Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Letter
  • Published:

False perception of motion in a patient who cannot compensate for eye movements

Abstract

We are usually unaware of the motion of an image across our retina that results from our own movement. For instance, during slow-tracking eye movements we do not mistake the shift of the image projected onto the retina for motion of the world around us, but instead perceive a stable world. Following early suggestions by von Helmholtz1, it is commonly believed that this spatial stability is achieved by subtracting the retinal motion signal from an internal reference signal, such as a copy of the movement command (efference copy)2,3,4. Object motion is perceived only if the two differ. Although this concept is widely accepted, its anatomical underpinning remains unknown. Here we describe the case of a patient with bilateral extrastriate cortex lesions, suffering from false perception of motion due to an inability to take eye movements into account when faced with self-induced retinal image slip. This is indicated by the fact that during smooth-pursuit eye movements, he perceives motion of the stationary world at a velocity that corresponds to the velocity of his eye movement; that is, he perceives the raw retinal image slip uncorrected for his own eye movements. We suspect that this deficiency reflects damage of a distinct parieto-occipital region that disentangles self-induced and externally induced visual motion by comparing retinal signals with a reference signal encoding eye movements and possibly ego-motion in general.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: MRI shows bilateral cyst-like local widenings (arrows) of the sulci of the occipital lobe mainly affecting parts of areas 18, 19 and possibly 37 on the lateral aspect of the hemispheres and areas 18 and 19 on the inferior aspect (ac).
Figure 2: Perception of pursuit-induced visual motion in patient R. W. (filled triangles) and 18 control subjects (50 individual measurements represented by open circles, partially lying on top of each other).
Figure 3: Evidence for unimpaired analysis of retinal motion in patient R.W.
Figure 4: Performance of R. W. (dots) and 6 control subjects (means and standard deviations) on a task assessing the sensitivity to changes in the velocity of a moving target.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. von Helmholtz, H. Physiological Optics Vol. III(Dover, New York, (1962)) (trans. from 3rd German edn of 1910).

    Google Scholar 

  2. von Holst, E. & Mittelstaedtt, H. Das Reafferenzprinzip. Naturwissenschaften 37, 464–476 (1950).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  3. Sperry, R. W. Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 43, 482–489 (1950).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Wertheim, A. H. Motion perception during self-motion: the direct versus inferential controversy revisited. Behav. Brain Sci. 17, 293–355 (1994).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Filehne, W. Über das optische Wahrnehmen von Bewegungen. Z. Sinnesphysiol. 53, 134–145 (1922).

    Google Scholar 

  6. Mack, A. & Herman, E. Position constancy during pursuit eye movements: an investigation of the Filehne illusion. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 25, 71–84 (1973).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Haarmeier, T. & Thier, P. Modification of the Filehne illusion by conditioning visual stimuli. Vis. Res. 36, 741–650 (1996).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Haarmeier, T. & Thier, P. An electrophysiological correlate of visual motion awareness in man. J. Cogn. Neurosci.(in the press).

  9. Rizzo, M., Nawrot, M. & Zihl, J. Motion and shape perception in cerebral akinetopsia. Brain 118, 1105–1127 (1995).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Regan, D., Kothe, A. C. & Sharpe, A. Recognition of motion-defined shapes in patients with multiple sclerosis and optic neuritis. Brain 114, 1129–1155 (1991).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Herbst, H., Ketabi, A., Thier, P. & Dichgans, J. Comparison of psychophysical and evoked potential methods in the detection of visual deficits in multiple sclerosis. Electroenceph. Clin. Neurophysiol. 104, 82–90 (1997).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Haarmeier, T. & Thier, P. Impaired dynamic visual acuity due to deficient smooth pursuit eye movements. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 23, 1950 (1997).

    Google Scholar 

  13. Komatsu, H. & Wurtz, R. J. Relation of cortical areas MT and MST to pursuit eye movements. I. Localization and visual properties of neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 60, 580–603 (1988).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Komatsu, H. & Wurtz, R. J. Relation of cortical areas MT and MST to pursuit eye movements. III. Interaction with full-field visual stimulation. J. Neurophysiol. 60, 621–644 (1988).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Erickson, R. G. & Thier, P. Aneuronal correlate of spatial stability during periods of self-induced visual motion. Exp. Brain Res. 86, 608–616 (1991).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Ilg, U. J. & Thier, P. Inability of rhesus monkey area V1 to discriminate between self-induced and externally induced retinal image slip. Eur. J. Neurosci. 8, 1156–1166 (1996).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Tootell, R. B. et al. Functional analysis of human MT and related visual cortical areas using magnetic resonance imaging. J. Neurosci. 15, 3215–3230 (1995).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Zeki, S. et al. Adirect demonstration of functional specialization in human visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 11, 641–649 (1991).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Lieberman, H. R. & Pentland, A. P. Microcomputer-based estimation of psychophysical thresholds: the best PEST. Behav. Res. Methods Instr. 14, 21–25 (1982).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. McKee, S. P., Klein, S. A. & Teller, D. Y. Statistical properties of forced choice psychometric functions: implications of probit analysis. Percept. Psychophy. 37, 286–298 (1985).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank patient R.W. for participating in our experiments. Supported by KFG ‘Neuroophthalmologie’.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Peter Thier.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Haarmeier, T., Thier, P., Repnow, M. et al. False perception of motion in a patient who cannot compensate for eye movements. Nature 389, 849–852 (1997). https://doi.org/10.1038/39872

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/39872

This article is cited by

Comments

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing