News & Views | Published:

Neuroscience

The split view of motion

Nature volume 468, pages 178179 (11 November 2010) | Download Citation

In both fruitflies and vertebrates, signals from photoreceptor cells are immediately split into two opposing channels in the downstream neurons. This might facilitate the computation of visual motion. See Letter p.300

Nearly a century ago, the great Spanish neuroanatomist Santiago Ramón y Cajal compared1 the vertebrate retina with the fly's compound eye and noted similarities in their neural circuits (Fig. 1). He redrew the cell bodies of the fly's monopolar cells, transforming them to vertebrate retinal bipolar neurons. Ultrastructural studies have since revealed that, indeed, both sets of neurons receive inputs from photoreceptor cells at structurally unique junctions called ribbon synapses in their first visual neuropiles, or neural switchboards — namely the fly's lamina and the retina's outer plexiform layer2. On page 300 of this issue, Joesch et al.3 further extend the analogy, reporting that, like their vertebrate bipolar-neuron counterparts, fly monopolar cells split photoreceptor signals into ON and OFF channels to encode brightness increment and decrement, respectively.

Figure 1: Similarity between fly and vertebrate visual systems.
Figure 1

Santiago Ramón y Cajal1 compared the monopolar cells in the fly visual system (left panel) with the bipolar cells in the vertebrate retina (right panel). He redrew the cell bodies of the former to take on the bipolar form (middle panel) (as shown by the added red arrows). The 'mysterious' black arrows (left panel), which appear in many of Cajal's drawings, correctly indicate the flow of visual information, from the photoreceptors (a, b) to the monopolar cells (c) and to the downstream neurons (h). Image: Cajal Legacy, Instituto Cajal (CSIC), Madrid

Two main types of fly monopolar cell — L1 and L2 — receive a similar number of synaptic inputs from the type of photoreceptors that mediate motion detection. Using genetic methods to manipulate the activity of specific neurons, behavioural studies4,5 have suggested that L1 and L2 have overlapping but differentiable roles in detecting visual motion.

By recording electrical activity from downstream motion-sensitive neurons, Joesch et al. provide a physiological basis for the behavioural observations. They find that blocking L1 eliminates the response to a moving bright edge (ON), whereas blocking L2 abolishes responses to a moving dark edge (OFF). In a separate paper6, the same group directly examines the activity of L2 neurons by calcium-imaging techniques and confirms that L2 encodes the OFF signals. Thus, as for vertebrate photoreceptors, the fly photoreceptor signal is split into ON and OFF channels at the first synapse.

Joesch and colleagues3 further unexpectedly find that L1 and L2 are electrically coupled through gap junctions — specialized complexes that connect the cytoplasm of two adjacent cells. This observation reconciles a conundrum of previous behavioural observations4,5: reconstituting photoreceptor input to either L1 or L2 is sufficient to generate a near-normal response to motion stimuli, whereas blocking the output of either L1 or L2 diminishes that response.

Why split photoreceptor signals into ON and OFF channels? It has been suggested7 that this coding mechanism satisfies both information and metabolic constraints: to keep the downstream retinal ganglion neurons at a high level of basal neural activity costs energy; to inhibit them below a certain basal level conveys little information. But why do only those fly photoreceptors that mediate motion detection split their signals into ON/OFF channels in the lamina? The other photoreceptors, which mediate colour vision, do not. Joesch et al. argue that splitting facilitates the computation of visual motion, and to do so they invoke the original Hassenstein–Reichardt model for motion detection.

The Hassenstein–Reichardt model was developed8 in the 1950s to explain behavioural experiments on the beetle Chlorophanus. Despite these humble origins, in its abstract form this model has arguably been the most influential theory of visual motion detection9. It explains how motion can be computed from local luminance changes — the fundamental form of visual motion. And it makes several counterintuitive predictions that match well with electrophysiological and behavioural data from many species, including humans.

The core computation of the Hassenstein–Reichardt model is based on a delay-and-compare mechanism: the signal from one photoreceptor is delayed and then compared, by multiplication, with the instantaneous signal derived from a neighbouring photoreceptor. Despite its simple construction, the multiplication stage is actually difficult to implement by known synaptic mechanisms, because this seemingly simple mathematical operation needs to be carried out in a way that respects the 'sign' of the signal — that is, multiplying two negative signals should generate a positive signal.

Joesch et al.3 point out that the 'original' Hassenstein–Reichardt model (reported in German and consequently lost to most of the English-speaking world) offers a solution to this problem. In that model, photoreceptor signals are first split into ON and OFF channels, which carry positive and negative components, respectively. Four separate multiplications for the two neighbouring signals are then added or subtracted to correct the sign and generate a direction-specific motion signal. In a way, the sign of the signal is 'remembered' by the signal paths, and the outcome is identical to a sign-corrected multiplication. Electrical engineers in the 1960s devised a similar solution called the four-quadrant (Gilbert) multiplier, which is still used in analog circuits today.

An advantage of the Hassenstein–Reichardt model is its robustness: inactivating either the ON or OFF channel reduces but does not abolish the motion response, consistent with both behavioural and electrophysiological data. Over time, however, several motion-detection models have been proposed, including the energy model10, which generates output identical to that of the Hassenstein–Reichardt model but has a very different internal structure. To determine which of these various models explains the behaviour of neurons mediating visual motion, direct access to the actual neural circuits is required.

As one cost of its abstract nature, the Hassenstein–Reichardt model is anatomically inexplicit and only hints at the actual neural implementation. To carry out four-quadrant multiplication, each elementary motion detector requires four separate pathways feeding ON and OFF channels into four multipliers, each of which converges on the wide-field motion-sensitive neurons from which Joesch et al.3 recorded. Previous anatomical and electrophysiological studies11,12 have revealed a number of candidate neurons for visual motion dectection. More are anticipated from ongoing ultrastructural projects to reconstruct these circuits in toto, especially those at the Janelia Farm campus of the Howard Hughes Medical Institute in Ashburn, Virginia. The recent convergence of anatomical, behavioural and electrophysiological investigations — all aided by powerful fly genetics — provides renewed hope that the neural mechanism of motion detection might finally be resolved in the near future.

References

  1. 1.

    & Trab. Lab. Invest. Biol. 13, 1–167 (1915).

  2. 2.

    Prog. Retinal Res. 12, 13–39 (1993).

  3. 3.

    , , , & Nature 468, 300–304 (2010).

  4. 4.

    & Neuron 59, 322–335 (2008).

  5. 5.

    et al. Neuron 56, 155–170 (2007).

  6. 6.

    , , , & Nature Neurosci. 13, 973–978 (2010).

  7. 7.

    Trends Neurosci. 15, 86–92 (1992).

  8. 8.

    & Z. Naturforsch. 11b, 513–524 (1956).

  9. 9.

    & Trends Neurosci. 12, 297–306 (1989).

  10. 10.

    & J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 2, 284–299 (1985).

  11. 11.

    & Cell Tissue Res. 258, 441–475 (1989).

  12. 12.

    & J. Neurosci. 15, 5596–5611 (1995).

Download references

Author information

Affiliations

  1. Chi-Hon Lee is in the Program in Cellular Regulation and Metabolism, Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Maryland 20892, USA.  leechih@mail.nih.gov

    • Chi -Hon Lee

Authors

  1. Search for Chi -Hon Lee in:

About this article

Publication history

Published

DOI

https://doi.org/10.1038/468178a

Further reading

Comments

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.

Newsletter Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing