Main

Although survival rates following extremely preterm (EP, <28 wk gestation) birth have increased in the last few decades, significant neurodevelopmental sequelae affects around 25–35% of survivors (1). These deficits encompass poorer general intelligence (IQ), academic underachievement, behavioral, attentional, socialization problems, and major motor deficits (1,2,3).

Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies have contributed substantially to the understanding and characterization of preterm brain injury. Brain alterations in preterm children noted on structural (4,5) and diffusion tensor MRI (6) have been shown to correlate with cognitive, motor, and behavioral outcomes in childhood and adolescence (4,5). Brain metabolites measured using magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS) may provide insights into the structural changes observed in volumetric and diffusion MRI studies. Using proton (1H) MRS, metabolites including N-acetylaspartate (NAA), choline-containing compounds (Cho), and creatine (Cr) can be quantified. NAA is primarily found in neurons and is a marker of neuronal integrity (7). The Cho peak on MRS has a contribution from several choline-containing compounds. High Cho levels are reported in pathologies involving increased cellular density and proliferation of membrane phospholipids (8). The Cr resonance comprises creatine and phosphocreatine, compounds involved in cellular energy metabolism (9). Cr levels remain relatively stable after the first year following birth (10) and are often used as a reference in MRS ratios. Although MRS metabolites are presented either as ratios or absolute concentrations, it is not always feasible to obtain the latter in clinical studies due to time constraints related to the imaging protocol. Most brain MRS studies in preterm populations have been performed during the neonatal period (34 wk postmenstrual age up to term-equivalent age) (11,12,13). There is limited information on MRS brain metabolites and associated functional outcomes in preterm young adults (14,15).

The posterior cingulate cortex is a highly connected brain region with diverse functions that encompass IQ, working memory, attention, and executive function (16), all of which are domains that are impaired in the preterm population (3,17,18). Abnormalities of posterior cingulate brain structure, metabolic activity, and function have been documented in neurodegenerative disorders, healthy ageing, major psychiatric disorders, and traumatic brain injury (16). However, how posterior cingulate metabolites are affected following EP birth is not known.

To address these research gaps, we aimed to understand the impact of EP birth on posterior cingulate brain metabolites in adolescence, and how the metabolites relate to function. We aimed to (i) compare MRS metabolite ratios that reflect neuronal integrity and cell membranes, i.e., total (t)NAA/tCr and tCho/tCr between EP and controls, and (ii) explore the relationship between MRS metabolite ratios and IQ, working memory and attention at age 18 y and whether these relationships differ between EP and controls. Given that we have previously reported an association between larger brain white matter volumes and higher IQ (5), we also wanted to explore whether the associations between MRS metabolite ratios and cognitive outcomes were independent of white matter volume in this region. We hypothesized that the EP group would have lower tNAA/tCR and tCho/tCr compared with term controls, and that higher tNAA/tCr within the posterior cingulate would predict better IQ, working memory and attention, independent of white matter volume.

Results

For the 18-y follow-up study, 172 (76% of those alive at 8 y of age) of the EP group and 160 (63%) of the term-control group were seen, of whom 150 EP and 134 controls had MRS data. The mean Cramer-Rao low bounds (expressed as a percentage of the fitted metabolite amplitude) for the included spectra were: tCho 7.8 ± 3.1%; tNaa 3.0 ± 0.9%; tCr 4.0 ± 1.0%.

Compared with EP survivors in the original cohort, EP participants who contributed MRS data to this study had higher IQ at 8 y (mean difference (95% CI) of 7.0 (1.9, 12.1)), lower rates of cerebral palsy at 8 y (7.5 vs. 25%) and less cystic periventricular leukomalacia (3.3 vs. 14.7%). Controls with MRS data had higher IQ at 8 y (mean difference (95% CI) of 6.2 (2.4, 10.1)), were more likely to be singletons (100 vs. 95%), and were less likely to have mothers with fewer than 12 y of education (32.3 vs. 46.4%) than controls without MRS data. All other characteristics were similar.

Participant characteristics of those included in the current study are summarized in Table 1 . Expected differences between groups were seen in all characteristics with the exception of male sex and small for gestational age at birth.

Table 1 Participant characteristics

MRS Metabolite Peak-Area Ratios

Compared with controls, EP adolescents had lower tNAA/tCr and tCho/tCr ratios ( Table 2 ). The magnitude of reduction in tCho/tCr was almost fivefold that of tNAA/tCr.

Table 2 Magnetic resonance spectroscopy metabolite ratios contrasted between the birth groups

Relationship Between MRS Metabolite Peak-Area Ratios and Outcomes

IQ, working memory and attention outcomes are summarized in Table 3 . Compared with term-born controls, the EP group had lower IQ and performed more poorly in most tasks of working memory and attention, with the exception of immediate memory and selective visual attention.

Table 3 General intelligence, memory, and attention outcomes contrasted between birth groups

Metabolite ratios involving tCho were associated with IQ, whereas ratios involving tNAA were associated with working memory and shifting attention ( Table 4 ). Increased tCho/tCr ratios were associated with better IQ; a relationship seen in the EP group only but not controls (P value for interaction 0.04). tNAA/tCr ratios correlated positively with tests of working memory and shifting attention; a relationship that was similar in both EP and control groups (P value >0.05 for all interactions). All relationships between MRS metabolite ratios and IQ, memory, and attention persisted even after adjustment for posterior cingulate white matter volume.

Table 4 Relationships between magnetic resonance spectroscopy metabolite ratios and cognition, working memory, and attention outcomes across all participants

Discussion

This study demonstrated that brain metabolite ratios tNAA/tCr and tCho/tCr in the posterior cingulate were lower in EP survivors in late adolescence compared with controls. These brain metabolite ratios were associated with IQ, working memory, and attention, independent of white matter volume in a similar brain region. Interestingly the relationship between tCho/tCr with IQ was seen in the EP group but not controls. This requires more study but may suggest that Cho is an important biomarker for cognitive dysfunction in EP survivors.

Unfortunately, we report values from metabolite ratios rather than absolute quantitation. MRS was obtained as part of an extensive MRI protocol and time limitations precluded longer MRS acquisition, which was necessary to obtain absolute metabolite quantitation. Therefore, the lower tNAA/tCr ratio in the EP survivors could be due to decreased NAA, increased Cr or both. NAA is found predominantly in neurons, where concentrations increase in the early stages of gray matter, thalamic, and cerebellar development. This is thought to reflect the rapid formation of dendritic arborizations and synaptic connections (10). In addition, NAA has a role in oligodendrocyte myelin formation, neuroimmune reactions, and intercellular signaling (19). NAA rapidly increases in the first few years following birth, then slows down to reach adult levels by mid-adolescence (10,20). Decreased NAA levels are seen in neurodegenerative disorders, such as Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment (21), cerebral atrophy in demyelinating and gray matter disorders (22), as well as disorders characterized by a lack of progressive cortical development, such as early onset schizophrenia (23). The impact of preterm birth on NAA levels in adolescence is inconclusive. One study reported lower NAA levels in the hippocampus of preterm 15-y olds compared with controls (14) but another reported similar NAA/Cr ratios in frontal white matter (15). Lower NAA levels in EP compared with controls in this study may reflect impairment in the early developmental formation of dendritic arborizations and synaptic connections, which persist into adolescence (10,23).

Developmentally, Cho concentrations in the brain decrease in the first year after birth and remain relatively constant until early adulthood (10). Lower tCho/tCr ratios in the EP group could be a result of decreased Cho, increased Cr, or both. The lower tCho/tCr ratios in the EP group suggest decreased membrane phospholipid content in the posterior cingulate. This may be a result of disrupted Cho metabolism of membrane phospholipids following preterm birth, reflecting altered phospholipid turnover and development with potentially reduced content of dendrites and synapses, as well as cholinergic neurons (24). Our findings do not concur with a previous report where Cho/Cr ratios in the frontal white matter were similar between preterm and control adolescents (15). However, in that study, the sample size was small (18 term controls and 18 very-low-birth-weight adolescents) and the MRS was obtained from a different region of the brain. There is no current data on MRS changes in the posterior cingulate region following EP birth. In the current study, we regarded Cr as a reference for NAA and Cho. Cr levels remain relatively stable after the first year following birth (10), however, caution is needed as Cr levels can change acutely in pathological conditions in experimental models (25).

