Introduction

Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a commonly diagnosed disorder affecting approximately 5–7% of children and 4% of adults in the United States (Kessler et al, 2006; Willcutt, 2012). Individuals with ADHD smoke at higher rates than the general population (Molina and Pelham, 2003; Kessler et al, 2006), initiate smoking at an earlier age (Milberger et al, 1997), and have higher levels of nicotine dependence (Pomerleau et al, 1995). Furthermore, adults with ADHD report greater craving and withdrawal symptoms upon quitting smoking (McClernon et al, 2011) and are more likely to relapse (Humfleet et al, 2005).

One hypothesis for the strong links between ADHD and smoking is that deficits of inhibitory control—or the ability to withhold prepotent/overlearned responses—may contribute to increased smoking reinforcement and relapse in this patient population (McClernon and Kollins, 2008a). Inhibitory control deficits are a central defining feature of ADHD (Barkley, 1997). Moreover, smoking abstinence worsens inhibitory control (Kozink et al, 2010; Ashare and Hawk, 2012; Kollins et al, 2013), and abstinence-induced deficits in inhibitory control have been shown to predict relapse (Powell et al, 2010; Froeliger et al, 2017). Importantly, inhibitory control deficits associated with smoking abstinence may be exacerbated among smokers with ADHD. In one study, overnight abstinence resulted in worse inhibitory control among ADHD as compared with non-ADHD smokers (McClernon et al, 2008b). Furthermore, smokers with ADHD exhibited greater smoking-reinforced responding (measured via Progressive Ratio Task) during abstinence compared with non-ADHD smokers, and smoking-reinforced responding was correlated with abstinence-induced deficits in inhibitory control (Kollins et al, 2013). As such, individuals with ADHD may be more reliant on smoking than non-ADHD smokers to avoid abstinence-induced deficits in inhibitory control, and further, abstinence-induced deficits may make avoidance of relapse more difficult.

Despite documenting the effects of smoking abstinence on inhibitory control deficits among ADHD smokers, little is known about the neurobiology underlying these effects. fMRI and PET studies suggest that inhibitory control is subserved by dopaminergic circuits in the striatum and cortical regions, including the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and right inferior frontal gyrus (rIFG) (Chikazoe et al, 2009; Congdon et al, 2010; Ghahremani et al, 2012). Individuals with ADHD are thought to exhibit altered dopamine transmission as evidenced by reduced dopamine D2/D3 receptor availability and attenuated dopamine receptor binding following MPH administration within the striatum and midbrain (Volkow et al, 2007, 2009). In addition, multiple fMRI studies have demonstrated attenuated frontostriatal activation during inhibitory control (Cubillo et al, 2012; Hart et al, 2013) or increased activation in parietal or cerebellar regions, suggesting recruitment of compensatory attentional processes among children and adults with ADHD relative to healthy controls (Dillo et al, 2010). Nicotine withdrawal is also associated with perturbation of dopaminergic neurotransmission (Zhang et al, 2012) and changes in task-related brain activation in executive control regions, including the rIFG, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), and ACC (Ashare et al, 2014; McClernon et al, 2015). In a previous study of non-ADHD smokers, we observed that smoking abstinence decreased inhibitory control and event-related activation in rIFG (Kozink et al, 2010). Based on these results, we hypothesized that smoking abstinence further decreases inhibitory control among ADHD smokers through its influence on relevant circuitry including the rIFG and ACC.

The present study was designed to evaluate the influence of smoking abstinence on behavioral and neural correlates of inhibitory control among smokers with and without ADHD. Participants were carefully screened to identify daily smokers who met diagnostic criteria for ADHD (ADHD Group) or who did not exhibit any DSM-IV Axis I disorder aside from nicotine dependence (Control Group). Three fMRI scans were conducted: once following smoking as usual (satiated) and twice following 24 h smoking abstinence. In one of the abstinence conditions, participants were scanned following administration of 40 mg methylphenidate (MPH); the other followed administration of placebo. During scanning, participants completed a modified Go/No-Go task that allows for assessment of both sustained and transient neural correlates of inhibitory control. We hypothesized that smoking abstinence would differentially alter inhibitory control behavior and neural activation in smokers with ADHD and that MPH would counteract these changes.

Materials and methods

Overview of Study Design

Participants completed five total sessions. First, a screening session was conducted, during which participants read and signed an Institutional Review Board approved consent form, and eligibility was determined. Participants then completed an fMRI training and medical safety session, which assessed tolerability of MPH effects and provided task training and acclimation to the scanner environment. Participants then completed three separate fMRI sessions under the following conditions: (1) Smoking as usual+placebo pill (SAT+PLAC); (2) 24 h abstinent+placebo pill (ABS+PLAC); and (3) 24 h abstinent+40 mg methylphenidate (ABS+MPH). FMRI sessions were held a minimum of 4 days apart (M=10.7, SD=6.4), and order was counterbalanced and randomly assigned. Participants were compensated up to $940 for completing all sessions.

Participants

Participants were right-handed, non-treatment-seeking smokers recruited from the community who were in good general health (determined by medical evaluation), smoked at least 10 cigarettes per day, had expired carbon monoxide (CO) 10 or positive urine cotinine 100 μl/ml, and met additional DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for ADHD (ADHD Group) or no diagnoses aside from nicotine dependence (Control Group) as determined by a comprehensive psychiatric evaluation conducted by a PhD-level psychologist (see Supplementary Methods and Materials). Participants were excluded if they had a current primary psychiatric disorder other than ADHD or nicotine dependence or current use of psychoactive medications (other than ADHD pharmacotherapies among ADHD participants). Participants currently receiving pharmacotherapy for ADHD (n=5 Adderall, n=1 Concerta+MPH prn) were eligible to participate if they agreed, in consultation with their medical provider, to a washout period of at least five times the half-life of their current medication prior to the training session and for the duration of participation. See Supplementary Methods and Materials for additional exclusion criteria.

Study Drug and Medication Safety

All participants completed a 4-h MPH safety session prior to scanning, during which MPH was administered via 40 mg capsule, and vitals and self-reported symptoms were assessed to ensure safety and tolerability (see Supplementary Methods and Materials for additional details). During the imaging sessions, drug was administered under partial double-blind conditions. Participants were provided with a blinded capsule (MPH or placebo) at all imaging sessions. Experimenters were aware that the capsule was a placebo in the satiated condition but were unaware of the capsule condition in the abstinent conditions.

fMRI Scanning Sessions

At the start of each scanning session, breath CO was obtained (CO5 was required for abstinent sessions) and MPH or placebo drug was administered. After 75 min, participants were escorted to the fMRI suite. In the satiated condition, participants also smoked one usual brand cigarette approximately 30 min prior to scanning. Immediately prior to entering the scanner, participants completed a modified version of the Shiffman–Jarvik Withdrawal Questionnaire (Shiffman and Jarvik, 1976).

Inhibitory Control Task

We used a modified version of a Go/No-Go task (see Figure 1) developed and validated by Chikazoe et al (2009). Unlike standard Go/No-Go tasks, this task features both frequent and rare Go stimuli, which allows for an evaluation of brain activation in response to No-Go stimuli (which are rare), controlling for Rare-Go trials, thus isolating inhibitory control from novelty detection. Stimuli consisted of two different Go signals (frequent=gray circles and rare=yellow or blue circles) and a No-Go signal (blue or yellow circles). Participants were randomly assigned to either ‘blue’ or ‘yellow’ versions of the task, denoting which color stimulus represented the Rare-Go vs No-Go signals; once assigned, colors were held constant across sessions for each participant. In our modified version, stimuli were presented in two different block types in order to also evaluate sustained inhibitory control: In Go-Only (GO) Blocks, stimuli were Frequent-Go (75% of trials) and Rare-Go (25% of trials) trials. In All-Trial (AT) Blocks, stimuli were Frequent-Go (75%), Rare-Go (12.5%), and No-Go (12.5%) trials. Each block consisted of 56 trials and was completed 3 times per run. Blocks were interleaved with 13-s fixation and 2-s instructions indicating ‘All Trials’ or ‘Go Only’. Participants completed two 6.25-min runs.

Figure 1
figure 1

Example of stimuli presented during Go-Only and All-Trial Blocks of the Go/No-Go task. Stimuli are presented for 400 ms, with 400 ms intertrial interval. Stimuli were presented in blocks, each lasting 44.8 s and consisting of 56 trials. Go-Only blocks include frequent Go stimuli (gray circles, 75% of trials) and rare Go stimuli (yellow circles, 25% of trials). All-Trial Blocks include frequent Go stimuli (gray circles, 75% of trials), rare Go stimuli (yellow circles, 12.5% of trials), and No-Go stimuli (blue circles, 12.5% of trials). Colors of rare Go and No-Go stimuli were counter-balanced across participants. A full color version of this figure is available at the Neuropsychopharmacology journal online.

PowerPoint slide

fMRI Acquisition

BOLD functional MR images were acquired using a 3 T GE Signal EXCITE HD scanner equipped with 40 mT/m gradients (Waukesha, WI). A gradient-recalled inward spiral pulse imaging sequence (SENSE spiral) collected 34 interleaved slices (TR=1.5 s, TE=30 ms, flip angle=60°, acquisition matrix=64 × 64, field of view=24.0 cm2, voxel size=3.75 × 3.75 × 4 mm3). Participants completed a 5-min resting state scan (reported separately) prior to Go/No-Go functional runs. After functional data collection, a T1-weighted 3D structural image was acquired with a fast spoiled-gradient recalled sequence for 162 slices (TR=7.584 ms, TE=2.936, flip angle=12, acquisition matrix=256 × 256, and voxel size=1 × 1 × 1).

fMRI Data Analysis

Images underwent preprocessing in FSL version 5.0.1: (1) rigid-body motion correction using MCFLIRT, (2) slice timing correction, (3) high-pass filtering, (4) registration to standard space using FLIRT, and (5) spatial smoothing using an 8 mm FWHM Gaussian kernel.

Sustained inhibitory control

Each block type (AT, GO) was modeled as a boxcar function convolved with double-γ hemodynamic response function. A contrast image was created for AT>GO blocks in order to isolate brain activation specific to sustained inhibitory control. Second-level models created a mean contrast image across both runs for each participant.

Transient inhibitory control

Events were modeled at the time of event onset convolved with a double-γ hemodynamic response function. Standard motion parameters and two nuisance regressors were included in the model: errors of commission on No-Go trials and correctly identified Rare-Go stimuli during the GO blocks. Two regressors of interest in the AT blocks (correctly identified No-Go and Rare-Go stimuli) were contrasted and carried forward to hypothesis testing. In order to be classified as correct, No-Go stimuli were required to be preceded by a correct Go trial. Frequent-Go stimuli across both block types served as the baseline.

Hypothesis testing

In order to increase the interpretability of complex interaction effects in the absence of a full-factorial design, both event-related and block contrasts were entered into two separate 2 (Group) × 2 (Condition) ANOVAs using mixed effects (Flame1) analysis in FSL. First, effects of abstinence were examined by comparing SAT+PLAC with ABS+PLAC conditions. In a second model, effects of MPH during abstinence were examined by comparing ABS+MPH with ABS+PLAC conditions. Thresholding for significance was defined as Z=2.3, with cluster correction of P<0.05 across the whole brain.

Results

Participant Characteristics

A total of 67 participants passed all inclusion and exclusion criteria and were enrolled in the study. Of these, 35 subjects (n=17 ADHD; n=18 controls) completed all sessions and provided usable data on primary outcomes (see Supplementary Methods and Materials for details on exclusions). ADHD and Control groups did not differ on any smoking or demographic variables except race (ADHD group=17.6% non-white; Control group=50% non-white; P=0.04) (see Supplementary Table S1 for participant characteristics). As expected, the ADHD group had higher CAARS DSM-IV t-scores for Inattentive, Hyperactive/Impulsive, and Total subscales (all P<0.001) than the control group. Within the ADHD group, n=8 were diagnosed with predominantly inattentive type, n=9 were combined type.

Withdrawal and Craving

Group × Condition interactions were observed for Craving, F(2,72)=5.73, P<0.005 (Figure 2a) and Habit Withdrawal, F(2,72)=3.32, P<0.05 (Supplementary Figure S1), due to the ADHD group reporting higher symptom levels in the ABS+PLAC condition compared with the Control group. See Supplementary Information for additional details on withdrawal and CO findings.

Figure 2
figure 2

Prescan ratings of craving on the Shiffman–Jarvik Withdrawal Scale (SJWS) (a) and No-Go trial accuracy (b) by group (ADHD, Control) and condition (SAT+PLAC, ABS+MPH, ABS+PLAC). (a) A significant group-by-condition interaction was observed; ADHD smokers reported greater craving than Controls during ABS+PLAC, t(72)=2.23, P<0.05, and ABS+MPH, t(72)=3.01, P<0.005, conditions. (b) A main effect of condition was observed such that No-Go accuracy was higher during ABS+MPH relative to ABS+PLAC condition; pairwise comparisons indicated a significant effect of ABS+MPH>ABS+PLAC on No-Go accuracy for ADHD smokers, t(72)=2.3, P<0.05, but not for Control smokers, t(72)=0.5, NS.

