Main

Prostate cancer (PC) has become one of the most common cancers among men in the Western world. Most patients are diagnosed with potentially curable disease. During recent years, it has become common practice to combine androgen deprivation (AD) with radical radiotherapy in a neo+adjuvant setting, since this approach has been shown to improve survival of patients with poor prognosis (Bolla et al, 1997). A striking increase in the incidence of PC with poor prognosis in men aged <60 years has been reported (Post et al, 1999). A combined approach has been suggested to reduce the mortality due to this disease (Demers et al, 2001). Androgen deprivation has also been proposed as an alternative to watchful waiting for men with clinically localised disease electing less aggressive management (Yang et al, 1998), and AD is widely used as a means of preventing progression if local therapies cannot be utilised for reasons of patient's preference or poor general condition (Gee, 1995).

Although AD is the treatment of choice in metastatic PC, there is no international consensus on indications or treatment duration in the use of neo+adjuvant treatment in PC. The indications for and duration of AD used in connection with radical treatment call for evidence-based recommendations. Any treatment of these patients causing adverse effects should therefore be carefully monitored to ensure that treatment-related effects do not exceed the disease-related (Fransson et al, 2001). A common practice outside clinical trials is to apply AD for 3–6 months, but treatments as long as several years are also practised. The use of AD appears to be most common in early-stage disease (T1–T2), as reported by Potosky et al (2001), these being patients who are usually long-term survivors in PC. At least 22 000 PC men each year in the US have been estimated to receive hormonal therapy (Potosky et al 2001).

Androgen deprivation therapy for PC is known to affect the quality of life (QoL) adversely, leading to increased fatigue, erectile difficulties and decline in sexual function (Potosky et al, 2002). Although cognitive complaints are not uncommon during AD treatment (Green et al, 2002a, 2002b), they have not hitherto been systematically addressed in this patient group. There have been suggestions that lowered testosterone level is associated with impairment of cognitive functioning (O'Connor et al, 2001) and that testosterone replacement therapy may improve certain memory performances (Cherrier et al, 2001), but these studies have addressed on healthy old men. Whether AD affects cognitive functioning in newly diagnosed PC patients in any significant way has not, to our knowledge, been previously reported. We hypothesised that AD may have an impact on cognitive performances in PC patients, especially on attention and memory, because these functions are known to be sensitive to disease-related and other kinds of stress effects (Barrett-Connor et al, 1999). A systematic study on cognitive functioning during 1 year of AD treatment of PC patients was conducted to study quantitatively the changes associated with this treatment.

Patients and methods

Patients

Extensive cognitive testing was undertaken on three occasions in 25 men receiving neo+adjuvant AD therapy for 12 months in connection with radical radiotherapy for PC. The patients had to fulfil at least two of the following criteria to be eligible for hormonal treatment: tumour grade >2, Gleason >5 and Prostate-specific antigen (PSA) >20. Only patients with newly diagnosed PC requiring therapy were accepted. Eight patients (32%) had performance status 0 and 17 patients (68%) status 1 as measured by WHO criteria (Miller, 1981). World Health Organisation (WHO) performance status 0 indicates full functioning with no symptoms, and WHO 1 indicates normal performance status with slight or occasional symptoms. Most of the patients had a T3 tumour (84%); there were two with T2 and two with T4 tumours, and all represented grade 2–3 tumours. Their mean PSA at diagnosis was 31 ng l−1 (s.d. 25.4). None of the patients developed progressive disease during the 12-month study period.

The 52 healthy controls for baseline evaluation were voluntary male participants who had no psychiatric or neurological disorders or history of drug or alcohol abuse. Control group of normal healthy men at baseline was used to comparison of basic cognitive performances of the PC patient group. The patients and control persons came from southwestern Finland and were matched for distribution of age and education. The mean age of the patients was 64.4 years (s.d. 6.5, range 49–75), and their mean education 8.9 years (s.d. 2.9, range 6–15). The mean age of the controls was 65.3 years (s.d. 6.6, range 46–77), education a mean of 8.5 years (s.d. 2.1, range 6–15). Mini-mental state examination (MMSE) was performed: for the patients, the mean score was 27.1 (s.d. 2.0, range 21–29), and for the controls 28.0 (s.d. 1.4, range 25–30). There was no statistically significant difference between the groups; two patients who had a lower score than controls were accepted.

