Main

Intellectual performance appears to be negatively associated with important health outcomes such as all-cause mortality (14) and cardiovascular disease (1). Developmental research has provided evidence that insults during critical periods in early life may have lifetime consequences (5). Numerous epidemiologic studies have reported that perinatal factors including birth weight, birth length, and gestational age may influence later intellectual performance, but the mechanisms for these associations are unclear (619). Understanding the early life influences on intellectual performance may be important for developing initiatives aimed at preventing adverse outcomes in later life.

The association between birth size and intellectual performance may be wholly or partly explained by later growth rather than perinatal factors (20) because birth size is known to be directly related to adult height (21) and a positive correlation between adult height and intelligence has been reported (6,10,2225). To our knowledge, only one study has so far examined the independent effects of birth weight and current size on intelligence, but this study did not include gestational age (10). Furthermore, only a small number of population-based studies exists on the association between birth weight and intelligence (18), and of these studies, the largest did not adjust for socioeconomic status (26).

To assess the associations of birth weight, birth length, and gestational age with adult intellectual performance and to explore whether the associations were influenced by adult body size and social factors, we conducted a historical birth cohort study following a large sample of male infants registered in the Medical Birth Registry of Norway and linked to data registered at the Norwegian Conscript Service as well as Statistics Norway.

METHODS

Study population.

In Norway, medical data on all births from 16 wk of gestation have been recorded since 1967 by the Medical Birth Registry of Norway (27). Data on general intellectual performance, height, and weight have been routinely recorded by the Norwegian Conscripts Service in all Norwegian men at the age of 18 y; only those who are permanently disabled before this age are exempt from attending (21,28). Data on delivery were linked with data on intellectual performance recorded by the Norwegian Conscript Service 1984–1999, by the unique 11-digit personal identification number. In addition, we added information on the highest attained maternal educational level in 1998 from Statistics Norway (29) and on disability status from the National Health Insurance Office (30). From 1967 to 1979, 393,570 singleton liveborn infant boys were registered in the Medical Birth Registry. In this birth cohort, 8383 (2.1%) died before military draft, 3788 (1.0%) emigrated, 5692 (1.4%) were permanently disabled, and 24,355 (6.2%) were untraceable (21). Birth size characteristics for this cohort have been published previously (21). Analyses were restricted to conscripts with data on intelligence testing and maternal educational level. Data on intellectual performance was missing for 18,979 of the drafted men, and another 14,612 men were excluded due to missing data on maternal educational level, thus excluding 33,591 (8.5%) men. Consequently, 317,761 births (80.7% of the birth cohort) were left for analysis.

Variables.

Intellectual performance was measured by a 53-min standardized group intelligence test, which was developed in 1953 for the Norwegian draft board and revised in 1962. The revised version was used throughout the study period. The test included time-limited subtests organized by increasing difficulty covering three categories of items: verbal analogues, number series (calculation) and geometric figures (an abbreviated version of Raven's Progressive Matrices) (31), comprising a total of 120 questions. All conscripts received standard instructions before the tests. These tests are highly correlated with the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (r = 0.73) (28,31,32). The performance is reported in nine standard scores; i.e. single-digit standard scores (with values from 1 to 9) based on the normal distribution in 1962 with a mean of 5.0 and SD of 1.96. The overall mean score was 5.20 and slightly higher than the standardized mean (33), which is in accordance with the gain in intellectual performance tests during the time period (28). A low score was defined as ≤3, corresponding to the 16th percentile of the distribution.

Data on birth weight (g), birth length (cm), gestational age (weeks), and year of birth, as well as data on the mother at the time of birth [age (years), marital status (married or unmarried), and parity (number of previous births, including stillbirths)] were obtained from the Medical Birth Registry. Data on birth weight, birth length, and gestational age were missing for 415 (0.1% of the study cohort), 3532 (1.1%), and 10,570 (2.8%) births, respectively. