Main

Heart rate (HR) and heart rate variability (HRV) are increasingly used to detect a variety of pathophysiologic alterations in the autonomic nervous system (ANS) regulation of cardiac control mechanisms (1). Infants who succumb to sudden infant death syndrome have higher HR and reduced HRV compared with other infants; one speculation is that autonomic disturbances may lead to cardiac instability or may indicate ANS alterations with the potential to affect other vital functions (2). Moreover, the circadian range of HR and certain quantities related to blood pressure (BP) are predictors of neonatal cardiovascular events (3). Hence, power spectrum analysis (PSA) of HRV has been used to assess fetal viability and maturity of the ANS (4,5).

Cocaine use by pregnant women has continued to be a prevalent public health problem. Prenatal cocaine exposure (PCE) may lead to abruptio placentae, premature delivery, intrauterine growth restriction, congenital malformations, and fetal death (6–8). However, controversy exists regarding the influences of PCE on the ANS control mechanism and on neonatal cardiovascular function. Some studies (9–11), have found elevated HR and reduced HRV in infants with PCE. On the other hand, Regalado et al. (12) found reduced HR and increased HRV following PCE, while another study focusing on the nonlinear dynamics of HRV (13) found no significant differences between PCE infants and controls in spectral power distribution, approximate entropy, correlation dimension, or nonlinear predictability. Spectral analyses in particular have not yielded a consensus about the nature of PCE-induced autonomic alterations. Higher HRV was attributed to increased spectral power across all frequency bands in quiet sleep and to increased spectral power across low-frequency (0.03–0.1Hz) and mid-frequency (0.1–0.2Hz) bands in active sleep (14). In contrast, Oriol et al. (9) found that spectral power during ten-minute periods of quiescent sleep was significantly smaller in infants with PCE. The apparent conflict among study results may be due partly to the differing demographic characteristics of the populations from which the samples were taken. Perhaps more importantly, numerous measures have been proposed to quantify HR and HRV; analyses based on different measures can lead to different conclusions.

Such controversy motivates the present study, in which we seek greater understanding of autonomic regulation and cardiac control mechanisms in infants with PCE compared with nonexposed infants. What distinguishes this study from those that have preceded it is that the infants were subjected to an orthostatic stress: specifically, a 25° head-up tilt took place during quiet sleep. Although the present study is not the first to involve orthostatic stress, we believe it is the first to investigate the effects of PCE on neonatal HR dynamics in response to orthostatic stress.

Regarding methodology for HR and HRV analysis, there has been a dichotomy in the literature; some analyses are based on time-domain measures, while others rely on frequency-domain measures. PSA and nonlinear analysis are commonly used since they define a full spectrum for R-wave inter-beat dynamics and can provide insights about the sources of autonomic alterations. However, frequency-domain approaches have the following limitations: 1) Information about the time sequence of HR dynamics can be lost. Without this information, it is difficult to assess how PCE affects the reaction to a specific intervention such as orthostatic stress; 2) Some frequency-domain approaches assume a certain kind of stability in the time series data; such an assumption may not be tenable when measurements take place both before and after an intervention (15,16); and 3) There is some disagreement about how to define high frequency and low frequency. Moreover, different summary statistics have been used to measure power over a frequency band, and final conclusions may be sensitive to the manner in which power is measured. 4) Frequency-domain quantities can be influenced by artifacts, requiring additional screening or noise reduction by the operator. Time-domain quantities are comparatively operator-independent (10).

The above points are not meant to impugn frequency-domain analyses but rather to explain why time-domain analyses are central to the present study, which concerns HR dynamics before and after an intervention. Temporal trajectories of HR and HRV are of primary importance as we seek to characterize the magnitudes, profiles, and durations of autonomic alterations.

SUBJECTS AND METHODS

Twenty-one infants with PCE were enrolled into the study. PCE was determined by maternal self-report or neonatal urine toxicology. Nine mothers who used cocaine also used other substances (3 – marijuana, 2 – marijuana + tobacco, 1 – marijuana and alcohol, and 3 – tobacco and alcohol). In the control group (n = 23), infants tested negative for cocaine; their mothers had no history of drug use, and were selected from the same population of African-American women as the mothers of PCE infants. Babies were recruited from the well baby nursery and neonatal intensive care unit (NICU) at the Regional Medical Center in Memphis, TN. All babies were examined within 120 h of birth. Written informed consent was obtained from each mother. Both Institutional Review Boards of the University of Tennessee and the University of Memphis approved the protocol.

Protocol and recording procedure.

Each infant had electrocardiogram (ECG) monitoring. A tilt table was placed under the infant's mattress, the infant was strapped to the table, and the table could be manually tilted to an angle of 25º. Two 30-min segments of the ECG were recorded, the first with the infant supine, and the second with the infant inclined in a 25° head-up position. ECG recordings were made with the infants in a state of quiet sleep (eyes closed, regular respiration, and no gross body movement). Recordings in the NICU were obtained with the Hewlett Packard M1046-9021C neonatal monitoring system (Palo Alto, CA), and in the well baby nursery with the NIMS Respitrace PT monitoring system (North Bay Village, FL), originally designed for the Collaborative Home Infant Monitoring Evaluation (CHIME) study (17). The R-wave peaks from each cardiac cycle in the Hewlett Packard recordings were determined using a peak detection algorithm (18); the R-R intervals had a resolution of 2 ms. The CHIME software used with the NIMS system generated R-R intervals with 1 ms resolution.

ECG artifact correction.

The Hewlett Packard recordings contained noise of both biologic and environmental origins. Noise was identified and removed through careful screening of the ECG wave form with the R-wave peak detection algorithm. After noise reduction, the ECG wave form was again input into the R-wave peak detection algorithm. The NIMS system recordings were artifact-free.

HR and HRV analysis.

Both time-domain and frequency-domain quantities were analyzed. Time-domain data included HR in beats per minute (bpm), SD of the R-R intervals (SDRR), and SD of differences between successive R-R intervals (SDDRR). SDRR is a measure of HRV that is influenced by the length and dispersion of R-R intervals. The length of R-R intervals is related to the parasympathetic control of HR, while the dispersion of R-R intervals reflects both vagal and sympathetic modulations (19). Since larger SDRR may be primarily due to longer R-R intervals rather than increased dispersion, we also considered a new quantity called the corrected SDRR (corSDRR); i.e. corrected for HR:

CorSDRR reflects dispersion of R-R intervals but not length. SDRR, SDDRR, and corSDRR were calculated using a 30-s window moved in 5-s increments; the SDRR, SDDRR, and corSDRR for each window were then assigned to the midpoint time of the window. So, for example, the SDRR at 15 s before tilt was based on data from 30 s before tilt to the time of tilting; the next SDRR was based on data from 25 s before tilt to 5 s after tilt.

Frequency-domain analyses for this study included both PSA and wavelet analysis; the former is based on a Fourier transform and assumes that data are wide-sense stationary (15,16). In PSA, the low frequency (LF) region (0.02–0.2 Hz) is viewed as a marker of both sympathetic and parasympathetic activation (20), while the high frequency (HF) region (0.2–2 Hz) is treated as a marker of parasympathetic activation (21). Low-frequency power (PL) is the integral of the power function over the LF region, high-frequency power (PH) is the integral of the power function over the HF region, and total power (PT) is the sum of the two. The ratios PL/PT, PH/PT, and PL/PH reflect the balance between sympathetic and parasympathetic activation.

Wavelet analysis tracks the temporal evolution of frequencies contained in time-series data (22). Specifically, a wavelet transform converts time-domain data into a collection of wavelet coefficients, some of which represent HF content and some of which represent LF content. A wavelet transform is like a Fourier transform, but there is an important difference: wavelet functions are localized in time, so that wavelet coefficients possess a temporal structure. Hence, a wavelet power function can be defined with time as the input. For this study, the wavelet power function was defined as the maximal wavelet coefficient among a collection of coefficients associated with a certain time. The wavelet power function was then decomposed into HF and LF wavelet power functions according to whether the maximal coefficient represented HF content or LF content. The integrals of the HF and LF wavelet power functions over the time periods when the infants were horizontal or head-up were defined as the HF and LF wavelet power for those time periods.

Statistical methods.

Data were analyzed using SAS (SAS Institute, Cary, NC), SIGMASTAT (Systat Software, Point Richmond, CA), and MATLAB (Mathworks, Natick, MA). Statistical significance was defined by a p < 0.05. For comparison of demographics and baseline characteristics, we used t-tests for continuous attributes, Fisher's exact test for dichotomous attributes, and Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests for discrete attributions.

