Key Points
-
Williams syndrome is a neurodevelopmental disorder with a prevalence of up to 1 in 7,500, caused by a hemizygous deletion of ∼1.6 megabases, containing ∼28 genes, on chromosome 7q11.23 through unequal homologous recombination during meiosis. Williams syndrome is characterized by typical facial features, cardiovascular abnormalities, mild to moderate mental retardation or learning difficulties, and unique neuropsychological and behavioural features that have made it a focus of research in neuroscience and genetics.
-
The neuropsychological profile of Williams syndrome shows a severe weakness in visuospatial construction, combined with relative strength in verbal short-term memory and language. However, neither language production nor verbal short-term memory is typically completely normal. Behaviourally, individuals with Williams syndrome show a striking social fearlessness and gregariousness, combined with strongly increased non-social fear.
-
We review recent advances made in defining the neural substrates of the unique neuropsychiatric features of Williams syndrome and define separable neural subsystems in this syndrome, specifying mechanisms for genetic influences on visuospatial cognition, social behaviour and memory. These results are discussed in the context of emerging data that link dissociable genetic contributions to these phenotypes through the study of knockout mouse models and atypical deletions in humans.
-
The brains of individuals with Williams syndrome are smaller and show regions of reduced grey matter volume, and abnormal gyrus and sulcus configuration. A reduction of grey matter volume and depth has been identified in the intraparietal sulcus, a region that is important for visuospatial constructive function. In addition, functional MRI studies show activation deficits in the adjacent parietal lobe as a functional correlate of a circumscribed dorsal visual stream deficit that can be linked to the structural abnormality and may underlie the severe visuospatial constructive impairment seen in Williams syndrome.
-
Convergent imaging evidence shows abnormal resting blood flow and activation of the anterior hippocampal formation together with only subtly abnormal structure. This might be linked to the severe long-term memory and visual-navigational impairments associated with Williams syndrome.
-
In individuals with Williams syndrome, the amygdala is less active to threatening faces, but shows increased activity to threatening non-social stimuli, mirroring the fear profile in behaviour. Interactions between the amygdala and prefrontal regulatory regions, especially the orbitofrontal cortex, are abnormal, which suggests that the neural systems for social and non-social fear are dissociable and underlie different genetic–developmental trajectories.
-
Knockout mouse models for LIM domain kinase 1 (Limk1) and cytoplasmic linker 2 (C yln2) show similar hippocampal abnormalities to those identified in humans, implicating these genes in hippocampal function. The results of studies of human families with small deletions suggest that LIMK1 is a promising candidate gene for involvement in the severe impairment in visuospatial construction seen in individuals with Williams syndrome, and that GTF2I (general transcription factor II i repeat domain-containing 1) hemideletion is necessary for mental retardation in this syndrome.
Abstract
Williams syndrome, a rare disorder caused by hemizygous microdeletion of about 28 genes on chromosome 7q11.23, has long intrigued neuroscientists with its unique combination of striking behavioural abnormalities, such as hypersociability, and characteristic neurocognitive profile. Williams syndrome, therefore, raises fundamental questions about the neural mechanisms of social behaviour, the modularity of mind and brain development, and provides a privileged setting to understand genetic influences on complex brain functions in a 'bottom-up' way. We review recent advances in uncovering the functional and structural neural substrates of Williams syndrome that provide an emerging understanding of how these are related to dissociable genetic contributions characterized both in special participant populations and animal models.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution
Relevant articles
Open Access articles citing this article.
-
Gene mapping methodology powered by induced genome rearrangements
Scientific Reports Open Access 05 October 2022
-
Atypical deletion of Williams–Beuren syndrome reveals the mechanism of neurodevelopmental disorders
BMC Medical Genomics Open Access 04 April 2022
-
An EEG investigation of alpha and beta activity during resting states in adults with Williams syndrome
BMC Psychology Open Access 05 May 2021
Access options
Subscribe to this journal
Receive 12 print issues and online access
$189.00 per year
only $15.75 per issue
Rent or buy this article
Get just this article for as long as you need it
$39.95
Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout





References
Strømme, P., Bjørnstad, P. G. & Ramstad, K. Prevalence estimation of Williams syndrome. J. Child Neurol. 17, 269–271 (2002).
Beuren, A. J., Apitz, J. & Harmjanz, D. Supravalvular aortic stenosis in association with mental retardation and a certain facial appearance. Circulation 26, 1235–1240 (1962).
Williams, J. C., Barratt-Boyes, B. G. & Lowe, J. B. Supravalvular aortic stenosis. Circulation 24, 1311–1318 (1961). The initial description of WS.
American Academy of Pediatrics: Health care supervision for children with Williams syndrome. Pediatrics 107, 1192–1204 (2001).
Morris, C. A. et al. GTF2I hemizygosity implicated in mental retardation in Williams syndrome: genotype–phenotype analysis of five families with deletions in the Williams syndrome region. Am. J. Med. Genet. 123A, 45–59 (2003). Provides evidence that GTF2I is important for normal intellectual ability.
Morris, C. A. in Williams–Beuren syndrome: Research, Evaluation, and Treatment (eds Morris, C. A., Lenhoff, H. M. & Wang, P. P.) 3–17 (Johns Hopkins Univ. Press, Baltimore, 2006).
Chapman, C. A., du Plessis, A. & Pober, B. R. Neurologic findings in children and adults with Williams syndrome. J. Child Neurol. 11, 63–65 (1996).
Cherniske, E. M. et al. Multisystem study of 20 older adults with Williams syndrome. Am. J. Med. Genet. 131A, 255–264 (2004).
Marler, J. A., Elfenbein, J. L., Ryals, B. M., Urban, Z. & Netzloff, M. L. Sensorineural hearing loss in children and adults with Williams syndrome. Am. J. Med. Genet. A 138, 318–327 (2005).
Mervis, C. B. & Klein-Tasman, B. P. Williams syndrome: cognition, personality, and adaptive behaviour. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 6, 148–158 (2000). Overview of cognition, language and personality in WS.
Mervis, C. B. et al. The Williams syndrome cognitive profile. Brain Cogn. 44, 604–628 (2000).
Farran, E. K. & Jarrold, C. Visuospatial cognition in Williams syndrome: reviewing and accounting for the strengths and weaknesses in performance. Dev. Neuropsychol. 23, 173–200 (2003).
Klein-Tasman, B. P. & Mervis, C. B. Distinctive personality characteristics of 8-, 9-, and 10-year-olds with Williams syndrome. Dev. Neuropsychol. 23, 269–290 (2003).
Bellugi, U., Adolphs, R., Cassady, C. & Chiles, M. Towards the neural basis for hypersociability in a genetic syndrome. Neuroreport 10, 1653–1657 (1999). A classic paper proposing amygdala involvement in the genesis of the social symptoms of WS.
Dykens, E. M. Anxiety, fears, and phobias in persons with Williams syndrome. Dev. Neuropsychol. 23, 291–316 (2003).
Leyfer, O. T., Woodruff-Borden, J., Klein-Tasman, B. P., Fricke, J. S. & Mervis, C. B. Prevalence of psychiatric disorders in 4–16-year-olds with Williams syndrome. Am. J. Med. Genet. B (in the press).
Meyer-Lindenberg, A. et al. Neural basis of genetically determined visuospatial construction deficit in Williams syndrome. Neuron 43, 623–631 (2004). Identification of structural–functional abnormalities in intraparietal sulcus and adjacent parietal areas underlying the visuoconstructive deficit in WS.
Urban, Z. et al. 7q11.23 deletions in Williams syndrome arise as a consequence of unequal meiotic crossover. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 59, 958–962 (1996).
Bayes, M., Magano, L. F., Rivera, N., Flores, R. & Perez Jurado, L. A. Mutational mechanisms of Williams–Beuren syndrome deletions. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 73, 131–151 (2003).
Korenberg, J. R. et al. VI. Genome structure and cognitive map of Williams syndrome. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12 (Suppl. 1), 89–107 (2000).
Hillier, L. W. et al. The DNA sequence of human chromosome 7. Nature 424, 157–164 (2003).
Hobart, H. H. et al. Heterozygotes for the microinversion of the Williams–Beuren region have an increased risk for affected offspring. Presented at the Annual Meeting of The American Society of Human Genetics, October 2004, Toronto, Ontario, Canada http://genetics.faseb.org/genetics/ashg/annmeet/2004/menu-annmeet-2004.shtml
Osborne, L. R. et al. A 1.5 million-base pair inversion polymorphism in families with Williams–Beuren syndrome. Nature Genet. 29, 321–325 (2001).
Somerville, M. J. et al. Severe expressive-language delay related to duplication of the Williams–Beuren locus. N. Engl. J. Med. 353, 1694–1701 (2005). Describes the first reported case of duplication of the WS region. Documents that the phenotype for duplication of 7q11.23 is very different from the phenotype for hemideletion (WS).
Jernigan, T. L. & Bellugi, U. Anomalous brain morphology on magnetic resonance images in Williams syndrome and Down syndrome. Arch. Neurol. 47, 529–533 (1990).
Mercuri, E. et al. Chiari I malformation in asymptomatic young children with Williams syndrome: clinical and MRI study. Eur. J. Paediatr. Neurol. 1, 177–181 (1997).
Pober, B. R. & Filiano, J. J. Association of Chiari I malformation and Williams syndrome. Pediatr. Neurol. 12, 84–88 (1995).
Schmitt, J. E., Eliez, S., Warsofsky, I. S., Bellugi, U. & Reiss, A. L. Corpus callosum morphology of Williams syndrome: relation to genetics and behavior. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 43, 155–159 (2001).
Tomaiuolo, F. et al. Morphology and morphometry of the corpus callosum in Williams syndrome: a T1-weighted MRI study. Neuroreport 13, 2281–2284 (2002).