PowerPoint slide

Task Performance

Overall effects of task

Accuracy on GO trials was uniformly high (>90%) across all conditions and block types (see Supplementary Table S2 for additional details). Accuracy was higher during GO blocks compared with AT blocks for both Frequent-Go, F(1,36)=24.44, P<0.0001, and Rare-Go Trials, F(1,36)=26.41, P<0.0001. A similar effect of Block Type was seen for RTs for both Frequent-Go, F(1,36)=198.18, P<0.0001, and Rare-Go trials, F(1,36)=311.16, P<0.0001, with participants responding faster during GO blocks compared with AT blocks.

Effects of smoking abstinence

No effects of condition or group were observed between ABS+PLAC and SAT+PLAC for accuracy on any trial type. Notably, RTs were slower in the ABS+PLAC compared with the SAT+PLAC for both Frequent-Go, F(1,33)=7.67, P<0.01, and Rare-Go trials, F(1,33)=6.23, P=0.02. Moreover, a Condition-by-Block-Type interaction was observed for Frequent, F(1,33)=6.5, P=0.02 and Rare-Go RTs, F(1,33)=5.45, P=0.03, such that abstinence slowed RTs primarily during GO blocks. No Group or Group-by-Condition interaction were observed for RT for either Frequent or Rare-Go Trials.

Effects of methylphenidate

As shown in Figure 2b, we observed an effect of Condition such that accuracy was better in the ABS+MPH condition compared with the ABS+PLAC condition, F(1,36)=5.34, P<0.05. ADHD status did not moderate this effect. RTs on Frequent-Go Trials were slower in the ABS+PLAC compared with ABS+MPH conditions, F(1,33)=4.72, P=0.04.

Neuroimaging Results

Sustained inhibitory control

Overall Task Effects. The contrast of AT>GO blocks was associated with bilateral activation in multiple brain regions, including superior parietal cortex, occipital cortex (OCC), angular gyrus, prefrontal cortex (PFC) including supplementary motor area, ACC, precentral gyrus, DLPFC, and rIFG, and thalamus, putamen, anterior insula, brain stem, and cerebellum (Supplementary Table S3; Supplementary Figure S2).

Effects of Smoking Abstinence. As shown in Figure 3a, a significant Group (ADHD, Control) × Condition (SAT+PLAC, ABS+PLAC) effect was observed in the left OCC (Zmax=4.18; k=1189 voxels; peak voxel=−22, −86, 40), including portions of occipital pole, cuneus, and lateral superior occipital cortex extending to superior parietal lobule. Post hoc analysis of BOLD signal extracted from this region (Figure 3b) suggested that the interaction was due to lower BOLD signal in the ADHD group in the ABS+PLAC condition relative to the SAT+PLAC condition (P=0.01), whereas the opposite pattern was observed for the Control group (P<0.05). Moreover, within the ABS+PLAC condition, BOLD signal was lower in the ADHD compared with Control group (P<0.01).

Figure 3
figure 3

(a) Significant group (ADHD, Control) by abstinence condition (SAT+PLAC, ABS+PLAC) interaction for sustained inhibitory control (All Trial blocks>Go Only blocks) in the left occipital cortex encompassing 1189 voxels, Zmax=4.18, peak voxel at −22, −86, 40. (b) Mean percentage of signal change extracted from left occipital cluster, plotted by group across SAT+PLAC and ABS+PLAC conditions. Error bars represent SEM.

PowerPoint slide

Effects of Methylphenidate. Evaluation of the effects of MPH on the AT>GO contrast revealed significant ABS+MPH>ABS+PLAC effects throughout bilateral superior parietal cortex, superior frontal gyrus, OCC, and insula (Zmax=4.99; k=50 270 voxels; peak voxel=−22, −50, 68; Supplementary Figure S3; Supplementary Table S4). No interaction between Group and MPH was observed.