The relationship between brain metabolite ratios in infancy and later developmental outcomes in preterm infants has been reported in early childhood, with little information in late adolescence. NAA/Cho ratios in the cerebellum positively correlated with cognitive scores on the Bayley-III developmental assessment at 2 y (13), whereas higher NAA/Cho but lower Cho/Cr ratios in the posterior periventricular white matter were associated with better motor development at 1 y (11). Another study, however, did not find an association between NAA/Cho ratios in the deep nuclear gray matter and general development at 18 mo to 2 y (26). In the current study, higher tCho/tCr ratios in adolescence were associated with better IQ scores, whereas higher tNAA/tCr ratios were associated with better working memory and attention scores in adolescence. The tCho/tCr-function relationships in adolescence are not reflective of the associations described by others in infancy. This may be a result of different underlying processes affecting brain metabolite-function many years after preterm birth.

There are, however, data from other adult-onset conditions that support the association between brain NAA and Cho and cognitive function. Higher NAA/Cr and lower Cho/Cr are associated with cognitive functioning in Alzheimer’s disease, mild cognitive impairment, schizophrenia, and traumatic brain injury (21,27). In Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment, NAA/Cr ratios in the posterior cingulate region decreased with cognitive and memory decline associated with disease progression (21,27). This is in contrast with the normal ageing process where increases in NAA, possibly neuronal hypertrophy, and Cho, reflecting glial proliferation, have been documented (28). In our study, we found that the relationships between metabolites and IQ were mostly involving Cho, whereas those with working memory and attention were related to NAA. Of interest was the differential relationship between Cho ratios and IQ for the EP and control groups. This may suggest vulnerability of choline-containing compounds following EP birth, and may reflect either delayed development or early decline of Cho. Further research into potential alterations of choline function in preterm survivors is needed, given the paucity of such studies in the current literature.

We have previously reported in this same cohort that volumes of multiple brain tissues and structures were smaller in the EP group compared with controls (5). Of particular relevance to this study was white matter volume; which was in the order of 6.0% smaller (95% confidence interval of −8.8% to −3.2%) in the EP group compared with controls. In the present study, brain metabolites sampled from a specified region of white matter (left posterior cingulate) had lower tNAA/tCr and tCho/tCr in EP compared with controls. Thus, we can speculate that not only is EP birth associated with long term effects on brain volumes, it is also associated with lower concentrations of brain metabolites that are markers of neurons and cell membranes within the white matter. In addition, the previous study also reported an association between larger brain white matter volumes and higher IQ scores. An interesting finding from the current study involves the relationships between MRS metabolite ratios and cognitive outcomes (IQ, memory, and attention) which are independent of posterior cingulate white matter volumes. This may suggest that following EP birth, brain metabolite ratios are influencing cognitive function via alternative pathways to brain volumes, and highlights the need for further research into understanding these pathways.

This study is the largest neuroimaging cohort of EP and term-born controls in the era of modern intensive care to be prospectively followed up to late adolescence. With a contemporaneous group of term-born controls, we were able to make direct group comparisons of our MRS data rather than relying on normative data, which may not accurately reflect what is seen in our population. In addition, there were functional outcome data from validated neuropsychological tests that investigated functions associated with the posterior cingulate cortex. Thus, we were able to further understand how these metabolites are related to cognitive functioning in EP adolescents. The differential relationship between tCho/tCr and IQ between EP and term controls seen in our study is a novel finding. This opens up the possibility of further research into how Cho may be differentially affected following preterm birth.

Although a high proportion of participants consented to have MRI and MRS examinations, outcomes were only available for 60–78% of those originally recruited. EP participants who returned for assessment had higher IQ at age 8 y, lower rates of brain injury and were less likely to have cerebral palsy compared with EP participants who were not assessed. Similarly, control participants who were assessed had higher IQ at 8 y and additionally were more likely to have mothers with greater than 12 y of education, than controls who did not return for assessment. Thus, the differences we have observed may have been greater if the whole cohort had been assessed. Finally, we had cross sectional MRS data rather than longitudinal data, which limited our ability to understand the trajectory of brain metabolite differences between the birth groups from birth until adolescence.