Eckert, M. A. et al. Evidence for superior parietal impairment in Williams syndrome. Neurology 64, 152–153 (2005).
Reiss, A. L. et al. IV. Neuroanatomy of Williams syndrome: a high-resolution MRI study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12 (Suppl. 1), 65–73 (2000).
Wang, P. P., Hesselink, J. R., Jernigan, T. L., Doherty, S. & Bellugi, U. Specific neurobehavioral profile of Williams' syndrome is associated with neocerebellar hemispheric preservation. Neurology 42, 1999–2002 (1992).
Jones, W. et al. Cerebellar abnormalities in infants and toddlers with Williams syndrome. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 44, 688–694 (2002).
Rae, C. et al. Brain biochemistry in Williams syndrome: evidence for a role of the cerebellum in cognition? Neurology 51, 33–40 (1998).
Boddaert, N. et al. Parieto-occipital grey matter abnormalities in children with Williams syndrome. Neuroimage (in the press).
Reiss, A. L. et al. An experiment of nature: brain anatomy parallels cognition and behaviour in Williams syndrome. J. Neurosci. 24, 5009–5015 (2004). A study of structural abnormalities in participants with WS and mental retardation using VBM.
Meyer-Lindenberg, A. et al. Neural correlates of genetically abnormal social cognition in Williams syndrome. Nature Neurosci. 8, 991–993 (2005). Identification of abnormal function in the amygdala and the prefrontal cortical regions that regulate it, suggesting mechanisms for hypersociability and increased non-social fear in WS.
Cammareri, V., Vignati, G., Nocera, G., Beck-Peccoz, P. & Persani, L. Thyroid hemiagenesis and elevated thyrotropin levels in a child with Williams syndrome. Am. J. Med. Genet. 85, 491–494 (1999).
Schmitt, J. E., Eliez, S., Bellugi, U. & Reiss, A. L. Analysis of cerebral shape in Williams syndrome. Arch. Neurol. 58, 283–287 (2001).
Schmitt, J. E. et al. Increased gyrification in Williams syndrome: evidence using 3D MRI methods. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 44, 292–295 (2002).
Thompson, P. M. et al. Abnormal cortical complexity and thickness profiles mapped in Williams syndrome. J. Neurosci. 25, 4146–4158 (2005). Innovative paper showing regionally increased gyrification and cortical thickness in participants with WS.
Jackowski, A. P. & Schultz, R. T. Foreshortened dorsal extension of the central sulcus in Williams syndrome. Cortex 41, 282–290 (2005).
Galaburda, A. M. et al. Dorsal forebrain anomaly in Williams syndrome. Arch. Neurol. 58, 1865–1869 (2001).
Kippenhan, J. S. et al. Genetic contributions to human gyrification: sulcal morphometry in Williams syndrome. J. Neurosci. 25, 7840–7846 (2005). An analysis of cortical geometry showing abnormal sulcal depth in the OFC and intraparietal sulcus in WS.
Van Essen, D. C. Towards a quantitative, probabilistic neuroanatomy of cerebral cortex. Cortex 40, 211–212 (2004).
Rakic, P. Genetic control of cortical convolutions. Science 303, 1983–1984 (2004).
Van Essen, D. C. A tension-based theory of morphogenesis and compact wiring in the central nervous system. Nature 385, 313–318 (1997).
Mima, T. & Mikawa, T. Folding of the tectal cortex by local remodeling of neural differentiation. Dev. Dyn. 229, 475–479 (2004).
Marenco, S. et al. Preliminary diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) observations in 5 individuals with Williams syndrome (WS). Neuroimage 22 (Suppl. 1), M0363 (2004).
Levitin, D. J. et al. Neural correlates of auditory perception in Williams syndrome: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 18, 74–82 (2003).
Winter, M., Pankau, R., Amm, M., Gosch, A. & Wessel, A. The spectrum of ocular features in the Williams–Beuren syndrome. Clin. Genet. 49, 28–31 (1996).
Kapp, M. E., von Noorden, G. K. & Jenkins, R. Strabismus in Williams syndrome. Am. J. Ophthalmol. 119, 355–360 (1995).
Atkinson, J. et al. Visual and visuospatial development in young children with Williams syndrome. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 43, 330–337 (2001).
Sadler, L. S., Olitsky, S. E. & Reynolds, J. D. Reduced stereoacuity in Williams syndrome. Am. J. Med. Genet. 66, 287–288 (1996).
Grice, S. J. et al. ERP abnormalities of illusory contour perception in Williams syndrome. Neuroreport 14, 1773–1777 (2003).
Olsen, R. K. et al. Retinotopic mapping of early visual areas in Williams syndrome. Presented at the Human Brain Mapping Meeting 2003 (New York, USA). Neuroimage 19 (Suppl. 1), 1550 (2003).