Correlation with Task Performance. Given the above-observed effects, we evaluated correlations between abstinence or MPH-induced changes in BOLD activation and task performance. As shown in Figure 4, positive correlations between abstinence-induced changes in inhibitory control accuracy and BOLD signal were observed in the full sample in multiple brain regions, including bilateral OCC and insula, right superior parietal cortex, and temporal gyrus (Supplementary Table S5). In these brain regions, decreased accuracy during smoking abstinence was associated with decreased BOLD signal. Group differences in the pattern of correlation were not observed nor were negative correlations. No correlations were observed between MPH-induced changes in task performance and BOLD activation.

Figure 4
figure 4

Whole-brain correlations between abstinence-induced changes in No-Go trial accuracy (ABS+PLAC−SAT+PLAC difference for NoGo percent correct) and abstinence-induced changes in sustained inhibitory control (ABS+PLAC−SAT+PLAC difference for All Trial>Go Only block effects), shown in blue. Significant clusters of activation were observed in the left lateral occipital cortex (13 980 voxels, Zmax=4.65, peak voxel at −18, −84, 40); left planum polare/central opercular cortex (2541 voxels, Zmax=3.85, peak voxel at −56, −8, 4); and right insula (1269 voxels, Zmax=4.83, peak voxel at 32, 12, 2). Activation map overlayed on occipital cortex cluster exhibiting group-by-abstinence condition interaction for sustained inhibitory control (Figure 3a), shown in red. Areas shown in green represent overlap between blue and red activation maps. A full color version of this figure is available at the Neuropsychopharmacology journal online.

PowerPoint slide

Transient inhibitory control

Task Effects. The contrast of No-Go>Rare-Go trials was associated with activation in superior parietal lobule, DLPFC, precentral gyrus, IFG, and frontal pole. Activation tended to be greater in the right than the left hemisphere. Bilateral activations were also observed in OCC, precuneus, middle temporal gyrus, supplementary motor area, anterior and posterior cingulate, putamen, anterior insula, and left cerebellum (Supplementary Table S6; Supplementary Figure S4).

Effects of Smoking Abstinence. No main effects of smoking condition or interactions with group were observed.

Effects of Methylphenidate. Administration of MPH (ABS+MPH>ABS+PLAC) resulted in increased No-Go>Rare-Go activation in a large cluster encompassing medial PFC, orbitofrontal cortex, and subgenual ACC extending to left hippocampus/parahippocampal gyrus (Zmax=4.08; k=11 546 voxels; peak voxel=14, 34, −6) and a cluster in precuneus/posterior cingulate (Zmax=3.76; k=1286 voxels; peak voxel=−20, −50, 22) (Supplementary Figure S5). Furthermore, a significant Group (ADHD, Control) by Condition (ABS+MPH, ABS+PLAC) interaction was observed in the left putamen (Zmax=3.19; k=1201 voxels; peak voxel=−20, 10, −6) (Figure 5a). Post hoc analysis of BOLD signal extracted from this region (Figure 5b) indicated that MPH increased BOLD signal in the ADHD group relative to the ABS+PLAC condition (P<0.001), whereas a non-significant trend in the opposite direction was observed for Controls (P=0.06).

Figure 5
figure 5

(a) Significant group (ADHD, Control) by methylphenidate condition (ABS+MPH, ABS+PLAC) interaction for transient inhibitory control (No-Go trials>Rare-Go trials) in left putamen encompassing 1201 voxels, Zmax=3.19, peak voxel at −20, 10, −6. (b) Mean percentage of signal change extracted from left putamen cluster, plotted by group across ABS+MPH and ABS+PLAC conditions. Error bars represent SEM.

PowerPoint slide

Discussion

This study is the first to examine the effects of smoking abstinence and methylphenidate on inhibitory control among smokers with and without ADHD, allowing us to probe the neurobiological mechanisms underlying comorbidity between smoking and ADHD. Our results provided compelling preliminary evidence that abstinence from smoking is associated with decreased activation in the OCC during sustained inhibitory control among ADHD smokers compared with non-ADHD smokers. Moreover, we found that abstinence-induced decreases in OCC activation were associated with decrements in inhibitory control performance in both ADHD and non-ADHD smokers.