We identified brain metabolite profiles that reflect altered development involving dendrites, synaptic formation and membranes in EP infants in late adolescence. This may underlie the association of EP birth with poorer performance on cognitive tasks. Of concern was little evidence of developmental “catch-up” of brain metabolites in late adolescence. It thus highlights the importance of understanding how EP birth potentially impacts on the normal ageing process as more EP survivors reach adulthood.

Methods

Participants

Participants were part of a prospective longitudinal cohort study of all EP adolescents born in the state of Victoria, Australia between 1991 and 1992. Term-born controls were contemporaneously recruited at birth, matched to the EP cohort on expected date of birth, mother’s country of birth (English-speaking versus other) and health insurance status (private or public). Follow up at ages 2 (29), 5 (30), and 8 y have been previously reported (31). At age 18 y, participants attended a comprehensive health and developmental assessment including MRI and MRS. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants, including their parents if aged less than 18 y at the time of assessment. Ethical approval for the original and follow-up studies were obtained from the Human Research and Ethics committees of the four participating centers, i.e., the Royal Women’s Hospital, Mercy Hospital for Women, Monash Medical Centre and Royal Children’s Hospital, Melbourne.

The MRI volumetric data, IQ, and the attention data have previously been reported on this cohort (5,32,33). In this manuscript, we report the MRS data and how that relates to IQ, working memory, and attention.

MRS

MRS was performed at two study sites, each using a Siemens 3T MAGNETOM Trio MRI system (Siemens Healthcare, Erlangen, Germany), a 12-channel receive-only head coil, and the same acquisition protocol. Proton (1H) MRS was obtained from a 20 × 15 × 10 mm voxel centred on the left posterior cingulate white matter ( Figure 1 ) using a Point Resolved Spectroscopy (PRESS) acquisition (repetition time = 3,000 ms, echo time = 135 ms, 128 averages). Spectra were quantified using LCModel (34). LCModel fits to in vivo spectra using a linear combination of simulated basis spectra for individual metabolites. The basis set used for this study included NAA, N-acetyl-aspartate-glutamate (NAAG), choline containing compounds (phosphocholine (PCh) and glycero-phosphocholine (GPC)) and creatine (Cr) and phosphocreatine (PCr). Metabolites were defined as follows: total NAA (tNAA) = NAA+NAAG; total choline (tCho) = GPC+PCh; total creatine (tCr) = Cr+PCr. LCModel provides an estimate of the standard deviation (SD) (Cramer-Rao lower bound) of the fitted amplitude of each metabolite included in the basis set expressed as a percentage of the amplitude. Metabolites with SD >50% are unreliably determined; major metabolites should have SD <20% (35). Each spectrum was manually checked and excluded if all of tNAA, tCho and tCr had SD >20% indicating a poor quality spectrum. In principle, LCModel allows estimation of absolute concentrations by reference to a separate unsuppressed water acquisition. However, at an echo time of 135 ms, large corrections for both water and metabolite T2 relaxations are required. For this study, there was insufficient time to directly estimate T2 relaxation and so only the metabolite ratios were calculated.

Figure 1
figure 1

Representative proton spectra and voxel placement centred on the left posterior cingulate region.

PowerPoint slide

Outcome Measures

For the purposes of this study, we focused on tests of functions thought to be associated with the posterior cingulate region, which included IQ, working memory and attention (16). Participants were assessed by trained examiners who were unaware of previous clinical history or group allocation.

IQ

IQ was estimated using a two-subtest version of the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) (36). The WASI IQ provides a standardized scale with a mean of 100 and SD of 15.

Working Memory

The Digit Recall and Backward Digit Recall subtests of the Working Memory Test Battery for Children were administered. In both subtests participants were presented with a sequence of digits of increasing span. In Digit Recall, the participants were required to recall the sequence of digits in the same order as they were presented, while in Backward Digit Recall participants were required to recall the digits in the reverse order. The maximum span, i.e., the longest recall sequence was recorded.

Attention

Three measures of attention, all derived from the Test of Everyday Attention (37), were used.

  • (i) Visual selective attention: The Telephone Search task involved identifying target stimuli (maximum = 20) that were interspersed amongst distractor stimuli in a simulated telephone directory.

