INTRODUCTION

Since introduction of the hypofrontality concept (Ingvar and Franzen, 1974), a dysfunction of prefrontal brain areas in schizophrenic patients has frequently been found with many different methodological approaches (eg, Andreasen et al, 1992; Carter et al, 1998; Fallgatter and Mueller, 2001; Goldberg et al, 1993; Riley et al, 2000). Besides the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), which has frequently been implicated in schizophrenic illnesses (Glahn et al, 2005; MacDonald and Carter, 2003; Rodriguez-Sanchez et al, 2005), particularly the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) is assumed to be affected by this hypofrontality, which has been shown with different neuroimaging techniques at rest (Tamminga et al, 1992) as well as during activation with neuropsychological tasks (Andreasen et al, 1992; Carter et al, 1997; Dolan et al, 1995; Haznedar et al, 1997; Siegel et al, 1993; Tamminga et al, 1992).

The availability of so-called ‘atypical’ antipsychotics broadened the therapeutic possibilities in the treatment of psychoses from the schizophrenic spectrum. The best-founded theoretical concept explaining differences in the way of action between typical and atypical antipsychotics focuses on the neurotransmitter systems addressed. Typical antipsychotics are thought to reduce positive symptoms like delusions and hallucinations mainly by blocking D2 receptors in the mesolimbic dopaminergic system. Atypical antipsychotics are supposed to additionally improve negative and cognitive symptoms by a supplementary action on neurotransmitter systems within the prefrontal cortex (Díaz-Mataix et al, 2005; Horacek et al, 2006; Ichikawa and Meltzer, 1999). Such a differential action on the prefrontal cortex is supported by neuropsychological studies indicating an improvement of cognitive performance during therapy with atypical compounds as compared to a poor response or even an exacerbation under typical antipsychotics (Borison, 1996; Gur et al, 2003; Lewander, 1994; Purdon et al, 2000), although even within the group of atypical antipsychotics substances seem to differ regarding their exact effects (eg, Woodward et al, 2005). Studies directly investigating the effect of medication not only on cognition but also on underlying prefrontal brain function are sparse. However, a recent 1H-Magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging study described higher levels of N-acetylaspartate in the ACC of schizophrenic patients treated with atypical as compared to typical neuroleptics. This finding indicated a more pronounced neuronal viability in the atypical group and was accompanied by a better neuropsychological performance (Braus et al, 2002).

During the last years we developed an electrophysiological marker of prefrontal cortex function termed NoGo anteriorization (NGA) (Fallgatter et al, 1997). Based on event-related potentials (ERP) derived from a multichannel EEG during performance of a Go–NoGo task, the NGA quantitatively describes the amount of anteriorization of the positive brain electrical field associated with the inhibition (NoGo condition) relative to the execution (Go condition) of a prepared motor response. In healthy subjects, the NGA has been shown to have a very high interindividual stability (Fallgatter et al, 1997, 2000; Fallgatter and Strik, 1999), an excellent short- (Fallgatter et al, 2001), and long-term test–retest reliability (Fallgatter et al, 2002a), and it appears to be independent of the participant's age and gender (Fallgatter et al, 1999). Moreover, three-dimensional source localization studies with the LORETA method (Low Resolution Electromagnetic Tomography; Pascual-Marqui et al, 1994) repeatedly found a significantly higher activation within prefrontal brain areas (particularly the ACC) during the response inhibition (NoGo) as compared to the response execution (Go) condition as the basis of the NGA (Strik et al, 1998; Fallgatter et al, 2002b). This finding is in line with fMRI and PET studies that also reported increased activation of the ACC during inhibitory processes (de Zubicaray et al, 2000; Ford et al, 2004; Matthews et al, 2004; Rubia et al, 2001). Based on these findings, the NGA has been suggested to be an electrophysiological correlate of prefrontal response control probably associated with activation of the ACC.

Compared to age- and gender-matched healthy controls, schizophrenic patients were found to have a significantly reduced mean NGA (Fallgatter and Mueller, 2001) and a significantly diminished activation of the ACC during processes of response inhibition as indicated by a LORETA source localization (Fallgatter et al, 2003). This electrophysiological finding is once again in line with findings obtained by other neuroimaging methods that similarly reported reduced activation in the ACC region in schizophrenic patients during processes of response inhibition and executive control (Carter et al, 2001; Laurens et al, 2003; Rubia et al, 2001; Volz et al, 1999).

The present study was designed to evaluate the effect of atypical vs typical antipsychotic treatment of 6 weeks duration in acutely admitted patients with endogenous psychoses. Prefrontal brain function was assessed with the NGA, and cognitive performance was measured with the Stroop task (cognitive interference) and the Verbal Fluency Test (VFT; word fluency). Based on the findings described above we hypothesized that treatment with atypical antipsychotics should lead to a more pronounced improvement in both prefrontal brain function (ie, higher NGA values) and cognitive performance (ie, better performance measures in neuropsychological tests of frontal lobe function) as compared to treatment with typical antipsychotics.

PATIENTS AND METHODS

Participants

Acutely admitted patients who were suspected to suffer from a schizophrenic disorder were included. Exclusion criteria were age below 18 and above 60 years, actual pretreatment with atypical antipsychotics, significant comorbidities with other currently present axis I disorders, a history of or an actually manifest disease of the CNS, or severe somatic diseases. Electrophysiological and neuropsychological investigations took place within 3 days after admittance to the hospital (baseline) and at a 6-week-follow-up. Forty-three patients completed both assessments with sufficient performance. Four patients had to be disclosed because of too many artifacts in at least one of the electrophysiological investigations, one other patient refused any neuroleptic treatment. From the remaining 38 patients (22 females; 33 right handed, five left handed; mean age 35.7±10.8 years) a minimum of 20 artifact-free EEG-epochs after the Go and the NoGo condition of the Continuous Performance Test (CPT) were available.

According to the SKID-I-Interview (Wittchen et al, 1997) these patients were diagnosed as suffering from disorganized (295.10; n=4), catatonic (295.20; n=1), paranoid (295.30; n=8), and undifferentiated (295.90; n=4) types of schizophrenia. Because of the time criteria in DSM-IV, part of the patients was diagnosed as suffering from schizophreniform disorders (295.40; n=14). Six patients were diagnosed with a schizoaffective disorder (295.70) and one patient with a psychotic disorder, not otherwise specified (298.9). With regard to psychiatric comorbidities, two patients were diagnosed with a Cannabis abuse (305.20), one with Bulimia nervosa (307.51), one patient had suffered from a polysubstance dependence in the past (304.80), one from sedative abuse (305.40), and another one from anorexia nervosa (307.1). The mean duration of the disease was 123.9±107.8 months, the number of admissions to psychiatric hospitals was 4.8±6.1, and the mean duration of all hospitalizations combined was 9.8±12.0 months. Only five patients had a positive family history for schizophrenia with an affected first-degree relative. CT and/or MRI scans as rated by an experienced neuroradiologist revealed indications for a mild-to-moderate brain atrophy in five patients. Electroencephalographic examinations disclosed a modest slowing of the basic rhythm in eight cases. Regarding their social and educational status, patients had a mean of 10.34±1.99 years of school education; the sample consisted of five university students, four university graduates, 25 patients with a completed professional training, and four patients without any learned profession.

The choice of medication in this clinical setting was fully up to the treating physicians on the wards, who were blind with respect to all neurophysiological and neuropsychological results. The investigators in the neurophysiological laboratory, on the other hand, were not informed about the patients' medication status. 10 patients (37.2±14.6 years, five female, 10 right handed) received neuroleptic treatment with typical antipsychotics (flupentixol, perazine, melperone, chlorprothixene, perphenazine; mean chlorpromazine-equivalents (CPZ) of 488.6±222.6 mg/d), 19 (34.4±9.1 years, 11 female and 17 right handed) were treated with atypical neuroleptics (clozapin, olanzapine, risperidone, solian, seroquel, ziprasidon; CPZ of 527.0±324.1 mg/day) and nine (36.9±10.5 years, six female, six right handed) were under a combination of atypical and typical antipsychotics (four risperidone+perazine, three risperidone+perazine+flupentixol, one risperidone+pipamperone+amisulpride, one melperone+perazine+risperidone; CPZ of 681.7±419.4 mg/day). The type of antipsychotic medication was held constant over the course of the treatment, although adjustments in dosages occurred in most patients, particularly around the beginning of the treatment period. CPZ equivalents were calculated according to the specifications of Laux et al (2000) on clinical empirical equivalent doses of antipsychotic drugs; values for ziprasidon were calculated on the basis of a publication on newer atypical antipsychotics by Woods (2003).

Additionally, at some point during their stationary treatment 28 of the patients were on low to medium doses of benzodiazepines, 17 received an antidepressant therapy, six patients were treated with carbamazepine, nine with valproat, six with lithium, and six with biperiden. The three patient groups were very similar with respect to this adjunctive medication; there was only one significant difference between the groups as indicated by a χ2 statistic (χ2=6.14, p<0.05) with almost half of the typically medicated patients but only one patient in each of the other two groups receiving lithium as co-medication. Regarding general demographic variables, the three groups of patients did not differ significantly regarding their mean age (t<0.7, p>0.5) or gender distribution (χ2=0.54, p=0.76); a trend for a difference in the distribution of the handedness occurred (χ2=4.84, p=0.09). Written informed consent was obtained from all patients after the procedures had been fully explained. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Wuerzburg, and the procedures involved were in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki Principles.

Psychopathological Assessment

An extensive psychopathological and psychometric investigation was performed with each patient consisting of the SKID-I-Interview, the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS; Overall and Gorham, 1962), the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS; Kay et al, 1987), and the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale/24 Items (HDRS; Hamilton, 1960).

Neuropsychological Investigation

Each neuropsychological assessment consisted of the VFT and the Stroop Color Word Task (Stroop, 1935). For the VFT, patients were instructed to name as many nouns as possible beginning with a certain letter (A, F, and S; 1 min each=‘letters version’) or belonging to a certain category of words (animals, fruits, and flowers; 1 min each=‘categories version’). Performance of the VFT has been suggested to involve Broca's area and prefrontal brain structures such as the DLPFC (Cuenod et al, 1995; Dickins et al, 2001; Gaillard et al, 2000).

The Stroop Color Word Task consisted of three parts: For the first part (‘word reading’), patients had to read color words (German words for ‘blue’, ‘red’, ‘green’, and ‘yellow’) that were printed in black ink on a white sheet. For the second part (‘color naming’), they had to name the colors of blue, red, green, and yellow lines as quickly as possible. The third part of the test (‘interference condition’) consisted of color words printed in an ink color that did not correspond to the word meaning of the color word (eg, the word ‘blue’ printed in red ink). Patients were instructed to simply name the ink color of the words. The time needed to complete this last condition has been suggested to reflect activity of prefrontal brain areas, particularly the ACC (Barch et al, 2001; Ruff et al, 2001; Swick and Jovanovic, 2002), stronger cognitive interference (which is usually reflected by longer reaction times) resulting in more pronounced prefrontal activation. The Stroop effect is a universal phenomenon that mirrors cognitive interference induced by conflicting stimuli; individuals differ regarding their susceptibility to being distracted by interfering conditions.

Electrophysiological Investigation

Patients performed the CPT (Rosvold et al, 1956) during registration of the ongoing EEG in a dimly lit, sound attenuated and electrically shielded room. On a computer screen 60 cm in front of them, letters were presented sequentially in a pseudo-randomized order. The letters on the screen were 12 mm high and 11 mm wide, resulting in a visual angle of 1.15° horizontally and 1.05° vertically. Subjects were instructed to press a response button only when the letter O was directly followed by the letter X. Speed and accuracy were emphasized equally during explanation of the test. A short training session was performed to ensure correct understanding of the instructions. The applied O-X version of the CPT consisted of a total of 400 letters (114 letters O=primer condition, 57 X following an O=Go condition, 57 other letters following an O=NoGo condition, and 172 letters not following an O=distractors) with a stimulus-onset asynchrony of 1850 ms and a presentation time of 200 ms each. Simultaneously, the EEG was recorded from 21 scalp electrodes placed according to the International 10/20 System. Three additional electrodes were attached at the outer canthi of both eyes and below the right eye for registration of eye movements. The technical equipment consisted of a 32-channel DC amplifier (Brain-Star system, Erlangen, Germany) and a data acquisition software (Neuroscan, Sterling, VA), which was calibrated with an external 100 μV/10 Hz signal. The hardware filter was set to a bandpass from 0.1–70 Hz, A/D rate was 256 Hz. Recording references were linked mastoids. All electrode impedances were below 5 kΩ.

Data Analysis

Data analysis was performed with the program ‘Vision Analyzer’ (Brain Products, Munich, Germany). After a filtering from 0.1–70 Hz and re-referencing of the data to an average reference, a correction for ocular artifacts (Gratton and Coles, 1989) was executed. After a computerized artifact rejection (only amplitudes <50 μV were allowed in all EEG channels within 100 ms before and 700 ms after stimulus presentation), the artifact-free epochs after correct responses were segmented and averaged to one Go and one NoGo ERP per subject. For the Nogo ERP, the time point of the most positive peak at electrode position Cz within a P300 time window (275–530 ms) was used to calculate the two-dimensional topography by means of the centroid method (Lehmann, 1987), whereas the respective peak at electrode position Pz was used for the Go-condition. The location of each individual centroid on an anterior–posterior axis was determined by numbers from 1 (level of electrode position Fpz) to 5 (level of Oz) as illustrated in Figure 1 (locations somewhere in between two electrode positions were expressed by respective decimal numbers). The smaller the numbers decoding the centroid location, the more anterior is their location. For a more detailed description of the centroid method, please confer the work of Lehmann (1987) and our previous publications (Fallgatter et al, 2001). The individual NGA was calculated as the difference between Go and NoGo centroid in the anterior–posterior axis. The grand average curves of the group of patients as a whole are shown in Figure 1, along with a schematic illustration of this quantification of the NGA as the geometrical distance between Go and NoGo centroid.

Figure 1
figure 1

Grand average curves of Go (Pz; thin line) and Nogo (Cz; bold line) trials at the baseline measurement for the group of patients as a whole (n=38). Maps illustrate the distribution of the positive brain electrical field at the respective peak of the P300. Schematic illustration of the quantification of the NGA as the geometrical distance between Go and Nogo centroid in the anterior-posterior direction.

Statistical Analysis

All statistical analyses were performed with the software SPSS for Windows (version 13.0). For the CPT reaction times, the NGA, and the Verbal Fluency scores 2 × 3 analyses of variance (ANOVAs) for repeated measurements were conducted comprising the inner-subject factor ‘time’ (baseline vs 6-week-follow-up) and the between-subject factor ‘medication’ (typical vs atypical vs combined antipsychotics). Regarding the Stroop task, we created two difference scores (Stroop interference time minus Stroop color naming and Stroop word reading time, respectively), to control for nonspecific effects of typical antipsychotic medication on reaction times and overall task engagement, and to specifically analyze the interference effect of the Stroop task. The two difference measures were then subjected to two-factorial ANOVAs (‘time × medication’) as described above. The Greenhouse–Geisser procedure was used to correct the degrees of freedom whenever necessary. Post hoc analyses were conducted using two-tailed t-tests for matched or independent samples; equality of variances was tested by means of Levene's test and corrections for unequality were performed when necessary. As the error rates in the CPT and the Stroop interference condition were not normally distributed, the Kruskal–Wallis test and the Wilcoxon's test were used for between-group and within-group comparisons. Correlations between the change in the NGA and changes in neuropsychological and psychometric variables from baseline to follow-up were calculated with parametric (Pearson) or non-parametric (Spearman) methods depending on whether the variables were normally distributed.

RESULTS

Psychopathological Results

The results of the psychopathological examinations (BPRS, PANSS, and HDRS) are summarized in Table 1; ‘pre’ and ‘post’ indicate scores at the beginning of the study and at the 6-week-follow-up, results are shown separately for the three groups of patients (typical, atypical, and combined antipsychotics). ANOVAs for repeated measurements were conducted to examine the influence of the factors ‘time’ and ‘medication’ on the clinical ratings. For all the variables (BPRS, PANSS positive/negative/global and total score, and HDRS) the main result was the same: a significant main effect of the factor ‘time’ (baseline vs follow-up) was observed (PANSSneg: F1, 35=8.73, p<0.01; rest: F1, 35>40.0, p<0.001), with no main effect ‘medication’ (F2, 35<1.8, p>0.18) and no significant interaction between the two factors (F2, 35<0.7, p>0.5).

Table 1 Psychometric Results

Performance Measures

The reaction times and error rates of the three groups of patients are displayed in Table 2, separately for the first CPT measurement and the follow-up examination. Commission errors occurred whenever a patient responded to a non-target stimulus; omission errors consisted of Go conditions the patient did not respond to. For the reaction times to Go stimuli, an ANOVA with the factors ‘time’ and ‘medication’ revealed no significant main effects (‘time’: F1, 35=0.59, NS; ‘medication’: F2, 35=0.02, NS) and no significant interaction between the two factors (F2, 35=1.48, NS). The commission and omission error rates were not normally distributed, but a Kruskal–Wallis test revealed that the three groups of patients did not differ significantly regarding their error rates in the baseline or follow-up measurement (p>0.15). Regarding the change in error rates from baseline to follow-up within the three groups of patients, the Wilcoxon's test indicated a significant improvement only for the number of commission errors in the group of atypically medicated patients (Z=−2.04, p<0.05).

Table 2 Performance in the CPT

NGA

Regarding the NGA, a two-factorial ANOVA revealed a significant interaction between the factors ‘time’ and ‘medication’ (F2, 35=6.95, p<0.01), which is in accordance with our hypotheses. Patients treated with typical antipsychotics showed a trend to a decrease of the NGA over time (NGAbaseline=0.75, NGAfollow-up=0.39, t9=2.21, p=0.055), whereas patients treated with atypical antipsychotics were characterized by a stabilization or even an increase in the NGA (atypical antipsychotics alone: NGAbaseline=0.71, NGAfollow-up=0.87, t18=−1.55, NS; combined antipsychotics: NGAbaseline=0.65, NGAfollow-up=1.04, t8=−2.86, p<0.05) (Figure 2). Further post-hoc tests revealed that the group of patients treated with typical antipsychotics initially (baseline) did not differ from patients under atypical antipsychotics or from the group with a combined treatment (t17=0.21, NS and t27=0.46, NS), whereas the typical group showed a significantly diminished NGA at follow-up as compared to the atypical (t27=2.72, p<0.05) as well as the combined group (t17=2.97, p<0.01). The atypical and the combined group did not differ from each other, neither at baseline (t26=0.38) nor at follow-up (t26=0.94, NS).

Figure 2
figure 2

Mean NGA at baseline and after 6 weeks of treatment with typical (n=10), atypical (n=19), or combined (n=9) antipsychotics (follow-up). Significant differences and trends for differences between groups (next to the brackets) and over time (above or below respective lines) were marked (*p<0.05, **p<0.01,+p<0.1). Standard errors are indicated by grey vertical lines.