For the time-domain analyses, linear mixed models (LMM) were fit to HR and log-transformed HRV data, as the distributions of untransformed SDRR, SDDRR, and corSDRR were right-skewed. Time was treated as a categorical variable to provide flexibility in modeling the temporal dynamics of HR and HRV. The LMM included both fixed effects and random effects. The random effects were included to accommodate intrasubject correlations. Ignoring these correlations could lead to inaccurate conclusions about the fixed effects; condensing the data to circumvent these correlations would discard information about the temporal trajectories of HR and HRV. We chose variance-covariance structures for the LMM by comparing the values of Akaike Information Criteria and Bayesian Information Criteria obtained with different variance-covariance structures, but variance and covariance parameters whose estimates were less than twice their standard errors were eliminated. Post hoc tests were conducted to investigate the magnitudes and durations of changes associated with the orthostatic stress; global tests were performed before the post hoc tests to ensure control of Type I error probabilities.

For the wavelet analysis, two more LMM were fit: one had as the response LF wavelet power, the other had as the response HF wavelet power, and both models included group, position, and their interaction as predictors. For the PSA, t-tests (or, in cases of strong departures from normality, nonparametric alternatives) were used to compare PL/PT, PH/PT, and PL/PH at the two positions within each group and between the two groups at each position.

RESULTS

Characteristics of enrolled subjects.

Demographics and baseline characteristics for the two groups are summarized in Table 1. Mothers who used cocaine were older than mothers of control infants (p = 0.004). No differences were noted between PCE and control groups as to the infants' age at enrollment, gender, birth measurements, Apgar scores, or percentages admitted to the NICU.

Table 1 Demographics and baseline characteristics for the PCE and control groups

Changes in HR.

Since any effects of the orthostatic stress would be predominantly instantaneous, the HR from 15 s before tilt to 15 s after tilt were analyzed and summarized in Fig. 1. The LMM results are presented in Table 2. There was a significant main effect for group (p = 0.024) but not for time (p = 0.104). In the 15 s preceding tilt, HR was marginally higher among PCE infants (140.53 ± 3.11 bpm) than among controls (133.16 ± 2.97 bpm, p = 0.091). During the 15 s following tilt, mean HR was significantly higher in exposed infants (147.01 ± 3.11 bpm) than in controls (136.27 ± 2.97 bpm, p = 0.015). There was a significant interaction between group and time (p = 0.049), prompting the post hoc tests with results shown in Table 2. The PCE infants had a significant sustained increase in HR during the 15 s after tilt compared with the 15 s pre-tilt (increase of 6.48 ± 2.85 bpm, p = 0.024). The controls had also an increase at the time of tilt but transient, only for a duration of 2 s (increase = 4.69 ± 2.28 bpm, p = 0.040). Moreover, the controls responded to the tilt instantaneously, while PCE infants had a delayed response. The increase in HR from 1 s before tilt to the time of tilt was higher for controls (difference = 5.14 ± 1.56 bpm, p = 0.001), while the increase from tilt to 1 s after tilt was higher for cocaine-exposed infants (difference = 7.09 ± 1.56 bpm, p < 0.001).

Figure 1
figure 1

Group means and standard errors for HR within 15 s of tilting (* time 0 is the tilt time); — Cocaine (n = 21), --- Control (n = 23).

Table 2 Post hoc comparisons for HR within 15 seconds of tilting

Log (SDDRR).

The LMM for log (SDDRR) was fit using the data from 30 s before tilt to 30 s after tilting. This accords with the time interval used in the model for HR since information from 15 s before tilt was required to calculate the value of log (SDDRR) at 30 s before tilting. Data are summarized in Fig. 2, and the LMM results are presented in Table 3. There was no significant main effect for group, but there was a significant main effect for time (p < 0.001), with interaction between group and time (p = 0.049).

Figure 2
figure 2

Group means and standard errors for log (SDDRR) within 30 s of tilting. The SDRR, SDDRR, and corrected SDRR were calculated using a 30-s window moved in 5-s increments; the SDRR, SDDRR, and corrected SDRR for each window were then assigned to the midpoint time of the window. We defined a pre-tilting segment (times: −30, −25, −20 s), an intra-tilting segment (times: −15, −10, −5, 0, 5, 10, 15 s), and a post-tilting segment (times: 20, 25, 30 s). — Cocaine (n = 21); --- Control (n = 23).

Table 3 Post hoc comparisons for log (SDDRR) within 30 seconds of tilting

We therefore partitioned the interval from 30 s before tilt to 30 s after tilt into three segments: a pre-tilt segment (times: −30, −25, −20 s), an intra-tilt segment (times: −15, −10, −5, 0, 5, 10, 15 s), and a post-tilt segment (times: 20, 25, 30 s). Thus, log (SDDRR) in the pre-tilt segment would not be affected by orthostatic stress, log (SDDRR) in the intra-tilt segment would be influenced by whatever instantaneous effects the tilting had, and log (SDDRR) in the post-tilt segment would be influenced by whatever sustained effects the tilting had.

For control infants, there was a marginal increase in log (SDDRR) from the pre-tilt segment to the intra-tilt segment (increase = 0.12 ± 0.07 log ms, p = 0.099) and then a pronounced decrease from the intra-tilt segment to the post-tilt segment (decrease = 0.26 ± 0.07 log ms, p = 0.001). Hence, log (SDDRR) was noticeably greater inside the intra-tilt segment compared with either baseline or post-tilt segments (difference = 0.19 ± 0.05 log ms, p < 0.001). Log (SDDRR) was not appreciably different between the pre- and post-tilt segments (p = 0.232). The PCE infants reacted quite differently. There was an increase in log (SDDRR) from the pre-tilt segment to the intra-tilt segment (increase = 0.23 ± 0.08 log ms, p = 0.003), with the elevated levels of log (SDDRR) persisting from the intra-tilt segment into the post-tilt segment. Consequently, log (SDDRR) during the intra-tilt and post-tilt segments was markedly higher than during the pre-tilt segment (difference = 0.25 ± 0.09 log ms, p = 0.004). Finally, log (SDDRR) peaked at different times for the two groups. Figure 2 shows that, for control infants, log (SDDRR) increased at 15 s pre-tilt (i.e. at the first time tilting effects could have been manifested in SDDRR) and peaked at 10 s before tilting. Moreover, log (SDDRR) remained elevated until 10 s after tilting but then dropped abruptly to levels that were maintained through the post-tilt segment. The responses of PCE infants were comparatively slow. At 15 s before tilt, there was no increase in log (SDDRR). Then log (SDDRR) increased steadily, beginning at 10 s before tilt and continuing to a peak at 10 s after tilt. Notably, log (SDDRR) did not return to normal levels in the post-tilt segment.

Log (corSDRR).

The data are summarized in Fig. 3. Neither main effect for group (p = 0.373) nor interaction between group and time (p = 0.372) was significant. Even so, the highly significant main effect for time (p < 0.001) warrants examining the trajectories of log (corSDRR) for all babies; log (corSDRR) increased significantly during the intra-tilt segment from pre-tilt (increase = 0.24 ± 0.04 log ms, p < 0.0001), with subsequent significant decrease at post-tilt segment (decrease = 0.23 ± 0.04 log ms, p < 0.001). Similar trajectories for log (corSDRR) for the PCE infants and controls were observed with log (SDRR) measurements with orthostatic stress.

Figure 3
figure 3

Group means and standard errors for log (corrected SDRR) within 30 s of tilting, — Cocaine (n = 21); --- Control (n = 23).

Wavelet analysis.

For concordance with time-domain analyses, LF wavelet power was defined as the integral of the LF wavelet power function over either the 15 s before tilting (horizontal position) or the 15 s after tilting (head-up position). There was a significant main effect for group (p = 0.039), attributable to the lower LF wavelet power for PCE infants in both the horizontal (29.43 ± 4.56 versus 40.54 ± 5.77 for control) and head-up positions (28.24 ± 5.24 versus 41.26 ± 7.66 for control). However, there was neither a significant main effect for position nor a significant interaction between group and position. The analogous LMM for HF wavelet power yielded no significant results.

Power spectrum analysis.

The stationarity assumption required for the PSA would not be tenable if we confined attention to the 15-s intervals immediately before and after tilting. Hence, the PSA entailed comparisons of PL/PT, PH/PT, and PL/PH based on the 5-min intervals immediately before and after tilting. For controls, there was a significant decrease in PL/PT from 0.80± to 0.75 ± 0.03 (p = 0.023) after tilt and a significant increase in PH/PT, 0.20 ± 0.02 to 0.25 ± 0.0.03 (p = 0.023). For PCE infants, there were no significant changes in any of the ratios. There were no significant differences between PCE and control infants in any of the ratios either pre- or post-tilt.