Frangiskakis, J. M. et al. LIM-kinase1 hemizygosity implicated in impaired visuospatial constructive cognition. Cell 86, 59–69 (1996). The first study implicating a specific gene in a cognitive characteristic of WS.
Ungerleider, L. G. & Mishkin, M. in Analysis of Visual Behavior (eds Ingle, D. J., Goodale, D. J. & Mansfield, R. J. W.) 549–586 (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 1982).
Van Essen, D. C., Anderson, C. H. & Felleman, D. J. Information processing in the primate visual system: an integrated systems perspective. Science 255, 419–423 (1992).
Landau, B., Hoffman, J. E. & Kurz, N. Object recognition with severe spatial deficits in Williams syndrome: sparing and breakdown. Cognition (in the press).
Atkinson, J. et al. Neurobiological models of visuospatial cognition in children with Williams syndrome: measures of dorsal-stream and frontal function. Dev. Neuropsychol. 23, 139–172 (2003).
Galaburda, A. M., Holinger, D. P., Bellugi, U. & Sherman, G. F. Williams syndrome: neuronal size and neuronal-packing density in primary visual cortex. Arch. Neurol. 59, 1461–1467 (2002).
Nakamura, M. et al. Williams syndrome and deficiency in visuospatial recognition. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 43, 617–621 (2001).
Paul, B. M., Stiles, J., Passarotti, A., Bavar, N. & Bellugi, U. Face and place processing in Williams syndrome: evidence for a dorsal-ventral dissociation. Neuroreport 13, 1115–1119 (2002).
Glabus, M. F. et al. Interindividual differences in functional interactions among prefrontal, parietal and parahippocampal regions during working memory. Cereb. Cortex 13, 1352–1361 (2003).
McIntosh, A. R. et al. Network analysis of cortical visual pathways mapped with PET. J. Neurosci. 14, 655–666 (1994).
Nardini, M., Breckenridge, K. E., Eastwood, R. L., Atkinson, J. & Braddick, O. J. Distinct developmental trajectories in three systems for spatial encoding between the ages of 3 and 6 years. Perception S33, 28b (2004).
O'Hearn, K., Landau, B. & Hoffman, J. E. Multiple object tracking in people with Williams syndrome and in normally developing children. Psychol. Sci. 16, 905–912 (2005).
Nichols, S. et al. Mechanisms of verbal memory impairment in four neurodevelopmental disorders. Brain Lang. 88, 180–189 (2004).
Vicari, S., Bellucci, S. & Carlesimo, G. A. Visual and spatial long-term memory: differential pattern of impairments in Williams and Down syndromes. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 47, 305–311 (2005).
Meyer-Lindenberg, A. et al. Functional, structural, and metabolic abnormalities of the hippocampal formation in Williams syndrome. J. Clin. Invest. 115, 1888–1895 (2005). Pronounced functional and subtle structural abnormalities in the hippocampal formation in WS.
Pan, J. W. & Takahashi, K. Interdependence of N-acetyl aspartate and high-energy phosphates in healthy human brain. Ann. Neurol. 57, 92–97 (2005).
Izumi, Y. & Zorumski, C. F. Involvement of nitric oxide in low glucose-mediated inhibition of hippocampal long-term potentiation. Synapse 25, 258–262 (1997).
Petroff, O. A., Errante, L. D., Rothman, D. L., Kim, J. H. & Spencer, D. D. Neuronal and glial metabolite content of the epileptogenic human hippocampus. Ann. Neurol. 52, 635–642 (2002).
Preston, A. R., Shrager, Y., Dudukovic, N. M. & Gabrieli, J. D. Hippocampal contribution to the novel use of relational information in declarative memory. Hippocampus 14, 148–152 (2004).
Grill-Spector, K. The neural basis of object perception. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 13, 159–166 (2003).
Suzuki, W. A. & Amaral, D. G. Perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices of the macaque monkey: cortical afferents. J. Comp. Neurol. 350, 497–533 (1994).
Egan, M. F. et al. The BDNF val66met polymorphism affects activity-dependent secretion of BDNF and human memory and hippocampal function. Cell 112, 257–269 (2003).
Bellugi, U., Bihrle, A., Jernigan, T., Trauner, D. & Doherty, S. Neuropsychological, neurological, and neuroanatomical profile of Williams syndrome. Am. J. Med. Genet. Suppl. 6, 115–125 (1990).
Bellugi, U., Sabo, H. & Vaid, J. in Language Development in Exceptional Circumstances (eds Bishop, D. V. M. & Mogford-Bevan, K.) 177–189 (Churchill Livingstone, Edinburgh; New York, 1988). A classic paper that sparked broad interest in WS in the cognitive neurosciences.
Bellugi, U., Wang, P. P. & Jernigan, T. L. in Atypical Cognitive Deficits in Developmental Disorders: Implications for Brain Function (eds Broman, S. H. & Grafman, J.) 23–56 (Erlbaum, Hillsdale, New Jersey, 1994).
Bates, E., Tager-Flusberg, H., Vicari, S. & Volterra, V. Debate over language's link with intelligence. Nature 413, 565–566 (2001).
Mervis, C. B. in Williams–Beuren Syndrome: Research, Evaluation, and Treatment (eds Morris, C. A., Lenhoff, H. M. & Wang, P. P.) 159–206 (Johns Hopkins Univ. Press, Baltimore, 2006).
Dunn, L. M. & Dunn, L. M. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (American Guidance Service, 1997).
Bellugi, U., Lichtenberger, L., Jones, W., Lai, Z. & St George, M. I. The neurocognitive profile of Williams Syndrome: a complex pattern of strengths and weaknesses. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12 (Suppl. 1), 7–29 (2000).
Mervis, C. B., Morris, C. A., Bertrand, J. & Robinson, B. F. in Neurodevelopmental Disorders (ed. Tager-Flusberg, H.) 65–110 (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massacusetts, 1999).
Temple, C. M., Almazan, M. & Sherwood, S. Lexical skills in Williams syndrome: a cognitive neuropsychological analysis. J. Neurolinguistics 15, 463–495 (2002).
Mervis, C. B., Robinson, B. F., Rowe, M. L., Becerra, A. M. & Klein-Tasman, B. P. Language abilities of individuals who have Williams syndrome. Int. Rev. Res. Ment. Retard. 27, 35–81 (2003).
Grant, J., Valian, V. & Karmiloff-Smith, A. A study of relative clauses in Williams syndrome. J. Child Lang. 29, 403–416 (2002).
Udwin, O. & Yule, W. Expressive language of children with Williams syndrome. Am. J. Med. Genet. Suppl. 6, 108–114 (1990).
Zukowski, A. Knowledge of constraints on compounding in children and adolescents with Williams syndrome. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 48, 79–92 (2005).
Karmiloff-Smith, A. et al. Language and Williams syndrome: how intact is 'intact'? Child Dev. 68, 246–262 (1997).
Levy, Y. & Hermon, S. Morphological abilities of Hebrew-speaking adolescents with Williams syndrome. Dev. Neuropsychol. 23, 59–83 (2003).
Laws, G. & Bishop, D. Pragmatic language impairment and social deficits in Williams syndrome: a comparison with Down's syndrome and specific language impairment. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 39, 45–64 (2004).
Robinson, B. F., Mervis, C. B. & Robinson, B. W. The roles of verbal short-term memory and working memory in the acquisition of grammar by children with Williams syndrome. Dev. Neuropsychol. 23, 13–31 (2003).
Lukács, A., Racsmány, M. & Pléh, C. Vocabulary and morphological patterns in Hungarian children with Williams syndrome: a preliminary report. Acta Linguistica Hungarica 48, 243–269 (2001).
Karmiloff-Smith, A., Brown, J. H., Grice, S. & Paterson, S. Dethroning the myth: cognitive dissociations and innate modularity in Williams syndrome. Dev. Neuropsychol. 23, 227–242 (2003).
Bellugi, U., Lichtenberger, L., Mills, D., Galaburda, A. & Korenberg, J. R. Bridging cognition, the brain and molecular genetics: evidence from Williams syndrome. Trends Neurosci. 22, 197–207 (1999).
Mons, A. W., Wilmowski, W. A. & Detweiler, C. Williams Syndrome: a Highly Musical Species (EO Productions International, 1996).
Levitin, D. J. & Bellugi, U. Musical abilities in individuals with Williams syndrome. Music Percept. 15, 357–389 (1998).
Levitin, D. J. et al. Characterizing the musical phenotype in individuals with Williams Syndrome. Child Neuropsychol. 10, 223–247 (2004).
Dykens, E. M., Rosner, B. A., Ly, T. & Sagun, J. Music and anxiety in Williams syndrome: a harmonious or discordant relationship? Am. J. Ment. Retard. 110, 346–358 (2005).
Carrasco, X., Castillo, S., Aravena, T., Rothhammer, P. & Aboitiz, F. Williams syndrome: pediatric, neurologic, and cognitive development. Pediatr. Neurol. 32, 166–172 (2005).
Gordon, F. E. in Musical Aptitude Profile (Houghton Mifflin, Boston, 1995).
Gordon, F. E. Primary Measures of Music Audiation (G. I. A. Publications, Chicago, 1980).
Don, A. J., Schellenberg, G. & Rourke, B. P. Music and language skills of children with Williams syndrome. Child Neuropsychol. 5, 154–170 (1999).
Hopyan, T., Dennis, M., Weksberg, R. & Cytrynbaum, C. Music skills and the expressive interpretation of music in children with Williams–Beuren syndrome: pitch, rhythm, melodic imagery, phrasing, and musical affect. Child Neuropsychol. 7, 42–53 (2001).
Jones, W. et al. II. Hypersociability in Williams syndrome. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12 (Suppl. 1), 30–46 (2000).