To be eligible for neuropsychological testing, patients had to meet the following inclusion criteria: (1) no evidence of progressive or metastatic disease, (2) no history of neurological/psychiatric signs or symptoms that might lead to deviant neuropsychological test results, (3) no abuse of alcohol or drugs and (4) the mother tongue being Finnish. Written informed consent was obtained from all patients. The study was approved by the joint ethical committee of Turku University Hospital and the University of Turku.

Androgen deprivation therapy was started with flutamide given for 4 weeks, and LHRH analogue (s.c. q 3 months four times) was added after 2 weeks. Thus, AD therapy lasted 12 months. Radiotherapy was given by the conformal technique using the 15 MV photons/Varian Clinac 2100C/D linear accelerator (Varian Inc. Palo Alto, CA, USA) to a mean tumour dose of 69 Gy (s.d. 3.15, range 61–77 Gy). No fixation was used. Portal images were taken with a Varian Portal Vision Mark 2 electronic portal imaging device, integrated with Varis Vision 5.0 software, on average once a week during about 7-week irradiation.

Neuropsychological testing

Neuropsychological testing was conducted using a cognitive test battery at baseline, and at 6 and 12 months. Individuals in the control group were examined only at baseline. The patients acted as their own controls in the follow-up. The patients also attended for clinical control check-up every 6 months after the onset of therapy and were interviewed with regard to physical and mental symptoms as experienced in daily life; PSA, Hb, serum creatinine and liver enzymes were taken and QoL forms were filled on the same occasions.

Our study was exploratory because there were no earlier reports of antiandrogen effects on cognition in newly diagnosed PC patients. The methods with corresponding references are explained in full detail in the Appendix. We selected an extensive and systematic test battery to evaluate basic verbal, visuomotor and memory domains, and overall level of performances, as well as cognitive speed and accuracy. Attentional domains and memory can be expected to be particularly sensitive to both disease- and treatment-related effects.

Cognitive processing was studied using CogniSpeed© software, planned to measure both automatic (well-learned) and controlled, attention demanding processing. Automatic tasks consist of recognition of familiar items (numbers and letters), whereas tasks of controlled processing demand working memory or sustaining attention. These functions may be most vulnerable to harmful effects, such as depletion of hormone concentrations. Attention and memory performances may reflect cognitive processing efficiency also in everyday situations. One purpose of the study was to find out whether there are cognitive domains sensitive to impair in response to a year-lasting lack of androgens.

In order to utilise all information provided by test variables, we used original data as continuous, not categorised variables. To ensure that the subject had capacity to improvement, we evaluated a subject's overall impairment on verbal, visuomotor and memory performances, using standard deviations of an independent norm group. Most patients performed as controls on this deterioration scale (range 0–18 points), which provides more information than MMSE: for the controls the range was 0–3 (mean 0.80) and for the patients 0–4 (mean 0.84) (Table 1). With regard to clinical practice, it is always possible to compare an individual score in any test, for example, in reaction time tests, with the means and standard deviations of a healthy control group.

Table 1 Verbal, visuomotor and memory performances in patients (n=25) and controls (n=52): comparison at baseline, and testing of patients from baseline through 12 months (means (s.e.'s))

Quality of life

At the time of cognitive testing, the patients were also asked to fill in EORTC QLQ-C30 forms (Fayers et al, 1997) completed with sexual function items. These items in this study included questions about level of sexual interest, ability to achieve and maintain erection, gynaecomastia, spousal relationship and mood. Other items asked respondents about overall physical or mental discomfort, worry, role functioning, limitations in daily activities and bother due to PC or treatments.

Statistical analyses

The data were summarised showing mean values and standard errors (s.e.). the patients who had measurements at baseline and at least at either of the two follow-up time points were included in the analyses. Statistical comparisons of patients and controls at baseline were made using two-sample t-test. Within the patient groups, the comparisons of different time points were carried out with analysis of variance for repeated measurements. The pairwise post hoc comparisons of time points were conducted by Tukey's method. The analyses were performed using MIXED procedure in the SAS System for Windows, release 6.12/1996. The MIXED procedure offers a sophisticated tool for analysis of follow-up data with possible missing data during follow-up (Littell et al, 1996). P-values less than 0.05 were considered to be statistically significant.

Results

At baseline, the AD group performed at a lower level than the control group, matched for distribution of age and education, in immediate memory of digit span (P=0.008), in visuomotor digit symbol (P=0.011) and in word fluency (P=0.049). Comparisons between controls and patients at baseline are shown in Tables 1 and 2.

Table 2 Cognitive speed and attention in patients ((n=25) and controls (n=52): comparison at baseline, and testing of patients from baseline through 12 months (means (s.e.'s))

Other performances in memory tests (semantic memory, visual memory, word recall), in visuomotor block design or naming tests did not differ between the groups. The depression scores were low and did not differ. Further, the AD group performed more slowly than the controls in the sustained attention vigilance task (P<0.001), but the groups did not differ in the other reaction time tasks or in the recognition task (Table 2). A comparison of the AD group and controls in reaction time tests in different attentional domains is presented in Figure 1.

Figure 1
figure 1

Cognitive performance speed at baseline in reaction time tasks in patients starting AD therapy (AD group) and controls (means (s.e.'s)).

During the period of 12 months on AD, some changes emerged in memory performances compared to the baseline values (Table 1). The patients improved their performances in episodic memory (immediate recall of objects, P=0.035, significant change for the first 6 months and delayed recall P<0.001, significant change for the whole test period) and in semantic memory (similarities, P=0.037, significant change for the latter half of the test period (Figure 2). Other performances, including reaction speed and accuracy in reaction time tests, remained unchanged through the follow-up testings (Table 2). Beck depression scores remained similar throughout the follow-up testings.

Figure 2
figure 2

Cognitive performance of patients during AD therapy from baseline through 6 and 12 months in three memory tests; statistically significant improvement, marked as *.

On the self-report QoL forms (Table 3) a significant worsening of physical functioning was seen when the results of 12 months were compared with that of baseline.

Table 3 Physical scale values during 12 months of androgen deprivation expressed as means and standard errors (s.e.)

During the treatment, the patients expressed significantly more suffering from fatigue, dyspnoea and diarrhoea compared to baseline. No statistically significant changes were reported in the case of emotional, social or cognitive functioning. Table 4 shows in detail the development of changes in physical and emotional items. Although the patients reported their sexual interest, male identity and erectile function to decrease significantly during AD, they did not feel more depressed nor did they indicate that their relationship with their spouse suffered from their present condition more than at baseline.

Table 4 Comparison of selected quality of life items during 12 months of androgen deprivation expressed as means and standard errors (s.e)

Discussion

Since AD treatment is commonly used in PC, the objective of our study was to investigate the effects of AD at 6 and 12 months on cognitive function in PC patients with a view to helping the clinician in decision-making and education of patients on treatment effects. The current study showed some deficits in verbal and visuomotor performances as well as sustained attention of the patient group in comparison to controls. During AD treatment, in spite of impaired physical function in these patients, they showed significant improvement in some memory performances and no cognitive impairments when AD treatment was given for up to 12 months. During 12-month testing of the AD group, significant improvement was observed compared to baseline values in episodic memory measures of object recall and in semantic memory, whereas no impairment was found in any cognitive performances evaluated, including cognitive speed in reaction time tasks.