DISCUSSION

This study has elucidated the effects of PCE on ANS regulation of cardiovascular function, documenting a previously unreported interaction between PCE and orthostatic stress. The interaction was most perceptible in the time-domain analyses of HR and log (SDDRR). While the controls had instantaneous but transient responses to the orthostatic stress, PCE infants had responses that were both delayed and prolonged. Although the frequency-domain analyses also made some distinction between controls and PCE infants, the inherent condensation of information associated with power spectral analyses may have limited what could be inferred from them.

Tachycardia in PCE infants may be attributed to increased sympathetic nervous system activity after exposure to a sympathomimetic drug. The increased sympathetic activity following orthostatic stress may represent an exaggerated carotid baroreceptor response. Head tilt-up position results in a small transient decrease in BP initiating a neurally mediated compensatory vasoconstriction and increase in heart rate (23). While we did not measure BP in our infants, Andreissen (24) and others have described maturation of baroreceptor reflex sensitivity (BRS) as infants reach term gestation. Thus in our control infants we can assume mature BRS and their HR changes reflect their BRS and autonomic cardiovascular control. While tilting led to elevations in HR for both the PCE and control infants in this study, the differences between groups suggest alterations in the neural control mechanisms in infants with PCE based on differences as to time to respond, degree of increase in HR, and duration of response.

HRV has been found increased, decreased, or essentially unchanged (10,12,13,25), for cocaine-exposed infants vis-a-vis controls. Our study, partly motivated by the presence of such conflicting results in the literature, found an interaction between PCE and orthostatic stress. This interaction was most evident with log (SDDRR) as the measure of HRV. Cocaine-exposed infants had decreased HRV in the pre-tilt and intra-tilt segments compared with controls, but they had similar or increased HRV in the post-tilt segment. Moreover, the effects of cocaine exposure on ANS regulation of cardiovascular function were manifested as differences in the times to peak HRV and the duration of elevated HRV.

For both PCE and controls, orthostatic stress led to increases also in log (SDRR), and log (corrected SDRR). The similar trajectories for log (SDRR) and log (corSDRR) suggest that both groups responded to tilt with changes (increases) in HRV. These increases in the presence of elevated HR likely reflect both sympathetic and vagal modulation in both PCE and controls. However the lower HRV measurements in exposed infants compared with controls pre- and intra-tilt, suggest the predominance of sympathetic modulation in the ANS control of cardiovascular function in PCE. In eliciting cardiovascular response to a different stress, DiPietro et al. reported shortened RR or increase in HR following an auditory stimulus, and with a trend to an increase in vagal tone (25).

The present study addressed how PCE affects responses from the sympathetic adrenal medullary (SAM) axis. In general, stressors can elicit responses from both the SAM and hypothalamic pituitary adrenal (HPA) axes. Animal studies also have demonstrated that ACTH and glucocorticoids contribute to cardiovascular regulation through modulation of the baroreceptor sensitivity and sympathetic activity (26,27). Effects for the HPA axis are usually quantified by differences in subjects' salivary cortisol levels before and after exposure to stressors. Magnamo (28) reported that, after being subjected to both noninvasive and invasive stressors, cocaine-exposed infants had significantly lower levels of cortisol than controls and that the effects of cocaine did not dissipate with the clearance of cocaine from an infant's system. Further, the lower cortisol levels may explain the state regulatory problems noted in PCE. Levine (29) found that higher cortisol levels were associated with better scores on measures of state regulation. More investigation is required to determine the mechanisms of suppressed cortisol response attributable to PCE (28). Studies will need also to examine the integration of the responses of the two systems (SAM and HPA) to stress.

Some studies have speculated that prenatal cocaine exposure may have long-term effects on autonomic function. A prospective cohort study by Mehta et al. (11) found that infants with PCE still had altered autonomic function at 2–6 mo of age. Although studies (30,31) suggest that vagal tone increases in normal children aged 3–6, it is not clear whether the same is true of children who were exposed to cocaine in utero. Further research is needed to clarify to what extent the effects of prenatal cocaine exposure persist into childhood, especially in response to stress. Investigators have also correlated the changes in HR and HRV according to levels of cocaine exposure and according to sleep states. We were not able to address these in our study and thus, this is a limitation. Also, with our small sample, it was not possible to analyze for the confounding effects of other drugs. Nevertheless, we were able to characterize the effects of PCE on cardiovascular control when BRS response is elicited. More importantly, we observed that the effects of PCE on the neonatal ANS could be demonstrated through the instantaneous HR and HRV changes in response to a stimulus such as the orthostatic stress.