Neuropsychological Results

VFT

For the number of words named in the letters version of the VFT, the two-factorial ANOVA revealed a significant main effect ‘time’ (F1, 35=4.64, p<0.05) as well as a significant interaction between the factors ‘time’ and ‘medication’ (F2, 35=4.43, p<0.05). In the typically medicated group of patients the number of words decreased from 27.7 at baseline to 25.2 at follow-up (t9=1.31, NS), whereas there was an increase in this measure for the atypical group (t18=3.21, p<0.01) and also for the combined group (t8=1.96, p<0.1; cf. Table 3). For the categories version no significant main effects occurred, but again a significant interaction between the factors ‘time’ and ‘medication’ was found (F2, 35=4.70, p<0.05). With a significant decrease in the number of words achieved by the typical group (t9=4.05, p<0.01), tendencies for increases of this measure were found for the atypically medicated group (t8=2.02, p<0.1) as well as the combined group (t8=1.56, NS; cf. Table 3).

Table 3 Neuropsychological Data

Stroop test

The ANOVA conducted for the two interference difference scores (interference time—color naming, and interference time—word reading) revealed significant interactions ‘time × medication’ (F2, 35=4.29 and 5.44, respectively, p<0.05 and 0.01, respectively). These interactions were due to the fact that within the typically medicated group patients needed longer to overcome the cognitive interference created by this task at the follow-up as compared to the baseline testing (t9=1.71, NS and t9=1.81, p<0.1, for the difference measures ‘interference—color naming’ and ‘interference—word reading’, respectively), whereas the other two groups completed the task more quickly at the 6-week-follow-up testing (atypical group: t18=1.67, NS and t18=2.03, p<0.1, respectively; combined group: t8=1.39/1.68, NS; cf. Table 3).

As the errors made during the Stroop interference condition were not normally distributed, Wilcoxon's test was used to analyze changes from baseline to follow-up. In the group of patients receiving typical antipsychotic medication the error rate remained almost unchanged (cf. Table 3; Z=−0.17, NS), whereas patients receiving atypical medication alone or in combination with typical antipsychotics made more errors at baseline than at follow up (Z=−3.00, p <0.01 and Z=−2.56, p<0.05, respectively).

Exploratory Correlation Analyses

Exploratory correlation analyses were performed to test whether the change of the NGA during treatment (baseline vs follow-up) correlated with a respective change in the neuropsychological or psychometric variables. Only one statistically significant correlation was revealed: The change of the NGA over time (NGAfollow-up−NGAbaseline) correlated with the change in Stroop interference time (corrected for either word reading or color naming; Interference timefollow-up−Interference timebaseline: r=−0.674/r=−0.599, p<0.001), an improvement in the NGA being associated with a decrease in Stroop interference time (Figure 3).

Figure 3
figure 3

Correlation between changes in Stroop interference time (corrected for word reading) and changes in the NGA over the course of the treatment.

DISCUSSION

The reported analyses suggest that atypical and typical antipsychotics exert a differential influence on the NGA as an electrophysiological marker of prefrontal brain function: whereas there was a trend to an increase in the NGA after 6 weeks of treatment with atypical antipsychotics and a significant increase in the combined group, patients treated with typical antipsychotics alone tended to have a decreased NGA at the follow-up testing. This resulted in a significantly diminished NGA after 6 weeks of treatment in the group of typically medicated patients as compared to the two groups treated with atypical antipsychotics, even though the NGA did not differ significantly between groups at the baseline measurement. The electrophysiological measure of the NGA has been suggested to reflect the function of a key structure within the prefrontal cortex, the ACC, during a response inhibition task. Therefore, an improvement of this index of prefrontal brain function owing to treatment with atypical antipsychotics, as well as a decrease of this parameter under typical antipsychotics, supports the study's hypothesis.

