INTRODUCTION

The isoprenoid/cholesterol biosynthetic pathway, also known as the mevalonate (MVA) pathway, is one of the most notorious metabolic processes as it leads to the production of cholesterol and other non-sterol isoprenoids, which are essential in the induction and the maintenance of several cellular processes. The key enzyme of this pathway is the 3-hydroxy-3-methylglutaryl coenzyme A reductase (HMGR) (Brown and Goldstein, 1980; Segatto et al, 2013). The role of the MVA pathway is well-established in the liver, where a major part of lipid metabolism takes place (Horton, 2002; Pallottini et al, 2007; Segatto et al, 2013). However, this metabolic pathway is ubiquitously expressed in all eukaryotic cells, and recent studies sustain a pivotal role for MVA end-products in the brain. Although the CNS constitutes the 2% of the body weight, it contains about 25% of the total body cholesterol (Pfrieger, 2003; Segatto et al, 2013). The majority of cholesterol is present in myelin sheaths and in neuronal membranes, where this lipid fulfills structural and functional tasks. Given the crucial role of cholesterol in regulating different neuronal processes, eukaryotes have developed sophisticated homeostatic mechanisms to preserve cholesterol levels in an optimal range in each brain region (Segatto et al, 2013). Thus, alterations in this essential equilibrium could lead to pathological consequences in the CNS, such as the Smith-Lemli-Opitz syndrome, Alzheimer’s, and Niemann-Pick type C diseases (Dietschy and Turley, 2004; Pfrieger, 2003; Segatto et al, 2011).

Besides cholesterol, isoprenoid compounds carry out important roles in the CNS. Prenylation, the covalent binding of farnesyl pyrophosphate (FPP) or geranylgeranyl pyrophosphate (GGPP) moieties to proteins, is a crucial post-translational modification for the regulation of protein localization on cell membranes and, in turn, for key cellular processes. Isoprenoids are not only required for protein prenylation but also constitute the side chain of Coenzyme Q (CoQ) (Matthews et al, 1998), whereas dolichols are involved in the N-linked glycosylation of proteins (Trapani et al, 2011b).

The essential role of MVA end-products in the CNS physiology is also supported by growing preclinical and clinical studies on the pleiotropic effects of statins in the brain. Statins are strong HMGR inhibitors widely prescribed in therapies against hypercholesterolemia and their benefits in preventing atherosclerosis and other cardiovascular diseases are incontrovertible. Recently, it has been reported that high-dose statin treatment induces effects in emotional, learning, and memory processes (Kilic et al, 2012; Douma et al, 2011; Baytan et al, 2008; While and Keen, 2010). However, the role of the MVA biosynthetic pathway in the modulation of emotional behavior and cognitive performance is still unclear because of the lack of systematic studies on the causal link between the activation of this metabolic pathway and behavioral and cognitive outcomes. As a consequence, the effects exerted by statins in the CNS still remain confusing and unconvincing. Moreover, no data about a low-dose statin treatment are available. A better understanding of these processes could be of great interest toward new pharmacological interventions for CNS disorders. Thus, the aim of the present work was to shed light on the role played by the MVA pathway in the modulation of emotional reactivity and memory consolidation in rodents, through the inhibition by simvastatin of HMGR both in vivo and in vitro.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Animals

Three-month-old male Wistar rats (Harlan Nossan, S Pietro al Natisone, Italy) were housed in groups of two and maintained under controlled temperature (20±1 °C), humidity (55±10%), and illumination (12/12 h light cycle with lights on at 0700 hours). Food and water were provided ad libitum. All procedures involving animal care or treatments were approved by the Italian Ministry of Health and performed in compliance with the guidelines of the US National Institutes of Health (NIH) and the Italian Ministry of Health (n° 231/2012-B, according to DL 116/92), the Declaration of Helsinki, the Guide for the Care and Use of Mammals in Neuroscience and Behavioral Research (National Research Council 2004) and the Directive 2010/63/EU of the European Parliament and of the Council of 22 September 2010 on the protection of animals used for scientific purposes.

Drug Treatment

Simvastatin (Sigma-Aldrich, Milan, Italy) was dissolved in a vehicle containing dimethyl sulfoxide—250 μl/kg body weight of 10% DMSO in sterile H2O (v/v)—and the dose of 1.5 mg/kg was daily administered by intraperitoneal injections for 3 weeks. Control animals were treated with vehicle only. Immediately after testing, rats were deeply anesthetized using Urethane (1.2 mg/kg) and plasma obtained from the blood collected into EDTA (1 mg/ml blood). Subsequently, rats were decapitated and their brains quickly removed. Brain regions of interest (amygdala, hippocampus, prefrontal cortex, dorsal, and ventral striatum) were collected and frozen in liquid nitrogen for subsequent biochemical analyses.

Plasma Cholesterol Analysis

Plasma cholesterol content was estimated by the colorimetric CHOD-POD kit in accordance to the manufacturer’s instructions (Assel, Rome, Italy).

Plasma Triglycerides Analysis

The amount of plasma triglycerides was assessed by using the Triglyceride Quantification Kit in accordance to the manufacturer’s instructions (BioVision, Mountain View, CA).

Behavioral Tests

The behavioral experiments were performed the day following the last administration of either simvastatin or vehicle. Different groups of rats were used for each behavioral test.

Social interaction test

The social interaction test was performed under dim light conditions in a Plexiglas arena (45 × 45 × 60 cm) with ∼2 cm of wood shavings covering the floor.

The test consisted in placing two similarly treated animals into the test cage for 10 min, with new sawdust as bedding. The animals in a pair did not differ more than 10 g in body weight; furthermore, they were housed in different cages and that therefore had no previous common social experience from the arrival in our Facility till the day of testing (File, 1980; Trezza et al, 2008).

The behavior of the animals was recorded on DVD for subsequent appropriate behavioral analysis carried out by the same observer, who was unaware of animal treatment, using the Observer 3.0 software (Noldus Information Technology BV, Wageningen, the Netherlands).

The total time spent in active social interaction was obtained as the sum of the time spent in the following behavioral elements scored per 10 min:

Play-related behaviors: (1) total time spent in pouncing (ie, when one animal attempts to nose or rub the nape of the neck of its play partner), which is an index of play solicitation; (2) total time spent in pinning (ie, the most common terminal component of a play bout, in which one animal stands over a supine partner), which is the consummatory measure of play (Panksepp et al, 1984; Trezza et al, 2010).

Social behaviors unrelated to play: total time spent in social exploration (sniffing any part of the body of the test partner, including the anogenital area; social grooming; following/chasing).

Elevated plus-maze

The elevated plus-maze test was performed as previously described (Pellow and File, 1986; Trezza et al, 2009). Briefly, the rats were individually placed on the central platform of the maze, facing a closed arm, and allowed to freely explore the maze for 5 min.

The 5 min test period was recorded on DVD for subsequent appropriate behavioral analysis carried out by the same observer, unaware of animal treatment, using the Observer 3.0 software (Noldus Information Technology BV, Wageningen, the Netherlands).

The following parameters were analyzed:

% time spent on the open arms (% TO): (seconds spent on the open arms of the maze/300) × 100;

% open arm entries (% OE): (the number of entries into the open arms of the maze/number of entries into open+center+closed arms) × 100;

Number of total arm entries (open+closed arm entries).

Inhibitory avoidance

The inhibitory-avoidance apparatus (Ugo Basile, Comerio, Italy) consisted of a Plexiglas cage with tilting floor, divided by a sliding door into two compartments (22 × 22 × 25 cm each). One of the compartments had white walls and was brightly illuminated by a 10-W bulb. The other compartment had black walls and was not illuminated. The tilting floor consisted of bars of stainless Steel connected to a source of scrambled shock. The procedure consisted of two sessions: acquisition and retention, that took place on 2 subsequent days (Mereu et al, 2003; Campolongo et al, 2007). For details see Supplementary Materials and Methods.

Lipid Extraction

Lipid extraction from brain regions (amygdala, hippocampus, prefrontal cortex, dorsal striatum, and ventral striatum) was assessed by following the previously described protocol (Trapani et al, 2011a).

HPLC-UV Analysis of Isoprenoids

Cholesterol, Coenzyme Q9 (CoQ9), Coenzyme Q10 (CoQ10), and dolichols were analyzed according to the previously described protocol (Tang et al, 2001) with modifications. For details see Supplementary Materials and Methods.

Lysate, Cytosol, and Membrane Preparation from the Brain Tissue

Lysate and membrane from the brain tissue were prepared slightly modifying our previously used protocol (Segatto et al, 2011). For details, see Supplementary Materials and Methods.

Synaptic Vesicle Preparation

Synaptic vesicles were prepared following the protocol described by Huttner et al (1983) with modifications. Rat brain regions (amygdala, hippocampus, prefrontal cortex, dorsal striatum, and ventral striatum) were homogenized in 10 vol of ice-cold HEPES-buffered sucrose (4 mM HEPES, 0.32 M sucrose, and 0.001 M PMSF, pH 7.4) with 10 strokes in a glass-Teflon homogenizer. Homogenates were centrifuged at 1000 g to remove nuclei, intact cells and cell debris (P1 fraction). The supernatant (S1) was spun at 10 000 g for 15 min to yield the crude synaptosomal pellet (P2). The supernatant (S2) was collected and subsequently centrifuged at 100 000 g for 15 min to produce the cytosolic fraction (S2′). The P2 fraction was then resuspended in 10 vol of HEPES-buffered sucrose and respun at 10 000 g for 15 min to obtain the washed crude synaptosomal fraction (P2′). The resulting pellet was lysed by hypoosmotic shock in 9 vol of ice-cold distilled water plus 0.001 M PMSF and three strokes of a glass-Teflon homogenizer. Four millimolars HEPES (pH 7.4) was rapidly added to the lysate which was mixed continuously in a cold room for 30 min to guarantee the total lysis of the sample. The lysate was subsequently centrifuged at 25 000 g for 20 min and the supernatant (S3) was collected and spun at 165 000 g for 2 h. The pellet of synaptic vesicles was finally solubilized in a sample buffer (0.125 M Tris-HCl, pH 6.8, containing 10% SDS, 0.001 M PMSF) and transferred into 1.5 ml Eppendorf tubes. Protein determination was assessed by the method of Lowry et al (1951). The synaptic vesicle samples were boiled for 3 min before loading for the western blotting method. In order to obtain an adequate amount of synaptic vesicles, each sample was made up from six brain regions pulled together from six different animals. The protein detection of synaptophysin (synaptic vesicle marker) and α-tubulin (cytosolic marker) verified and confirmed a high degree of purity of the synaptic vesicles (Supplementary Figure S1).

Rab3 ‘In Vitro’ Degradation Assay

Rab3 degradation assay was performed according to the protocol used by Pallottini et al (2004) with modifications. For details see Supplementary Materials and Methods.

Hippocampal Neuron Primary Cultures and Drug Treatment

Primary hippocampal neurons were isolated and cultured according to the previously described protocol (Oh et al, 2006). For details see Supplementary Materials and Methods.

Western Blotting Analysis

Western blot method was performed slightly modifying the protocol described by Trapani et al (2011a) by using the following antibodies: LDLr ab30532 (Abcam); SREBP-2 ab28482 (Abcam); NeuN A60 (Chemicon); GFAP 134B1 (Synaptic Systems); Rab3 G-1, CREB, p-CREB, H-Ras M90, RhoA 26C4, Akt1 B-1, RhoGDI A-20, RabGDI (V-20)-R, synaptophysin H-93, and caveolin N-20 (Santa Cruz Biotechnology); p-Akt 193H12 (Cell Signaling). For details see the Supplementary Materials and Methods.

Immunohistochemistry

Immunohistochemical staining of p-CREB and PSD-95 was performed on sagittal brain sections of simvastatin- and vehicle-treated animals according to the previously used protocols (Moreno et al, 1995; Fanelli et al, 2013). For details see Supplementary Materials and Methods.

Statistical Analysis

Data obtained from behavioral tests are expressed as means±SEM (standard error of the mean); data derived from the analysis of biochemical results are expressed as means±SD (standard deviation). Data were analyzed with unpaired Student’s t test or with one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) followed by the Dunnett post-test. Values of P<0.05 were considered to indicate a significant difference. Statistical analysis was performed using GRAPHPAD INSTAT3 (GraphPad, La Jolla, CA, USA) for Windows.

RESULTS

Systemic Effects of HMGR Inhibition on Lipid Metabolism

The inhibition of HMGR, and in turn the reduction in MVA end-products formation, leads to a homeostatic response that determines the increase of the low-density lipoprotein (LDL) receptor family members through the enhancement of their transcription and translocation onto cell membranes, subsequently reducing the amount of plasma lipids (Jones et al, 1998). Thus, the efficacy of the pharmacological treatment was checked by estimating the plasma cholesterol and triglycerides. As shown in Supplementary Table S1, both cholesterol (t34=3.606, P=0.001) and triglycerides (t34=4.562, P>0.0001) significantly decreased after simvastatin treatment (18% and 32%, respectively). Nevertheless, similar body weight of simvastatin- and vehicle-treated rats was observed, indicating that HMGR inhibition failed to induce changes in the basal metabolism of the animals (data not shown).

Effects of HMGR Inhibition on Emotional Reactivity and Cognitive Performance

To evaluate the potential role of the MVA pathway inhibition by simvastatin in the modulation of emotional reactivity, rats were tested in the social interaction and elevated plus-maze tests, two validated animal methods to evaluate anxiety in rodents.

Chronic simvastatin treatment decreased the total time spent in active social interaction compared with vehicle-treated rats (t18=4.123, P<0.001; Figure 1a), expressed as the sum of the time spent in play-related behaviors (t18=3.386, P=0.003; figure not shown) and in social behaviors unrelated to play (t18=2.390, P=0.028; figure not shown). In contrast, the percentage of time spent in the open arms (t19=−1.349, P=0.193; Figure 1b), the percentage of open entries (t19=−1.062, P=0.302; Figure 1c), and the total arm entries (t19=1.768, P=0.093; Figure 1d) evaluated in the elevated plus-maze test were unaffected by the pharmacological inhibition of HMGR.

Figure 1
figure 1

Effects of chronic simvastatin treatment in the social interaction, elevated plus-maze, and inhibitory avoidance tests. Simvastatin treatment (1.5 mg/kg per day; i.p.) reduced the total time spent in active social interaction (a). No statistically significant difference was found in (b) the percentage of time spent in the open arms, (c) the percentage of open entries and in (d) the total arm entries evaluated in the elevated plus-maze test. Simvastatin treatment (e) had no effects on the approach latency in the acquisition trial of the inhibitory avoidance task, whereas (f) enhanced 24-h avoidance latencies in the retention trial. (g) No statistically significance difference was found between simvastatin-treated rats and their controls in response to shock delivered during the acquisition trial of the inhibitory avoidance test. Data represent mean values±SEM. *P<0.05, ***P<0.001 vs control (n=10–11 per treatment group). Veh, vehicle-treated rats. Sim, simvastatin-treated rats.

PowerPoint slide

To investigate whether the MVA pathway has a role in the modulation of memory consolidation, rats were tested in the inhibitory avoidance task. Chronic simvastatin treatment had no effect on the approach latencies during the acquisition trial (t17=−0.620, P=0.544; Figure 1e); however, it caused a statistically significant improvement in 24-h retention performance (t17=−2.245, P=0.038; Figure 1f). This effects were not secondary to drug-induced hypersensivity to the electrical shock delivered during the acquisition trial, as simvastatin- and vehicle-treated rats did not differ in the response to the aversive stimulus during the acquisition trial (t17=−0.204, P=0.841; Figure 1g).

Simvastatin Efficacy and Tolerance in the CNS

Evidence for the drug efficacy in the CNS were given by checking the levels of the active nuclear fraction of the transcription factor SREBP-2 (nSREBP-2) and the subsequent increase in LDL receptor (LDLr), which are induced by a compensative response due to intracellular cholesterol decrease (Brown and Goldstein, 1997; Trapani et al, 2011a). The biochemical analysis was mainly carried out on the amygdala, hippocampus, prefrontal cortex, dorsal striatum, and ventral striatum, whose interplay is deeply involved in the modulation of anxiety, memory, and learning (Mathew et al, 2001; LaBar and Cabeza, 2006). Our results showed that HMGR inhibition by chronic simvastatin treatment induced a classical feedback response, which led to a strong increase of the active nSREBP-2 in all the brain regions analyzed (amygdala: t10=2.139, P=0.0291; hippocampus: t10=2.434, P=0.0176; prefrontal cortex: t10=2.590, P=0.0135; dorsal striatum: t10=2.045, P=0.034; ventral striatum: t10=2.637, P=0.0124; Figure 2a). HMGR inhibition was also supported by the subsequent and the contributory increase in LDLr expression (amygdala: t10=2.263, P=0.0236; hippocampus: t10=2.057, P=0.0334; prefrontal cortex: t10=1.933, P=0.0410; dorsal striatum: t10=1.909, P=0.0427; and ventral striatum: t10=3.969, P=0.0013; Figure 2b). Moreover, the direct proof of the pharmacological inhibition was also given by assessing the HMGR activity (amygdala: t8=7.694, P<0.0001; hippocampus: t8=1.266, P<0.0001; prefrontal cortex: t8=7.259, P<0.0001; dorsal striatum: t8=4.934, P=0.0011; and ventral striatum: t8=5.092, P=0.0009; Supplementary Figure S2a), which was reduced in all the analyzed brain areas.

Figure 2
figure 2

Efficacy of 3-hydroxy 3-methylglutaryl Coenzyme A reductase (HMGR) inhibition by simvastatin and effects on neuronal and astrocytic content in different brain regions. (a) Representative western blot and densitometric analysis of nSREBP-2 in amygdala (Am), hippocampus (Hp), prefrontal cortex, dorsal striatum, and ventral striatum. (b) Representative western blot and densitometric analysis of low-density lipoprotein receptor (LDLr) in amygdala (Am), hippocampus (Hp), prefrontal cortex, dorsal striatum, and ventral striatum. (c) Representative western blot and densitometric analysis of the neuronal marker NeuN in amygdala (Am), hippocampus (Hp), prefrontal cortex, dorsal striatum, and ventral striatum. (d) Representative western blot and densitometric analysis of the astrocytic marker GFAP in amygdala (Am), hippocampus (Hp), prefrontal cortex, dorsal striatum, and ventral striatum. Protein levels were normalized with α-tubulin. The data are expressed as arbitrary units obtained by analyzing the protein bands with Image J software for Windows, for details see the main text. All the results obtained are presented as the mean±SD *P<0.05, **P<0.01; Student’s t-test with respect to the control group of the same brain region analyzed. n=6 animals/group. Veh, vehicle-treated rats. Sim, simvastatin-treated rats.

PowerPoint slide

Once simvastatin efficacy was ascertained, we focused on the potential toxic effects exerted by HMGR inhibition on different brain areas. Protein levels of the neuronal marker NeuN (amygdala: t10=1.627, P=0.0674; hippocampus: t10=0.9974, P=0.1710; prefrontal cortex: t10=1.271, P=0.1162; dorsal striatum: t10=5.005, P=0.3138; and ventral striatum: t10=0.8166, P=0.2166; Figure 2c) and of the astrocyte marker GFAP (amygdala: t10=0.5949, P=0.4769; hippocampus: t10=1.614, P=0.0688; prefrontal cortex: t10=1.358, P=0.1022; dorsal striatum: t10=0.8068, P=0.2193; and ventral striatum: t10=0.7905, P=0.2238; Figure 2d) were unchanged in the examined brain areas. The analysis of cleaved caspase-3 also showed that the protein levels of this executive caspase were unaffected by simvastatin treatment (amygdala: t10=0.5627, P=0.2930; hippocampus: t10=0.06597, P=0.4744; prefrontal cortex: t10=0.8944, P=0.1961; dorsal striatum: t10=0.7460, P=0.2364; and ventral striatum: t10=0.06921, P=0.4731; Supplementary Figure S2b).

Effect of Simvastatin on Cholesterol, Dolichols, and CoQs

A prospective perturbation in the MVA pathway end-products induced by HMGR inhibition could be at the root of the behavioral and cognitive effects observed in vivo.

Thus, lipid estimation was performed in order to evaluate the effect of simvastatin on the main sterol and non-sterol compounds. As observable in Table 1, tissue cholesterol (amygdala: t7=0.2031, P=0.8448; hippocampus: t8=0.3502, P=0.7352; prefrontal cortex: t8=0.4874, P=0.6391; dorsal striatum: t7=1.556, P=0.1636; and ventral striatum: t6=0.5193, P=0.6221), CoQ9 (amygdala: t6=0.1524, P=0.8838; hippocampus: t8=0.04377, P=0.9662; prefrontal cortex: t8=1.211, P=0.2605; dorsal striatum: t8=1.623, P=0.1433; and ventral striatum: t5=0.2318, P=0.8259), CoQ10 (amygdala: t6=0.06480, P=0.9504; hippocampus: t8=1.301, P=0.2294; prefrontal cortex: t8=1.294, P=0.2319; dorsal striatum: t8=1.440, P=0.1879; and ventral striatum: t5=0.2467, P=0.8149), and dolichol (amygdala: t8=1.805, P=0.1087; hippocampus: t8=0.007556, P=0.9942; prefrontal cortex: t8=0.9631, P=0.3637; dorsal striatum: t8=0.2134, P=0.8363; and ventral striatum: t6=0.8525, P=0.4266) were not affected by HMGR inhibition among the brain regions analyzed.

Table 1 Effect of Simvastatin on Lipid Composition of Selected Brain Regions

Prenylated Protein Involved in Synaptic Vesicle Release: Rab3 Determination

Simvastatin treatment could also induce a downregulation in the activity of small GTPases by affecting their prenylation. For the pivotal role in neurotransmitter release, Rab3 protein localization was analyzed. Rab3 fraction associated with the synaptic vesicle membranes was strongly reduced in the hippocampus (t2=3.675, P=0.0334; Figure 3a) and the prefrontal cortex (t2=7.178, P=0.0094; Figure 3a) after chronic simvastatin treatment, whereas no differences in the protein localization were observable in amygdala (t2=1.028, P=0.4120; Figure 3a), dorsal striatum (t2=1.592, P=0.1262; Figure 3a), and ventral striatum (t2=0.3310, P=0.3860; Figure 3a). It is well accepted that membrane vs cytoplasm localization of Rab3, and other prenylated small GTPases in general, mirrors the prenylation status and, as a consequence, the activity of the protein of interest (Seasholtz et al, 1999; Homma et al, 2008). Thus, the amount of Rab3 active fraction was expressed as the membrane:cytosol ratio. To further assess Rab prenylation, coimmunoprecipitation experiments between RabGDI and Rab3 were performed on cytosolic fractions. The level of prenylated Rab3 is strongly reduced in the hippocampus (t4=1.315, P=0.0002, Supplementary Figure S3a) and the prefrontal cortex (t4=6.759, P=0.0025, Supplementary Figure S3b) of simvastatin-treated rats. Moreover, the total amount of RabGDI both in the hippocampus (t6=1.422, P=0.2048, Supplementary Figure S3c) and the prefrontal cortex (t6=0.8552, P=0.4253, Supplementary Figure S3d) did not change between the two experimental groups. The lack of the concurrent accumulation of unprenylated Rab3 in the cytosolic fraction (generally a typical phenomenon observed after statin administration) is in line with the results obtained by other research groups, who demonstrated that, under specific conditions, some prenylated proteins could have a shorter half-life compared with the membrane-bound forms (Haklai et al, 1998; Indolfi et al, 2002). To test this hypothesis, an ‘in vitro’ degradation assay of Rab3 was performed. The results show that Rab3 was more susceptible to degradational events in the hippocampus and the prefrontal cortex, whereas no differences were detectable between the two experimental groups in the other brain regions (Figure 3b).

Figure 3
figure 3

Effect of 3-hydroxy 3-methylglutaryl Coenzyme A reductase (HMGR) inhibition on Rab3 membrane localization and degradation in different brain regions. (a) Representative Western blot and densitometric analysis of Rab3 in preparations of synaptic vesicle membranes from amygdala (Am), hippocampus (Hp), prefrontal cortex, dorsal striatum and ventral striatum. Rab3 active fraction was expressed as the membrane:cytosol ratio. Data are represented as arbitrary units obtained by analyzing the protein bands with Image J software for Windows, for details see the main text. All the results are expressed as the mean±SD *P<0.05 and **P<0.01; Student’s t test with respect to the control group of the same brain region analyzed. n=6 animals/group. Veh=vehicle-treated rats. Sim=simvastatin-treated rats. (b) Representative Western blot obtained by Rab3 degradation assay in cytosolic fractions from amygdala, hippocampus, prefrontal cortex, dorsal striatum and ventral striatum. The time course analysis was performed at 0, 1, 2, 4, 8, 16, and 24 h. The results are represented as arbitrary units obtained by evaluating the protein bands with Image J software for Windows, for details see the main text. n=6 animals/group. Veh=vehicle-treated rats. Sim=simvastatin-treated rats.

PowerPoint slide

Prenylated Proteins Involved in Long-Term Potentiation: Ras and RhoA Determinations

The enhanced memory consolidation in a 24 h inhibitory avoidance task could correlate with changes in long-term potentiation (LTP). Thus, prenylated proteins such as Ras and RhoA, involved in the modulation of this form of synaptic plasticity (Mazzucchelli and Brambilla, 2000; Rex et al, 2009) were evaluated. HMGR inhibition did not induce any statistically significant modification in Ras translocation (amygdala: t10=0.2375, P=0.4085; hippocampus: t10=0.2929, P=0.3881; prefrontal cortex: t10=0.2798, P=0.3927; dorsal striatum: t10=0.01021, P=0.4960; and ventral striatum: t10=0.1791, P=0.4307; Figure 4a), whereas it strongly reduced the active membrane-bound form of RhoA in the amygdala (t10=2.701, P=0.0111; Figure 4b) and the hippocampus (t10=2.601, P=0.0132; Figure 4b), with the contributory build-up of the protein in the cytosol. On the contrary, no differences were observable in the prefrontal cortex (t10=0.2867, P=0.3901; Figure 4b), the dorsal striatum (t10=1.442, P=0.0899; Figure 4b), and the ventral striatum (t10=0.2531, P=0.4026; Figure 4b). As for Rab3, coimmunoprecipitation experiments highlighted a reduction in RhoA/RhoGDI complexes in both the amygdala (t4=6.188, P=0.0035, Supplementary Figure S4a) and the hippocampus (t4=6.636, P=0.0027, Supplementary Figure S4b), without showing any change in the RhoGDI protein content (amygdala: t6=1.493, P=0.1860, Supplementary Figure S4c; hippocampus: t6=0.5134, P=0.6260, Supplementary Figure S4d). In addition, the reduction in RhoA translocation to the membrane was accompanied by a marked and significant increase in Akt activation state in both the amygdala (t10=2.726, P=0.0107; Figure 4c) and the hippocampus (t10=2.724, P=0.0107; Figure 4c), whereas no differences are detectable in the prefrontal cortex (t10=1.365, P=0.1010; Figure 4c), the dorsal striatum (t10=0.3652, P=0.3613; Figure 4c), and the ventral striatum (t10=0.1979, P=0.4236; Figure 4c). Similar results were also obtained from cell cultures. In primary rat hippocampal neurons, simvastatin treatment reproduced the reduction in RhoA active fraction observed in vivo, as demonstrated from the decrease in the membrane:cytosol ratio (F(6, 21)=0.4016, P=0.0078). The supplementation of the medium with either MVA (the direct product of HMGR) or geranylgeraniol (GG, one of the MVA pathway products and the substrate for RhoA prenylation) completely reversed the effect of the pharmacological inhibition of HMGR. On the other hand, the administration of MVA, GG, and Rho kinase (ROCK) inhibitor hydroxyfasudil (HF) alone did not cause any effect in the protein translocation as expected (Figure 5a). The estimation of Akt protein levels in primary neuronal culture strongly suggests that Akt phosphorylation was dependent on RhoA inhibition by simvastatin, as the coadministration of MVA and GG completely restored the basal levels of Akt activation state (F(6, 21)=37,02, P<0.0001). HF-mediated inhibition of ROCK, the main downstream effector of RhoA, further supports this hypothesis by mimicking the effect of simvastatin through the induction of Akt phosphorylation (Figure 5b). Moreover, Akt activation induced a significant increase in the phosphorylation of the transcription factor CREB (F(6, 21)=0.4849, P=0.003; Figure 5c).

Figure 4
figure 4

Effect of 3-hydroxy 3-methylglutaryl Coenzyme A reductase (HMGR) inhibition by simvastatin on Ras, RhoA membrane localization and Akt activation in different brain regions. (a) Representative Western blot and densitometric analysis of Ras in amygdala (Am), hippocampus (Hp), prefrontal cortex, dorsal striatum, and ventral striatum. Ras active fraction was expressed as the membrane:cytosol ratio. (b) Representative western blot and densitometric analysis of RhoA in amygdala (Am), hippocampus (Hp), prefrontal cortex, dorsal striatum, and ventral striatum. RhoA active fraction was expressed as the membrane:cytosol ratio. (c) Representative western blot and densitometric analysis of Akt phosphorylation/activation in amygdala (Am), hippocampus (Hp), prefrontal cortex, dorsal striatum, and ventral striatum. Protein levels were normalized with α-tubulin. The results are represented as arbitrary units obtained by analyzing the protein bands with Image J software for Windows, for details see the main text. All the data are expressed as the mean±SD *P<0.05; Student’s t-test with respect to the control group of the same brain region analyzed. n=6 animals/group. Veh, vehicle-treated rats. Sim, simvastatin-treated rats.

PowerPoint slide

Figure 5
figure 5

Akt phosphorylation and the consequent CREB activation are dependent on the RhoA/ROCK pathway. (a) Representative western blot and densitometric analysis of RhoA in primary hippocampal neurons. RhoA active fraction was expressed as the membrane:cytosol ratio. All the data are expressed as arbitrary units obtained by analyzing the protein bands with Image J software for Windows, for details see the main text. (b) Representative western blot and densitometric analysis of Akt phosphorylation/activation in primary hippocampal neurons. p-Akt protein levels were normalized with total Akt levels and α-tubulin. (c) Representative western blot and densitometric analysis of CREB phosphorylation/activation in primary hippocampal neurons. p-CREB protein levels were normalized with total CREB levels and α-tubulin. All the data are expressed as arbitrary units obtained by evaluating the protein bands with Image J software for Windows, for details see the main text. All the results represent the mean±SD of at least three different experiments. *P<0.05, **P<0.01, and ***P<0.001determined using one-way one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) followed by the Dunnett post-test. GG, geranylgeraniol; HF, hydroxyfasudil; MVA, mevalonate; Sim, simvastatin; Veh, vehicle.

PowerPoint slide

Immunohistochemical Analyses

Morphological analysis of brains from either simvastatin- or vehicle-treated rats revealed good preservation of structures and overall normal cytoarchitecture. Immunohistochemical localization of p-CREB showed wide neuronal distribution in the hippocampal formation of both treated and control groups. Immunostaining was mainly seen in the nucleus of pyramidal cells in CA1–CA3 fields and of granule cells in DG. In addition, mossy cells in the hylus were especially immunoreactive. Consistent with western blot results obtained from primary hippocampal neuron cultures, remarkably higher immunostaining intensity throughout the hippocampus was detected in simvastatin-treated brains, as compared with their untreated counterparts (Figure 6a). PSD-95 also immunolocalized to the pyramidal cell layer of CA1–CA3, to granule neurons of DG, and to hylar mossy neurons. However, at difference with p-CREB immunohistochemistry, the staining intensity was comparable between the two experimental conditions, not being affected by simvastatin treatment (Figure 6b).

Figure 6
figure 6

p-CREB and PSD-95 distribution in hippocampus of simvastatin- and vehicle-treated rats. (a) Overview of p-CREB immunohistochemistry in a sagittal section of rat brain. Hippocampal formation showed higher positivity in the nucleus of pyramidal cells in CA1–CA3 fields and of granule cells in DG. Mossy cells in the hylus were especially immunoreactive. (b) Overview of PSD-95 immunohistochemistry in a sagittal section of rat brain showing immunopositivity localized to pyramidal cell layer of CA1–CA3, to granule neurons of DG, and to hylar mossy neurons. GL, granule cell layer; Hp, hippocampus; ML, molecular layer; Or, stratum oriens; PoDG, polymorphic layer; Pyr, stratum pyramidale; Rad, stratum radiatum.