Gosch, A. & Pankau, R. Social–emotional and behavioral adjustment in children with Williams–Beuren syndrome. Am. J. Med. Genet. 53, 335–339 (1994).
Mervis, C. B. Williams syndrome: 15 years of psychological research. Dev. Neuropsychol. 23, 1–12 (2003).
Frigerio, E. et al. Is everybody always my friend? Perception of approachability in Williams syndrome. Neuropsychologia 44, 254–259 (2006).
Davies, M., Udwin, O. & Howlin, P. Adults with Williams syndrome. Preliminary study of social, emotional and behavioural difficulties. Br. J. Psychiatry 172, 273–276 (1998).
Greer, M. K., Brown, F. R., Pai, G. S., Choudry, S. H. & Klein, A. J. Cognitive, adaptive, and behavioral characteristics of Williams syndrome. Am. J. Med. Genet. 74, 521–525 (1997).
Gagliardi, C. et al. Facial expression recognition in Williams syndrome. Neuropsychologia 41, 733–738 (2003).
Tager-Flusberg, H. & Sullivan, K. A componential view of theory of mind: evidence from Williams syndrome. Cognition 76, 59–90 (2000). Relates cognitive subcomponents of theory of mind to social function in WS.
Plesa Skwerer, D., Faja, S., Schofield, C., Verbalis, A. & Tager-Flusberg, H. Perceiving facial and vocal expressions of emotion in Williams syndrome. Am. J. Ment. Retard. (in the press).
Udwin, O. & Yule, W. A cognitive and behavioural phenotype in Williams syndrome. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 13, 232–244 (1991).
Davies, M., Howlin, P. & Udwin, O. Independence and adaptive behavior in adults with Williams syndrome. Am. J. Med. Genet. 70, 188–195 (1997).
Levine, K. & Wharton, R. Williams syndrome and happiness. Am. J. Ment. Retard. 105, 363–371 (2000).
LeDoux, J. The emotional brain, fear, and the amygdala. Cell. Mol. Neurobiol. 23, 727–738 (2003).
Amaral, D. G. The primate amygdala and the neurobiology of social behavior: implications for understanding social anxiety. Biol. Psychiatry 51, 11–17 (2002).
Hariri, A. R., Tessitore, A., Mattay, V. S., Fera, F. & Weinberger, D. R. The amygdala response to emotional stimuli: a comparison of faces and scenes. Neuroimage 17, 317–323 (2002).
Dilger, S. et al. Brain activation to phobia-related pictures in spider phobic humans: an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neurosci. Lett. 348, 29–32 (2003).
Kringelbach, M. L. & Rolls, E. T. The functional neuroanatomy of the human orbitofrontal cortex: evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychology. Prog. Neurobiol. 72, 341–372 (2004).
Pears, A., Parkinson, J. A., Hopewell, L., Everitt, B. J. & Roberts, A. C. Lesions of the orbitofrontal but not medial prefrontal cortex disrupt conditioned reinforcement in primates. J. Neurosci. 23, 11189–11201 (2003).
Adolphs, R. Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour. Nature Rev. Neurosci. 4, 165–178 (2003). An excellent and inspiring synthesis of social cognitive neuroscience.
Rolls, E. T., Hornak, J., Wade, D. & McGrath, J. Emotion-related learning in patients with social and emotional changes associated with frontal lobe damage. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 57, 1518–1524 (1994).
Stone, V. E., Baron-Cohen, S. & Knight, R. T. Frontal lobe contributions to theory of mind. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 10, 640–656 (1998).
Bauman, M. D., Lavenex, P., Mason, W. A., Capitanio, J. P. & Amaral, D. G. The development of social behaviour following neonatal amygdala lesions in rhesus monkeys. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1388–1411 (2004). Dissociation of social/non-social fears in the primate after neonatal lesions.
Meyer-Lindenberg, A. et al. Neural mechanisms for genetic risk for violence and impulsivity in humans. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA (in the press).
Fodor, J. A. The Modularity of Mind: an Essay on Faculty Psychology (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 1983).
Paterson, S. J., Brown, J. H., Gsödl, M. K., Johnson, M. H. & Karmiloff-Smith, A. Cognitive modularity and genetic disorders. Science 286, 2355–2358 (1999).
Pinker, S. Words and Rules: The Ingredients of Language (Basic Books, New York, 1999).
Piattelli-Palmarini, M. Speaking of learning: how do we acquire our marvellous facility for expressing ourselves in words? Nature 411, 887–888 (2001).
Karmiloff-Smith, A. Beyond Modularity: a Developmental Perspective on Cognitive Science (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 1992).
Hoogenraad, C. C. et al. Targeted mutation of Cyln2 in the Williams syndrome critical region links CLIP-115 haploinsufficiency to neurodevelopmental abnormalities in mice. Nature Genet. 32, 116–127 (2002). A key paper that shows the effects of Cyln2 hemi-insufficiency on hippocampal function in mice.