This differential effect of atypical vs typical antipsychotic treatment on prefrontal brain function as indicated by the NGA is further corroborated by the neuropsychological findings: ANOVA for the behavioral measures in the VFT and also in the Stroop test (particularly interference time) indicated a significantly different effect between groups with a pattern of improvement in the atypical and the combined group and a deterioration in the typical group. Similarly, both groups of patients treated with atypical compounds made significantly fewer errors in the Stroop interference condition at follow-up than at baseline, whereas the error rates in the group of patients receiving typical antipsychotics remained basically unchanged.

Overall, the findings of this study are in line with the notion that atypical antipsychotics improve prefrontal brain functions (Braus et al, 2001; Ende et al, 2000; Honey et al, 1999; Ichikawa et al, 2002; Pehek and Yamamoto, 1994), whereas typical antipsychotics exert a less pronounced positive effect (Ichikawa et al, 2002; Li et al, 1998) or even interfere with prefrontal functioning (Bartlett et al, 1994; Braus et al, 2001; Madsen et al, 1998; Miller et al, 2001). Furthermore, our data are compatible with a number of previous studies indicating a superior effect of atypical antipsychotics on neurocognitive functioning in schizophrenic patients (Bilder et al, 2002; Braus et al, 2002; Cuesta et al, 2001; Gallhofer et al, 1996; Green et al, 1997; Lee et al, 1994; Purdon et al, 2001; Stip and Lussier, 1996; Velligan et al, 2002). The fact that the group of patients treated with a combination of typical and atypical antipsychotics showed results very similar to the atypically medicated group without any signs for an adverse effect of the typical compounds is not easily interpreted but seems to indicate nonlinear interactions between the two groups of neuroleptic drugs.

Moreover, the changes of the NGA over time correlated significantly with the changes in Stroop interference time (Figure 3) but not with the change in the number of words produced in the two versions of the VFT. This significant correlation is plausible as both the NGA and Stroop interference time are thought to reflect activity of the ACC. The VFT is also considered to activate prefrontal brain structures but—besides the Broca area—preferentially the DLPFC and not the ACC. In accordance with this notion, changes in Stroop interference performance significantly correlated with changes in the NGA as an index of ACC activity, whereas changes in VFT performance did not.

One weakness of this clinical study is certainly the absence of a randomization procedure for the different treatment groups. However, sound statistical analyses of patient variables (age, gender, handedness, diagnoses, number of previous hospital treatments, and duration of disease), psychometric scores (HDRS; PANSS: Positive, Negative, Global Symptomatology and total score; BPRS) and the initial NGA did not reveal any significant differences between groups at baseline. Regarding the neurocognitive measures (number of words in the letters and categories version of the VFT; Stroop word reading, color naming, and interference time; CPT performance), only the number of words in the categories version of the VFT tended to differ between the typically and the atypically medicated group of patients at baseline. None of the other neuropsychological scores revealed a significant difference between any of the groups at the initial testing (p>0.35). The absence of meaningful initial differences between the three groups of patients indicates a sufficient homogeneity between groups, which allows a thorough interpretation of the results. However, as it is impossible to completely rule out the possibility that selection effects based on covariates beyond the ones discussed above were responsible for the observed group differences, future studies with randomly assigned medication groups seem inevitable. Moreover, the relatively small and unequal sample sizes and the resulting statistical findings, that do not allow a completely unequivocal interpretation of the results, put further emphasis on the necessity of replication studies with larger sample sizes in order to verify and further investigate the differential effect of typical and atypical antipsychotics (and their interaction) on prefrontal brain function. Also, when interpreting the present findings it should be kept in mind that these might not be representative for the entire population of schizophrenic patients (relatively high percentage of female patients and relatively high mean age in the present patient sample; see Method section). Finally, future investigations in the field should be designed to test the predictive value of an initially diminished NGA for the treatment response to atypical antipsychotics.