  • (ii) Auditory selective attention: The Elevator with Distraction task involved participants imagining that they were in an elevator in which the visual floor indicator was not working. Participants were asked to calculate which floor they were located by counting a series of specific tones while ignoring the presence of a distractor tone. Ten trials were administered and the variable of interest was the number of correct trials (range 0–10).

  • (iii) Shifting attention: The Elevator with Reversal task required participants to count strings of medium pitched tones. Interspersed with these medium pitched tones were both high (indicating that the subject must switch to counting up) and low tones (indicating that the subject must switch to counting down). Ten trials were administered and the variable of interest was the number of correct trials (range 0–10).

Statistical Analyses

Data were analyzed using STATA 13.0 (StataCorp, College Station, TX). Participant characteristics were compared (i) between EP and controls, as well as (ii) participants with and without analyzable MRS within the EP and control groups, using t-tests and chi squared. Group differences in MRS metabolite ratios were explored using linear regression fitted using generalized estimating equations with robust (sandwich) estimation of standard errors to allow for multiple births within a family, using separate models for each brain metabolite ratio. IQ, memory, and attention variables were compared between groups using t-tests, or Wilcoxon rank sum tests for non-normal data. Unlike IQ scores, which were age-appropriate, the working memory and attention results were presented as raw scores and thus, analyses were repeated adjusted for corrected age at assessment. The relationships between brain metabolite ratios and functional outcomes (that were approximately normally distributed) were explored using linear regression, again fitted using generalized estimating equations with robust (sandwich) estimation of standard errors, with a separate model for each metabolite ratio-outcome combination. For functional outcome variables that had a skewed distribution, we used logistic regression with the outcomes dichotomized using the median for the particular variable as the cut-point. To determine whether the relationship between the metabolite ratio and outcome was the same in the EP and control groups, we used an interaction effect. To determine whether MRS metabolite ratios independently predicted IQ, memory, and attention, we added left posterior cingulate white matter volume as an additional step to the regression. Left posterior cingulate white matter volume was automatically generated from the T1-weighted structural MRI scan using FreeSurfer software as previously described (5).

We acknowledge the multiple comparisons within this study and hence we interpret our findings by focusing on overall patterns and magnitude of differences, rather than on individual P values.

Statement of Financial Support

This work was supported by the Australian National Health and Medical Research Council (Project Grant ID 491246 to L.D., P.A., S.W., J.C., as well as C Robertson, S Hope, and D Hacking; Centre of Clinical Research Excellence Grant ID 546519; Senior Research Fellowship ID 628371 to P.A.; Early Career Fellowship ID 1053787 to J.C.; Early Career Fellowship ID 1012236 to D.T.; Career Development Fellowship ID 1053609 to K.J.L.), the UK Department of Health’s NIHR Biomedical Research Centres funding scheme to AB, and the Victorian Government Operational Infrastructure Support Program. The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Disclosure

None.

Victorian Infant Collaborative Study Group Members

Convenor; Lex W Doyle, MD, FRACP1,2,3,5; Collaborators (in alphabetical order): Peter J Anderson, PhD3,5, Alice Burnett, PhD1,5, Catherine Callanan, RN1, Elizabeth Carse, FRACP7, Margaret P Charlton, M Ed Psych7, Jeanie Cheong, MD, FRACP1,2,5, Cinzia R De Luca, BSc, PhD1,2, Marie Hayes, RN7, Esther Hutchinson, BSc (Hons)1, Elaine Kelly, MA16, Marion McDonald, RN1, Carly Molloy, PhD1,2, Gillian Opie, FRACP6, Gehan Roberts, MPH, PhD, FRACP1,3,5,8, Colin Robertson, MD, FRACP8, Karissa Searle1, Andrew Watkins, FRACP6, Amanda Williamson6, Michelle Wilson-Ching, PhD1,2, Stephen J Wood, PhD4,9, Heather Woods, RN6. From: the 1Premature Infant Follow-up Program at the Royal Women’s Hospital, the Departments of 2Obstetrics and Gynaecology, 3Paediatrics and 4Psychiatry at The University of Melbourne, 5Murdoch Childrens Research Institute, 6Mercy Hospital for Women, 7Monash Medical Centre, and the 8Royal Children’s Hospital, Melbourne, Australia, and 9School of Psychology, the University of Birmingham, United Kingdom.