Meng, Y. et al. Abnormal spine morphology and enhanced LTP in LIMK-1 knockout mice. Neuron 35, 121–133 (2002). Abnormal microstructure and hippocampal function in Limk1 null knockouts.
Zhao, C. et al. Hippocampal and visuospatial learning defects in mice with a deletion of frizzled 9, a gene in the Williams syndrome deletion interval. Development 132, 2917–2927 (2005).
Durkin, M. E., Keck-Waggoner, C. L., Popescu, N. C. & Thorgeirsson, S. S. Integration of a c-myc transgene results in disruption of the mouse Gtf2ird1 gene, the homologue of the human GTF2IRD1 gene hemizygously deleted in Williams–Beuren syndrome. Genomics 73, 20–27 (2001).
Tassabehji, M. et al. GTF2IRD1 in craniofacial development of humans and mice. Science 310, 1184–1187 (2005).
Bayarsaihan, D. et al. Genomic organization of the genes Gtf2ird1, Gtf2i, and Ncf1 at the mouse chromosome 5 region syntenic to the human chromosome 7q11.23 Williams syndrome critical region. Genomics 79, 137–143 (2002).
DeSilva, U. et al. Generation and comparative analysis of approximately 3.3 Mb of mouse genomic sequence orthologous to the region of human chromosome 7q11.23 implicated in Williams syndrome. Genome Res. 12, 3–15 (2002).
Proschel, C., Blouin, M. J., Gutowski, N. J., Ludwig, R. & Noble, M. Limk1 is predominantly expressed in neural tissues and phosphorylates serine, threonine and tyrosine residues in vitro. Oncogene 11, 1271–1281 (1995).
Hoogenraad, C. C., Akhmanova, A., Galjart, N. & De Zeeuw, C. I. LIMK1 and CLIP-115: linking cytoskeletal defects to Williams syndrome. Bioessays 26, 41–150 (2004).
Franke, Y., Peoples, R. J. & Francke, U. Identification of GTF2IRD1, a putative transcription factor within the Williams–Beuren syndrome deletion at 7q11.23. Cytogenet. Cell Genet. 86, 296–304 (1999).
Wang, Y. K., Perez-Jurado, L. A. & Francke, U. A mouse single-copy gene, Gtf2i, the homolog of human GTF2I, that is duplicated in the Williams–Beuren syndrome deletion region. Genomics 48, 163–170 (1998).
Cairo, S., Merla, G., Urbinati, F., Ballabio, A. & Reymond, A. WBSCR14, a gene mapping to the Williams–Beuren syndrome deleted region, is a new member of the Mlx transcription factor network. Hum. Mol. Genet. 10, 617–627 (2001).
Botta, A. et al. Expression analysis and protein localization of the human HPC-1/syntaxin 1A, a gene deleted in Williams syndrome. Genomics 62, 525–528 (1999).
Botta, A. et al. Detection of an atypical 7q11.23 deletion in Williams syndrome patients which does not include the STX1A and FZD3 genes. J. Med. Genet. 36, 478–480 (1999).
Heller, R., Rauch, A., Luttgen, S., Schroder, B. & Winterpacht, A. Partial deletion of the critical 1.5 Mb interval in Williams–Beuren syndrome. J. Med. Genet. 40, E99 (2003).
Karmiloff-Smith, A. et al. Using case study comparisons to explore genotype–phenotype correlations in Williams–Beuren syndrome. J. Med. Genet. 40, 136–140 (2003).
Tassabehji, M. et al. Williams syndrome: use of chromosomal microdeletions as a tool to dissect cognitive and physical phenotypes. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 64, 118–125 (1999).
Donnai, D. & Karmiloff-Smith, A. Williams syndrome: from genotype through to the cognitive phenotype. Am. J. Med. Genet. 97, 164–171 (2000).
Hirota, H. et al. Williams syndrome deficits in visual spatial processing linked to GTF2IRD1 and GTF2I on chromosome 7q11.23. Genet. Med. 5, 311–321 (2003).
Elliott, C. D. Differential Ability Scales (Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, Texas, 1990).
Kaufman, A. S. & Kaufman, N. L. Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test (American Guidance Services, Pines, Minnesota, 1990).
American Psychiatric Association Task Force on DSM-IV. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC, 1994).
Pelphrey, K., Adolphs, R. & Morris, J. P. Neuroanatomical substrates of social cognition dysfunction in autism. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 10, 259–271 (2004).
Reiss, A. L., Feinstein, C., Rosenbaum, K. N., Borengasser-Caruso, M. A. & Goldsmith, B. M. Autism associated with Williams syndrome. J. Pediatr. 106, 247–249 (1985).
Gillberg, C. & Rasmussen, P. Brief report: four case histories and a literature review of Williams syndrome and autistic behavior. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 24, 381–393 (1994).
Baron-Cohen, S. & Belmonte, M. K. Autism: a window onto the development of the social and the analytic brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 28, 109–126 (2005).