PowerPoint slide

DISCUSSION

The present study was performed to provide a deeper understanding of the CNS functional consequences following the pharmacological inhibition of the isoprenoid/cholesterol biosynthetic pathway in rodents. To this aim HMGR, the key and rate-limiting enzyme of this pathway, was inhibited in rats by chronic treatment with simvastatin, a powerful HMGR inhibitor, which is able to cross the blood–brain barrier because of its lipophilic properties (Saheki et al, 1994). To analyze the biological role of the MVA pathway on rat emotionality, we performed two of the most popular animal tests of anxiety currently used: the social interaction and the elevated plus-maze tests (File and Hyde, 1978; File, 1980; Pellow et al, 1985; Pellow and File, 1986). Chronic simvastatin treatment at a low dose induced a significant decrease in the total time spent in active social investigation in the social interaction test, whereas no significant differences were observed in the elevated plus-maze test. Although both the elevated plus-maze and the social interaction tests are two validated animal models to measure anxiety-like behaviors in rodents (File, 1980; File and Seth, 2003; Pinheiro et al, 2007; Pellow and File, 1986), it has been suggested that these tests evoke different states of anxiety in the laboratory animal (Gonzales et al, 1996; File, 1992) mediated by different neurobiological pathways (Cheeta et al, 2000; File et al, 2000; File et al, 2004), and thus they could have different sensitivity to simvastatin treatment. Specifically, it has been suggested that the elevated plus-maze test mimics a state of generalized anxiety, as it produces an approach/avoidance conflict because the animal is exposed to a novel situation that supposedly creates a conflict between the motivation to explore the environment and an unconditioned fear of novelty (File et al, 2004), and this may be the underlying mechanism rendering the elevated plus-maze test sensitive to anxiolytic-like drugs (Handley and McBlane, 1993; Pinheiro et al, 2007). On the other hand, in the social interaction test, the variable is the time spent by pairs of male rats in active social interaction and one rat influences the behavior of the other; in fact, when pairs of male rats are placed in a situation in which neither one has established its territory, they engage in active social interaction (File and Hyde, 1978). Nevertheless, the social interaction test is also sensitive to a number of environmental and physiological factors such as test conditions (light level and familiarity to the test arena) that can affect anxiety mimicking a state of anxiety most similar to that experienced in generalized anxiety disorder (File, 1980; File and Seth, 2003). Furthermore, our results showed that the pharmacological modulation of the MVA pathway end-products is also involved in the modulation of memory consolidation of aversive experiences; in particular, HMGR inhibition by simvastatin treatment led to a specific enhancement of the consolidation phase of the memory process, as the approach latency, measured during the first day of the test, did not differ between the two experimental groups. Our results also corroborate with existing and recently published data, showing that a higher dose of simvastatin (10 mg/kg) resulted in improved memory performance in the passive avoidance test (Douma et al, 2011). Even though it was demonstrated that simvastatin induces apoptosis in neurons and astrocytes (Marz et al, 2007), the drug, at a dose of 1.5 mg/kg, did not cause any cell loss as observable by the unchanged levels of NeuN, GFAP, and of the executive caspase-3, excluding that potential neurotoxic effects exerted by simvastatin could be responsible for the behavioral outcomes observed in the current study. The lack of toxic effects could be explained by the low dose of simvastatin used in the present study. In addition, the safety in terms of necrosis and/or apoptosis of this chronic pharmacological treatment was also demonstrated in our previous work (Trapani et al, 2011a). The estimation of tissue cholesterol, dolichol, and CoQs through HPLC analyses also excluded the involvement of these end-products in the onset of the behavioral and cognitive changes. The lack of any variation in the amount of these isoprenoids following HMGR inhibition is not surprising if it is considered that they possess very long half-lives in the brain (Andersson et al, 1999). Thus, a 3-week chronic simvastatin treatment, at the dose used in our study, might not be able to impair the physiological levels of cholesterol, dolichol, CoQ9, and CoQ10 in the rat brain areas taken into consideration. On the opposite, membrane-bound Rab3 in the synaptic vesicle fraction, which corresponds to the active fraction of the protein, resulted to be strongly decreased in both the hippocampus and the prefrontal cortex after chronic HMGR inhibition. It is well-known that Rab and Rho proteins require prenylation for membrane association and for binding to GDIs. GDIs hold prenylated proteins in the cytosol, acting not only as passive regulators of GTPase activity but also preventing their degradation (Boulter and Garcia-Mata, 2010; Mohamed et al, 2012). GDI capture of membrane-bound Rabs could be physically prevented by modifications in the cholesterol content within the membranes (Ganley and Pfeffer, 2006). Even though the amount of cholesterol into membranes was not evaluated in this work, GDI capture of prenylated Rab3 in cytosolic fractions excluded potential interferences of membrane cholesterol in RabGDI/Rab3 interaction, suggesting that the deregulation in Rab3 subcellular distribution is due, at least in part, to an impairment in Rab3 prenylation induced by HMGR inhibition. Moreover, Rab3 seem to be more susceptible to degradational events both in the hippocampus and the prefrontal cortex of simvastatin-treated rats. These findings lead us to hypothesize that the lack of prenylation could impair Rab3/RabGDI interaction in the cytosol, thus preventing the role of RabGDI in protecting Rab proteins from degradation. Although there is a conflicting evidence in literature about the modulation of the specific synaptic mechanisms, it is clear and well defined that Rab3 carries out important physiological roles in neurotransmitter release at a late step during the synaptic vesicle exocytosis (Geppert and Sudhof, 1998). Perturbations in Rab3 activity have already been established. For instance, deregulations in Mss4 (mammalian suppressor of Sec4), a regulator of Rab3 activity, are strongly linked to impairments in neurotransmitter release and to the appearance of neurodegenerative and psychological disorders in rodents such as depressive-like syndromes (Andriamampandry et al, 2002; Baskys et al, 2007; Blaveri et al, 2010). Given the important role of the hippocampus and the prefrontal cortex in the modulation of anxiety (Whitton and Curzon, 1990; Christianson et al, 2009), the decrease in Rab3 active fraction in both the brain regions as a molecular consequence of HMGR inhibition could be related to an impairment in the social behavior observed in the social interaction test and could represent a good explanation of the MVA pathway-related molecular mechanism underlying the social anxiety-related behavior observed in our study.

Besides the reduction in membrane-bound Rab3 protein levels, the role of the MVA pathway in emotional memory consolidation could be strongly dependent on the modulation of prenylated proteins that are crucial for the induction and the maintenance of LTP. In particular, a strong and statistically significant decrease in RhoA active fraction was observed in the amygdala and the hippocampus. As for Rab3, additional experiments based on RhoA/RhoGDI interaction further support the previous finding, indicating that the reduced amount of RhoA is the consequence of a defect in prenylation caused by HMGR pharmacological inhibition. Among Rho GTPases, RhoA has been implicated in key neurobiological processes, integrating extracellular and intracellular molecular signals to orchestrate refined and coordinated changes in gene expression and actin cytoskeleton, essential prerequisites for the neurite outgrowth and the modulation of synaptic connectivity (Gopalakrishnan et al, 2008; Lingor et al, 2007). For these reasons, it is not surprising that RhoA activity has been deeply related to the onset of developmental disabilities such as mental retardation (Ramakers and Storm, 2002). As RhoA is also considered a negative regulator of Akt, whose phosphorylation, and in turn activation, is a key and triggering event for LTP induction and consequent memory retention (Ming et al, 2002; Sui et al, 2008), Akt protein levels and phosphorylation state were analyzed. In the present work, the reduction in membrane-bound RhoA is accompanied by a sustained Akt activation. In order to fully confirm the causality between HMGR activity, RhoA activation, and Akt phosphorylation, an additional experiment was performed on cell culture. To this aim, primary hippocampal neurons were chosen as an experimental model in order to avoid the well-known incapability of some used compounds (eg, MVA) to cross the blood–brain barrier in vivo (Popjak et al, 1977). The rescue experiment performed on primary hippocampal neurons demonstrated that the modulation of Akt is strictly dependent on the RhoA/ROCK pathway, whose activation could be heavily affected by HMGR inhibition. A role of Akt in the modulation of cognitive performances in rodents has already been proposed, as it is able to induce LTP and other synaptic plasticity phenomena in both the amygdala and the hippocampus (Opazo et al, 2003; Sui et al, 2008; Lin et al, 2008). Akt could promote these processes, at least in part, by activating the nuclear factor CREB (Du and Montminy, 1998). Indeed, CREB-dependent gene transcription appears to be an essential component of long-term memory formation (Silva et al, 1998). Our data strengthen this possibility, as Akt activation is followed by an increased CREB phosphorylation in simvastatin- and HF-treated hippocampal neurons. The results obtained from hippocampal cell cultures are further sustained by p-CREB immunostaining, which is particularly positive in the hippocampus of simvastatin-treated rats if compared with vehicle ones. On the contrary no differences, in terms of immunoreactivity, were detectable in PSD-95. The obtained results are in agreement with other recent published data, which demonstrated that chronic simvastatin treatment restores the expression of the learning- and memory-related genes c-Fos and Egr-1 without inducing any modulation in the classical synaptic markers synaptophysin and PSD-95 (Tong et al, 2012). Moreover, considering that c-Fos and Egr-1 are downstream of CREB, our findings support the hypothetical model on cognition effects induced by simvastatin proposed by Tong et al, 2012. However, we cannot exclude that perturbations in cytoskeleton remodeling following HMGR inhibition, and in turn RhoA inactivation, could contribute, together with Akt induction, to the enhancement of the consolidation of aversive memories. Considering the divergences in the brain regional metabolism, it is possible to speculate that each brain area can be considered as a unique structure with a specific cellular context, able to react in a different manner to the same stimulus (Segatto et al, 2013). Moreover in this case, despite the generalized influence on the MVA pathway in the CNS, it is clear that simvastatin treatment selectively affected prenylated proteins in specific brain regions. Different effects of HMGR inhibition in dependence on the brain area taken into consideration have already been reported (Wang et al, 2006). Thus, the selective effects of simvastatin treatment in reducing the amount of Rab3 and RhoA active fractions observed in this study could be influenced by differences in metabolism, function, turnover, or relative abundance of specific prenylated proteins in each brain region.

Despite the involvement of Rab3 and RhoA, other cellular mechanisms not evaluated in this work could contribute to the behavioral and cognitive outcomes induced by simvastatin. Growing evidence supports the hypothesis that statins could lead to brain effects, acting through pleiotropic mechanisms (Sierra et al, 2011). For instance, previous works identified changes in the plasma cholesterol with the onset of mood/anxiety disorders and the modulation of memory performance (Peter et al, 2002; Henderson et al, 2003; Granholm et al, 2008). Even though the biological significance of these connections is not widely accepted and remains to be clarified because of the presence of contradictory data (Papakostas et al, 2004; Taylor et al, 2011; Reitz et al, 2005), we cannot exclude that changes in plasma lipids, also observed in our work, could have a role in the functional effects exerted by simvastatin administration. Moreover, a very recent paper showed that the enhancement of the autophagic flux alleviates memory deficits in a transgenic mouse model of AD (Li et al, 2013). As statins induce autophagy in different cell types (Wei et al, 2013; Parikh et al, 2010), it is possible that a potential modulation of this process could participate, together with the alterations highlighted in this work, in the increased memory consolidation observed in simvastatin-treated rats.

In summary, these findings indicate that the modulation of the isoprenoid/cholesterol biosynthetic pathway is critically involved in the physiological modulation of both emotional and cognitive processes in rodents (Supplementary Figure S5). Even though our results provide some hints for the mechanisms of action of statins in the CNS, more efforts should be done in order to better understand their pleiotropic molecular effects in the CNS. Thus, the present work sets the stage for a future deeper understanding of the effects induced by simvastatin, which could be useful to better define the emerging and tight connection between MVA pathway and brain physiopathology.

FUNDING AND DISCLOSURE

The authors declare no conflict of interest.