44 were downloaded from the Psychiatric Genetics Consortium (https://www.med.unc.edu/pgc/results-and-downloads). Summary statistics for depression excluded 23andMe participants.

Analyses

Quality-control procedures were applied to summary statistics prior to analyses. Genetic variants were removed if they had incomplete association statistics, were non-biallelic, or strand ambiguous. Variants with alleles that did not match those in the 1000 Genomes reference panel (phase 3), with info scores < 0.9 and minor allele frequency < 0.01 were excluded.

Linkage disequilibrium (LD) score regression45 was used to estimate SNP-h2 for traits and the genetic correlation (rg) between traits. Variants were merged with the HapMap3 reference panel to ensure good imputation quality45. Heritability and genetic correlations were converted to liability scales using lifetime prevalence of 1% for schizophrenia, 15% for depression, and 2% for bipolar disorder46,47,48. Effective sample sizes were used for adolescent PENS, because TEDS and CATSS included siblings20. To account for sample overlap, the LD score regression intercept was left unconstrained to estimate associations between positive psychotic experiences and depression, because both included UK Biobank participants, and between psychiatric disorders that include PGC participants. Benjamini–Hochberg correction for multiple testing was performed at a false discovery rate of 0.05 to account for 105 correlations estimated.

Gene-wide associations and gene mapping were performed using the FUMA pipeline49 and results were compared across psychotic experiences and psychiatric disorders. To identify genes at genome-wide significance, MAGMA v1.0650 was used to aggregate the p-values of SNPs within gene coding regions (0 kb annotation window). Default parameters and an SNP-wide mean model were employed. Bonferroni-corrected p-value thresholds were set for the number of genes tested within each phenotype. Positional mapping of variants within 10 kb of gene regions and likely to have functional consequences (Combined Annotation-Dependent Depletion score (CADD score) ≥ 12.37), gene mapping using expression quantitative trait loci associations and chromatin interactions were performed for independent lead SNPs (at p < 1 × 10–5 for psychotic experiences, p < 1 × 10−6 for depression, and p < 1 × 10−8 for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder) and 1000 Genomes reference panel variants in LD with independent SNPs at r2 ≥ 0.6 using recommended parameters49. Additional details are provided in the Supplementary Note.

MR51 was conducted to test for causal relationships between phenotypes that had significant genetic correlations. SNPs were selected as instrumental variables based on the clumping algorithm in PLINK52 using an r2 threshold of 0.05 within a 500 kb window at genome-wide significant levels (p < 5 × 10−8) for schizophrenia. Summary statistics for depression did not include 23andMe participants and had an insufficient number of genome-wide significant variants for MR analyses. Instead, genome-wide significant variants were obtained from the publication44. Insufficient genome-wide significant variants associated with psychotic experiences meant that p-value thresholds for psychotic experiences were set at p < 5 × 10−5 to allow for at least 20 instrumental variables. We report MR analyses using equivalent p-value thresholds for all phenotypes (p < 5 × 10−5) in the Supplementary Material.

Generalized summary-data-based MR (GSMR) was used due to its advantages of accounting for residual LD structure between instrumental variables (set at LD r2 > 0.1) and for sampling variation in the exposure and outcome GWAS53. MR-Egger regression54, weighted median55, and weighted mode56 methods were conducted as sensitivity analyses for possible violation of MR assumptions. SNPs identified as potentially pleiotropic or with residual LD structure in GSMR Heidi-outlier analyses were also excluded from sensitivity analyses.

Results

Common additive genetic variance accounted for 8%–10% of phenotypic variation in adolescent PENS (paranoia/hallucinations and negative symptoms did not have significant SNP-h2 estimates in these analyses), 30%–37% in schizotypy during adulthood, and 7%–10% in positive psychotic experiences during adulthood (Table 1).

Table 1 Genome-wide association study sample sizes and SNP-heritability estimates.

Genetic correlations are shown in Fig. 1. Genetic correlations could not be computed for paranoia and hallucinations, and negative symptoms, likely due to low SNP-h2. For these comparisons, we report genetic covariance (ρg), which indicates direction of correlation (Supplementary Table S1).

Fig. 1: Heat map showing genetic correlations between psychotic experiences and psychiatric disorders.
figure 1

PENS, Psychotic experiences (PE) and negative symptom traits; NA, genetic correlations could not be computed due to low SNP heritability or sample size (see Supplementary Table S1 for genetic covariance estimates). *Nominally statistically significant genetic correlations at p < .05; **genetic correlations that survived Benjamini–Hochberg correction for multiple testing for 105 pairwise correlations (at a FDR of 0.05); genetic correlations reported using unconstrained LD score regression intercept between phenotypes with sample overlap.