To minimise individual variability and learning effect in reaction time tasks, there was a practice round preceding the CogniSpeed© subtests. The repeated measures of CogniSpeed© tests have shown a reliable and steady course over trials in healthy persons and in the study with brain tumour patients (Lilja et al, 2001). Furthermore, the six tests of cognitive deterioration, including verbal, visuomotor and memory tests, are sensitive to cognitive impairment, even if the same versions are repeated (Kujala et al, 1997). Learning effects normally occur in free recall tasks in repeated measurements. We do not propose that there could not be improvement in performances of controls, if tested, because learning effects normally occur in free recall tasks in repeated measurements. Learning effect can be considered a positive sign of one's capacity to utilise experience rather than a negative source of error.

Androgen deprivation therapy for PC is known to affect the QoL adversely, leading to increased fatigue, erectile difficulties and declines in sexual functioning (Lubeck et al, 2001). The duration of AD in clinical practice varies from 3 months to 3 years and even longer. The present study set-up was motivated by the common practice of using AD in the treatment of early-stage PC outside clinical trials. We chose to test 12 months of treatment in view of superior effects shown compared to shorter (Gleave et al, 2001), and avoided longer treatment in order to delay the development of hormone-resistant disease (Newling, 2000).

We focused on effects of hormonal treatment on attention and information processing as well as memory function as these domains of cognition are considered vulnerable to changes in sexual hormone level (Barrett-Connor et al, 1999). In the literature, there are some indications that androgen treatment may modulate aspects of cognitive performance (Wolf et al, 2000). It has also been suggested that testosterone replacement therapy may have a positive effect on cognition, in particular on improved spatial and verbal memory in healthy older men (O'Connor et al, 2001). This is partly in agreement with the present results, which showed selective improvement in episodic and semantic memory measures, whereas attention and information processing speed remained unchanged through the treatment period. On the other hand, decreased testosterone levels may be associated with impaired cognition in aging men (Cherrier et al, 2001). In the present study, the patients performed some tasks at lower level than the control persons of similar age and education, when the groups were compared at baseline before the start of antiandrogen therapy. Thus, this impairment may refer to disease-related rather than to more specific effects. Longitudinal cognitive stability and improvements may suggest some positive hormonal influence on brain functioning.

We recognise some limitations in the interpretation of our results. We used normal controls at baseline as a reference group for the PC patients. However, the groups were matched for basic characteristics: they were of similar age and education, with no major diseases. In the longitudinal study, the effect of AD treatment on cognition was evaluated using the patient's own baseline values. This strengthens the interpretation of results, while elderly PC patients are a heterogenous patient group with often several comorbidities that could affect the results of cognitive tests. Our results with similar pattern of cognitive testing with patients treated with curative radiotherapy at the same dose level as the study patients without AD resulted in no statistically significant changes in cognition (Salminen et al, 2002).

In concord with the observations of Van Dam et al (1998) in chemotherapy-treated breast cancer patients, we observed that patients complained, for example, of depressive mood and similar discomforts, although these were not confirmed by the Beck depression results. It has been proposed that objective test results and subjective reports of patients regarding their cognitive functioning and mood are not always related; there has been no relationship between the self-reported difficulties and the performance of patients in objective tests (Cull et al, 1966; Ly et al, 2001). Furthermore, PC patients with nonlocal cancer randomised either to AD or follow-up did not differ in subjective cognitive function (Green et al, 2002a, 2002b). Subjective reports have been found to be related to anxiety and depression, being indicative more of emotional distress than cognitive deficits (Van Dam et al, 1998; Ly et al, 2001).

In comparison to a report by Fransson et al (2001), who observed decreased QoL in the social functioning of PC patients treated with radiotherapy only, our patients did not evince this, although the forms they filled in were similar. The difference is in evaluation time, which was over 3 years in Fransson's series. The deterioration in physical function reported by our patients was in line with the previous results (Lubeck et al, 2001).

Conclusion

Healthy PC patients treated with AD in connection with curative radiotherapy maintain their cognitive functioning. Cognitive deficits found at baseline were associated with disease severity rather than with treatment-related factors. The documentation of cognitive function has substantial implications for informed patient's support when hesitating between treatment options. A longer follow-up study with cognitive assessment is needed if AD is extended for several years, and further studies are needed to clarify the effects of AD in patients with severe comorbidity.