We hypothesized that ADHD smokers would exhibit decreased task-related activation throughout key fronto-parietal regions, including rIFG and ACC, which have previously been shown to be involved in inhibitory control, during abstinence relative to both satiated and MPH conditions. Although the Go/No-Go task reliably recruited multiple regions throughout expected circuitry, group-by-abstinence interaction effects were restricted to OCC—an unexpected finding. However, recruitment of OCC is frequently observed in fMRI studies of inhibitory control (Watanabe et al, 2002; Simmonds et al, 2008), and activation in this region has been proposed to be the result of top–down modulation of visual processing from fronto-parietal regions involved in inhibitory and attentional control (Parks and Madden, 2013). This perspective is supported by findings that attention-modulated activation of visual cortex and performance on a visual discrimination task are directly correlated with structural connectivity with fronto-parietal regions (Bennett et al, 2012; Greenberg et al, 2012). Thus, in the context of the inhibitory control task in present study, such activation may reflect increased attentional demands required for successful visual discrimination during All-Trial blocks compared with simple motor performance during Go-Only blocks.

Our results also indicated that greater abstinence-induced decrements in No-Go trial accuracy correlated with abstinence-induced attenuation of OCC activation. That is, decreased OCC activation during abstinence was directly associated with a decline in behavioral performance, across both diagnostic groups. This finding provides further support for the role of OCC in inhibitory control and attentional processes and its disruption during smoking abstinence and suggests that this should be an important area of further study.

Although not typically a primary focus of investigation in studies of ADHD, converging evidence suggests a potential role for OCC in core attentional deficits in the disorder. Neuroimaging studies have routinely observed group differences between ADHD and control subjects in OCC activation in addition to other regions during tasks of inhibition or attention (see Castellanos and Proal, 2012 for a review). In addition, anatomical studies have observed decreased gray matter volume in primary visual cortex among adults with ADHD relative to controls (Ahrendts et al, 2011), and decreased cortical thickness across multiple regions, including PFC and OCC, was observed among persistent relative to remitted ADHD among adults in a 33-year longitudinal study (Proal et al, 2011). Furthermore, several studies have found evidence of altered structural (Pironti et al, 2014) and functional (Cocchi et al, 2012) connectivity between OCC and frontal regions among adults with ADHD vs controls.

Long-range reciprocal connections between PFC and visual cortex have been shown to be under dopaminergic control (Noudoost and Moore, 2011), and cocaine selectively increases extracellular dopamine in visual cortical regions responsible for processing visual stimuli (Muller and Huston, 2007). In the present study, administration of MPH—which increases extracellular dopamine and norepinephrine levels via blockade of catecholamine transporters (Volkow et al, 2005; Hannestad et al, 2010)—improved behavioral performance and robustly increased inhibitory control-related brain activation for both ADHD and non-ADHD smokers, including in some OCC regions shown to be attenuated among ADHD smokers during ABS+PLAC. Together, these findings suggest the possibility that smoking withdrawal in ADHD smokers may exacerbate deficits in dopamine transmission mediating attentional control in visual cortex and that MPH may help to offset these deficits.

MPH administration also robustly increased transient inhibitory control-related activation throughout PFC in both groups, whereas similar effects were observed in the putamen only for ADHD smokers. These results are remarkably consistent with a previous study demonstrating robust MPH-induced increases in inhibitory control-related activation throughout PFC in both ADHD and control boys, but opposing effects of MPH in the caudate and putamen, such that striatal activation was increased by MPH in the ADHD group but decreased in the control group (Vaidya et al, 1998). Such effects may be due to MPH’s affinity for norepinephrine transporter binding in the PFC, leading to increased norepinephrine transmission in these regions (Berridge et al, 2006; Engert and Pruessner, 2008). By contrast, dopaminergic effects in the striatum may vary as a function of baseline dopamine activity or underlying deficits (Vaidya et al, 1998; Engert and Pruessner, 2008). However, given nonspecific effects of MPH on catecholamine transmission, further research is needed to disentangle the contribution of specific catecholamine transmission on withdrawal or MPH effects among smokers with ADHD.

Although a main effect of smoking abstinence was not observed for No-Go accuracy, whole-brain correlation analyses revealed multiple brain areas in which decreased activation during abstinence was associated with abstinence-induced decrements in performance. This pattern was consistent across both groups and included areas of OCC, discussed above, as well as regions including bilateral insula, right superior parietal cortex, and superior temporal gyrus. The insula, in particular, has been repeatedly implicated in studies of nicotine dependence (Janes et al, 2010; Addicott et al, 2015), as well as studies of inhibitory control (Xue et al, 2008), and has been proposed as a primary region linking interoceptive awareness and cognitive control processes. Previous studies have demonstrated an association between weaker functional connectivity within a network containing the insula and other regions involved in motor planning and cognitive control and greater likelihood of relapse during a smoking quit attempt (Janes et al, 2010; Addicott et al, 2015). Although speculative, the current results are consistent with a role for the insula in response inhibition among smokers, which may be contribute to deficits in cognitive control during withdrawal.