Bauman, M. L. & Kemper, T. L. Neuroanatomic observations of the brain in autism: a review and future directions. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 23, 183–187 (2005).
Baron-Cohen, S. et al. Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: an fMRI study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 11, 1891–1898 (1999).
Critchley, H. D. et al. The functional neuroanatomy of social behaviour: changes in cerebral blood flow when people with autistic disorder process facial expressions. Brain 123, 2203–2212 (2000).
Wang, A. T., Dapretto, M., Hariri, A. R., Sigman, M. & Bookheimer, S. Y. Neural correlates of facial affect processing in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 43, 481–490 (2004).
Schultz, R. T. et al. Abnormal ventral temporal cortical activity during face discrimination among individuals with autism and Asperger syndrome. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 57, 331–340 (2000).
Pierce, K., Muller, R. A., Ambrose, J., Allen, G. & Courchesne, E. Face processing occurs outside the fusiform 'face area' in autism: evidence from functional MRI. Brain 124, 2059–2073 (2001).
Dalton, K. M. et al. Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism. Nature Neurosci. 8, 519–526 (2005).
Adolphs, R. The neurobiology of social cognition. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 11, 231–239 (2001).
Martin, J. H. Neuroanatomy: Text and Atlas 2nd edn (Appleton & Lange, Stamford, Connecticut, 1996).
Galaburda, A. M., Holinger, D. P., Bellugi, U. & Sherman, G. F. Williams syndrome: neuronal size and neuronal-packing density in primary visual cortex. Arch. Neurol. 59, 1461–1467 (2002).
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) Intramural Research Program (IRP), a grant from the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (C.B.M.) and a grant from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (C.B.M.). We thank C. Rainey for help with figure preparation, L. Kempf for preparation of the supplementary table and S. Kippenhan, P. Kohn and C. Morris for their ongoing collaboration on our neuroimaging work.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
Supplementary information
Related links
Related links
DATABASES
OMIM
FURTHER INFORMATION
Glossary
- Haploinsufficiency
-
Presence of only a single functional copy of a gene that does not provide sufficient transcript or protein production to assure normal function.
- Hypercalcaemia
-
Abnormally high calcium concentration in the blood.
- Hyperreflexia
-
Exaggerated deep tendon reflexes.
- Strabismus
-
Eye misalignment; also known as 'crossed eyes'.
- Nystagmus
-
Involuntary and often rapid and repetitive oscillatory movements of the eyeballs.
- Homologous recombination
-
Exchange of DNA segments of similar sequence. Occurs by breakage and reunion in paired chromosomes during meiosis.
- (Arnold-)Chiari malformations
-
A group of disorders characterized by protrusion of the cerebellum through the large opening in the base of the skull into the spinal canal.
- Differential Ability Scales-School Age
-
(DAS-School Age). A standardized assessment of general intellectual functioning designed to provide specific information about an individual's strengths and weaknesses across a wide range of intellectual abilities. It is particularly appropriate for assessing individuals with WS because it yields separate standard scores for verbal, nonverbal reasoning and spatial abilities, as well as an overall standard score (general conceptual ability (GCA), which is similar to IQ).
- Voxel-based morphometry
-
(VBM). A widely used method for the analysis of imaging data that enables a statistically principled voxel-wise between-groups comparison of local grey matter volume, unconstrained by anatomical landmarks.
- Retinotopic mapping
-
A functional imaging technique that can be used to delineate the extent of visual brain areas by capitalizing on the fact that they represent retinal information in a consistent spatial map.
- Long-term potentiation
-
(LTP). Enduring increase in the amplitude of excitatory postsynaptic potentials as a result of high-frequency stimulation of afferent pathways; LTP has been most studied in the hippocampus.
- Theory of mind
-
The ability to interpret people's behaviour in terms of their mental states. Includes both social–perceptual (capacity to distinguish between people and objects, and to infer mental disposition from facial, prosodic and body expressions) and social–cognitive (explicit representation of and reasoning about others' beliefs and intentions) components.
- Endophenotype
-
A quantitative biological trait associated with a complex genetic disorder that is hoped to more directly index the underlying pathophysiology, facilitating efforts to find or characterize contributing genes.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Mervis, C. & Faith Berman, K. Neural mechanisms in Williams syndrome: a unique window to genetic influences on cognition and behaviour. Nat Rev Neurosci 7, 380–393 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1906
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1906
This article is cited by
-
In individuals with Williams syndrome, dysregulation of methylation in non-coding regions of neuronal and oligodendrocyte DNA is associated with pathology and cortical development
Molecular Psychiatry (2023)
-
Atypical deletion of Williams–Beuren syndrome reveals the mechanism of neurodevelopmental disorders
BMC Medical Genomics (2022)
-
Gene mapping methodology powered by induced genome rearrangements
Scientific Reports (2022)
-
An EEG investigation of alpha and beta activity during resting states in adults with Williams syndrome
BMC Psychology (2021)
-
Williams syndrome
Nature Reviews Disease Primers (2021)