Genetic correlations with psychiatric disorders

For positive psychotic experiences in UK Biobank, auditory hallucinations were significantly genetically correlated with schizophrenia (rg = 0.39, p = 2.27 × 10−5) and depression (rg = 0.69, p = 8.07 × 10−6), but not with bipolar disorder (rg = 0.17, p = 0.082). Likewise, visual hallucinations was significantly correlated with schizophrenia (rg = 0.27, p = 4.12 × 10−7) and depression (rg = 0.41, p = 2.00 × 10−4) but not with bipolar disorder (rg = 0.14, p = 0.149). We observed high and significant genetic correlations between delusions of persecution with schizophrenia (rg = 0.67, p = 0.002) and depression (rg = 0.96, p = 0.001), whereas the genetic correlation with bipolar disorder was not significant after correction for multiple testing (rg = 0.65, p = .019). Genetic correlations between delusions of reference with schizophrenia (rg = 0.72, p = 0.007), depression (rg = 0.79, p = 0.021) and bipolar disorder (rg = 0.61, p = 0.033) were at nominal significance (p < 0.05) but did not survive correction for multiple testing.

Between adolescent PENS and psychiatric disorders, genetic correlations were not significant after correction for multiple testing. Nominally significant genetic correlations were observed between cognitive disorganization with schizophrenia (rg = 0.19, p = 0.034) and depression (rg = 0.40, p = 0.006) and between anhedonia and depression (rg = 0.33, p = 0.021). Negative symptoms covaried positively with schizophrenia (ρg = 0.03, p = 1.37 × 10−4) and depression (ρg = 0.03, p = 2.41 × 10−7). Positive genetic covariation was observed between paranoia and hallucinations, and depression (ρg = 0.03, p = 1.43 × 10−4). No significant genetic correlations were found with bipolar disorder.

Between psychiatric disorders and schizotypy in adults, hypomania was genetically correlated with depression at nominal significance (rg = 0.17, p = 0.014). We did not find evidence that the schizotypy scales correlated with schizophrenia or bipolar disorder.

Genetic stability of psychotic experiences across age

PENS during adolescence were not significantly genetically correlated with positive psychotic experiences and schizotypy during adulthood. Between the adult samples, hypomania was genetically correlated with visual hallucinations (rg = 0.37, p = 0.024). Physical anhedonia showed a negative genetic correlation with auditory hallucinations (rg = −0.42, p = 0.034), but not after correction for multiple testing.

Genetic correlations within samples

Within positive psychotic experiences in UK Biobank, auditory and visual hallucinations were highly and significantly genetically correlated (rg = 0.95, p = 7.61 × 10−5). Genetic correlations between delusions of reference with auditory (rg = 0.93, p = 0.038) and visual hallucinations (rg = 0.83, p = 0.050) were not significant after correction for multiple testing. Delusions of persecution were not genetically correlated with auditory or visual hallucinations. A genetic correlation between delusions of persecution and delusions of reference could not be computed due to low SNP-h2 in both phenotypes. but these two items showed positive genetic covariance (ρg = 0.01, p = 0.003).

There was a nominally significant association between physical and social anhedonia (rg = 0.54, p = 0.044). No other genetic correlations between schizotypy scales were significant.

Within adolescent PENS scales, positive genetic covariance was identified between anhedonia and negative symptoms (ρg = 0.11, p = 0.012). No significant genetic overlap between other adolescent PENS was found.

Comparison of implicated genes across mapping strategies and phenotypes

Gene mapping and genome-wide gene association results from FUMA were compared across psychotic experiences and psychiatric disorders (Table 2). Full results for each phenotype are provided in Supplementary Figs. S13 and Supplementary Tables S27. Results revealed 32 genes for adolescent PENS, of which PAN3 mapped to adolescent cognitive disorganization and schizotypy in adulthood (perceptual aberrations), and NADK2 to adolescent negative symptoms and delusions of reference in UK Biobank; none overlapped with psychiatric disorders. Seventy-three genes were found for adult schizotypy including the aforementioned PAN3 and six genes that were also indicated for schizophrenia. ANK3 overlapped with both positive psychotic experiences (visual hallucinations) and schizophrenia. For positive psychotic experiences in adults, 104 genes were identified, seven of which overlapped with schizophrenia and, as mentioned above, one with schizotypy and one with adolescent PENS (Fig. 2).

Table 2 Gene-associations and gene-mapping results for overlapping genes across phenotypes.
Fig. 2: Number of overlapping genes between psychotic experiences across age and psychiatric disorders.
figure 2

PENS, Psychotic experiences (PE) and negative symptom traits. Genes identified using (a) genome-wide gene associations in MAGMA after Bonferroni correction for the number of gene associations tested, (b) positional mapping that prioritized genes based on variant functional annotations obtained using ANNOVAR, (c) eQTL (expressive quantitative trait) mapping, and (d) chromatin interaction mapping.

Mendelian randomization

Results from GSMR analyses and MR sensitivity analyses are summarized in Table 3, and MR-Egger intercept tests, heterogeneity statistics, and plots in Supplementary Table S8 and Supplementary Figs. S45.

Table 3 Mendelian randomization analyses.

GSMR analyses provided evidence for a bidirectional association between auditory and visual hallucinations (PGSMR = 1.73 × 10−21 for auditory hallucinations as the exposure; PGSMR = 4.25 × 10−25 for visual hallucinations as the exposure) and replicated in all MR sensitivity analyses.

We observed evidence of a directional effect of schizophrenia liability on delusions of persecution (PGSMR = 9.33 × 10−5); however, the effect was small and replicated in weighted median but not in MR-Egger, nor in weighted mode analyses. No evidence of a significant effect in the other direction was found.

Only the MR-Egger method indicated significant directional effects of a liability to depression on a propensity to report auditory hallucinations and delusions of persecution. However, MR-Egger intercept tests indicated the presence of directional pleiotropy in both instances (Supplementary Table S8), indicating that MR-Egger results may be reliable as it adjusts for non-zero intercepts allowing for more robust estimates when horizontal pleiotropy is present compared with the other methods. The discrepancy between MR methods may also be due to outliers not identified by Heidi-outlier analysis. MR analyses using IVs selected at p < 5 × 10−5 for all exposures did not affect our main conclusions (Supplementary Table S9).

Discussion

This study investigated whether common genetic variation underlying psychotic experiences overlaps across adolescence and adulthood, and with psychiatric disorders. Our results suggest that with increasing age from adolescence to adulthood, psychotic experiences become more linked genetically to schizophrenia and major depression. We did not observe evidence of significant genetic stability of psychotic experiences across adolescence and adulthood in existing data. However, our study implicated specific genes that are associated with psychotic experiences across development and genes shared between psychotic experiences and psychiatric disorders.

Associations between psychotic experiences and psychiatric disorders

We found that positive psychotic experiences in UK Biobank (specifically experiences of auditory and visual hallucinations) shared a moderate to substantial amount of common genetic variation with depression and schizophrenia, consistent with an independent report26. Our MR analyses indicated that liability to schizophrenia may be directionally associated with a propensity to experience delusions of persecution and depression liability with a propensity to report auditory hallucinations and delusions of persecution. This latter finding concurs with a twin study showing that depression and psychotic experiences phenotypically influence one another in adolescence over and above genetic influences57. However, these MR effect sizes were small and not consistently replicated in sensitivity analyses, and are therefore unlikely to reflect true causal effects.

This study reported genetic overlap between adolescent PENS and psychiatric disorders. We used more recent summary statistics from larger GWAS of depression and bipolar disorder43,44, which allowed more reliable estimations compared with previous studies20. Our results add to the growing body of evidence that there are genetic associations between adolescent PENS and psychiatric disorders20,24. There were more significant genetic correlations between psychotic experiences and depression than with schizophrenia. This could be explored further in terms of the underlying reasons.

Schizotypy domains did not appear to share common additive genetic influences with schizophrenia nor with bipolar disorder. There was suggestive evidence of genetic overlap between hypomania and depression. Schizotypy has been associated with an increased risk of schizophrenia-spectrum disorders13, but longitudinal evidence of an association between schizotypy and subsequently being diagnosed with schizophrenia is limited58 and findings inconsistent59.

Bipolar disorder does not appear to share genetic overlap with adolescent PENS or adult schizotypy. It is possible that common genetic influences on bipolar disorder, in contrast to those on depression and schizophrenia, are not involved in these earlier adolescent forms of psychopathology. Future studies with better-powered GWAS will be able to test this hypothesis further.

We found evidence that some genes may be involved in both psychotic experiences and psychiatric disorders. Seven genes were implicated in both positive psychotic experiences in UK Biobank and schizophrenia: ANK3, a gene in the 10q21.2 region, has previously been associated with bipolar disorder and schizophrenia60,61,62. TSNARE1 (8q24.3) has previously been implicated in schizophrenia and cognitive function42,63. The gene SNX7 (1p21.3) has known associations with mathematical ability64. Three neighboring genes located on band 12q24.11, FAM216A, PPP1CC, and GPN3, and a nearby gene TMEM116 (12q24.13) have been associated with white blood cell and platelet count, heart rate, and body mass index65,66,67.

Six mapped genes overlapped between adult schizotypy and psychiatric disorders, including ANK3 discussed above. As noted elsewhere22, we found evidence that CACNA1C (12p13.33), with well-replicated associations with psychiatric and developmental disorders42,60,61, may be involved in hypomania, schizophrenia, and bipolar disorder. Located within the same 12p13.33 region, the genes DCP1B, TSPAN9, and FKBP4 also overlapped between hypomania and schizophrenia, and have previously been associated with a range of physical health phenotypes including obesity and waist–hip ratio66,68. Findings indicated the possible involvement of CLK1 (2q33.1) in perceptual aberrations and schizophrenia, a gene previously associated with breast cancer69.

Our results suggest that psychotic experiences may share more genome-wide genetic overlap with schizophrenia and major depression in adulthood than adolescence. Participants had narrow age ranges in the adolescent PENS (15–19 years old) and adult schizotypy (all born in the same year) samples, and were assessed on current psychotic experiences, whereas UK Biobank participants had a wider and older age range (40–69 years old), and were asked to report on lifetime psychotic experiences. It is therefore possible that psychotic experiences reported in UK Biobank may have been persistent rather than transitory. According to the persistent-impairment model, those with persistent psychotic experiences are at a higher risk of developing psychotic disorders and may therefore share more genetic influences with psychiatric disorders70. It is also possible that some of those in the adolescent samples may be genetically liable to psychotic experiences but will have not yet developed them. A previous study found that genetic liability to schizophrenia was positively associated with adolescent paranoia and hallucinations only after excluding non-zero scores20, suggesting that genetic liability to schizophrenia may vary in adolescents who did not (yet) report psychotic experiences. This may explain why we found that genetic overlap between psychotic experiences and psychiatric disorders increased with age.

Stability of genetic influences on psychotic experiences across age

We did not find significant genetic overlap across PENS (adolescence), schizotypy (adulthood), and positive psychotic experiences (adulthood). Different genetic influences may be involved in psychotic experiences across the lifespan. For example, some psychotic experiences may be adolescent-specific, tied in with development and lifestyle factors such as sleep problems, risk-taking, and experimenting with drugs. Psychotic experiences in older adults may have different etiological pathways, in part associated with conditions such as medication side effects, dementia, eye conditions, and Charles Bonnet syndrome. However, it was not possible to rule out that the different measurements used across ages led to the lack of genetic overlap. Another reason why no cross-age genetic overlap was found may be the smaller GWAS sample sizes for adolescent PENS and adult schizotypy. In agreement with our findings, an independent report found that genetic risk for psychotic experiences in adults did not predict psychotic experiences during mid-adolescence26.

We found that some of the same genes may be involved in psychotic experiences across age. The gene ANK3 was implicated for adult schizotypy and adult psychotic experiences. The gene PAN3 (13q12.2) was implicated in adolescent cognitive disorganization and schizotypy. NADK2 (mitochondrial) on 5p13.2 was mapped to both negative symptoms in adolescence and to adult psychotic experiences.

Within-sample associations between specific types of psychotic experiences

We also investigated within-sample genetic associations and found that auditory and visual hallucinations during adulthood shared the same common genetic influences and were bidirectionally associated in MR analyses. This bidirectional association may indicate a pervasive shared genetic basis between auditory and visual hallucinatory experiences rather than true causal associations, because pleiotropic effects of the genetic instruments through a latent common precursor (which would violate MR assumptions) seem likely. As such, experiences resembling auditory and visual hallucinations may share biological pathways.

Limitations

We note caution in interpreting MR results that compared within-sample psychotic experiences, because sample overlap could lead to weak instrument bias away from the null71. We may not have detected true causal effects due to weak instrument bias in analyses that used psychotic experiences as the exposure, because genetic variants below conventional genome-wide significance levels were used. Future studies could re-evaluate these causal associations once larger GWAS for psychotic experiences become available. Furthermore, future studies with greater power might explore the associations of psychotic experiences with a wider array of phenotypes.

Many of the overlapping genes we report were identified based mainly on chromatin interactions. The chromatin interaction data had a high resolution (10 kb) resulting in more mapped genes and included enhancer–promoter and promoter–promoter interactions only72, therefore providing strong hypotheses for variant–gene associations. For psychotic experiences and depression, functional consequences on genes were annotated to variants clumped below conventional genome-wide significance levels. As such, these results will benefit from replication once better-powered GWAS become available.

There are known limitations when using summary statistics from large biobanks including ascertainment bias and the possibility that collider bias may result in overinflated genetic correlations73,74. Loss to follow-up in the cohort studies used in GWAS may have an impact on the generalizability of the genetic findings75. However, we note that attrition based on phenotypic or genetic risk for psychosis would lead, if anything, to underestimates of the genetic overlap between psychotic experience traits and psychiatric disorders.

Implications and conclusions

Psychotic experiences during adulthood and adolescence share genetic influences with psychiatric disorders. This study implicated specific genes that may be involved in both psychotic experiences in the community and psychiatric disorders, and genes associated with psychotic experiences across development. Subclinical experiences of auditory and visual hallucinations in adults may have similar biological etiologies. Our findings and other independent reports indicate that psychotic experiences assessed using different measures at different developmental stages may not reflect genetically similar phenomena.