The present findings also raise questions about the clinical utility of MPH as a smoking cessation aid. Our results suggested that nicotine withdrawal and MPH have opposing neuromodulatory effects, but we observed a lack of robust effects of MPH on subjective craving and withdrawal. These findings are consistent with the results of recent randomized controlled trials evaluating MPH and another stimulant (lisdexamfetamine dimesylate), which have observed negative results on smoking outcomes despite improvements in ADHD symptoms (Winhusen et al, 2010; Kollins et al, 2014). However, individual differences in efficacy of MPH as a smoking cessation adjunct have been observed among ADHD smokers (Covey et al, 2011), raising the possibility that MPH might improve inhibitory control and smoking outcomes among a subset of ADHD smokers, potentially those with greater disruptions in visual attention. Furthermore, it is worth noting that all of these studies included concurrent treatment with nicotine patch, whereas our study did not examine effects of MPH under satiated conditions. Thus it is unclear whether MPH may improve outcomes in a subset of ADHD smokers attempting to quit without the aid of nicotine replacement.

The present study had several strengths, including evaluation of neurocognitive function across three separate pharmacological conditions, comprehensive diagnostic assessment for both ADHD and control groups, and use of medication wash-out period prior to testing. However, several limitations are also noted. Given the clinical sample and use of repeated sessions, sample size within each group was relatively small, limiting conclusions that can be drawn without further replication. Sample size also prevented examining the effects of medication treatment history, which could have contributed to residual effects in the ADHD group despite the medication wash-out period. Unintended group differences in racial composition presented an additional potential confound. We did not examine ADHD subtypes or sex differences, which may be important considerations for future studies, particularly given the moderating role of ovarian hormones in nicotine reinforcement and ADHD symptoms (Van Voorhees et al, 2012). Finally, our study design did not allow us to examine dose–response effects of MPH or effects of prolonged exposure, and lack of a satiation+MPH condition prevented determination of whether MPH effects were specific to abstinence or reflected drug effects more broadly.

Although beyond the scope of the present analyses, an important future direction will be to determine whether differences in visual attention in OCC are reflective of broader differences in functional connectivity with fronto-parietal regions involved in cognitive control. Furthermore, it will be important to evaluate whether the effects of abstinence on neural markers of inhibitory control generalize to smoking-related contexts or are predictive of cessation outcomes in this patient population. For example, diminished activation in OCC during attentional control of visual stimulus processing may lead to greater attentional bias to smoking cues (Janes et al, 2010), thereby predisposing ADHD smokers to relapse. It is important to note that the extent to which response inhibition in the context of the Go/No-Go task relates to inhibitory control and decision-making in real-world contexts is unclear. However, recent work has demonstrated an association between BOLD activation during the Go/No-Go task and ability to refrain from smoking in both laboratory and real-world settings (Froeliger et al, 2017), suggesting an important link with clinical outcomes. Additional research is needed to clarify how common or dissociable mechanisms underlying different aspects of inhibitory control confer vulnerability to smoking among individuals with ADHD.

Overall, our findings provide important preliminary evidence on potential neurobiological mechanisms underlying comorbidity between smoking and ADHD. Specifically, abstinence from smoking is associated with decreased activation in OCC during sustained inhibition among ADHD smokers compared with non-ADHD smokers that is associated with inhibitory control performance. These findings advance our understanding of ADHD–smoking comorbidity and suggest important areas for future investigation, including replication in a larger sample, functional connectivity analysis, and examining association between neurocognitive mechanisms and smoking behavior.

Funding and disclosure

This research was supported by NIH grants R01 DA024838 (to FJM), K24 DA023464 (to SHK), K23 DA042898 (to JAO), and K23 DA039294 (to MMS). SHK has received research support and/or consulting fees from the following commercial sources in the past 24 months: Purdue Canada, Akili Interactive, Alcobra, Bose, Jazz, Ironshore, Medgenics, Sunovion, Rhodes, Shire, SK Life Sciences, KemPharm, and NLS Pharma. The other authors declare no conflict of interest.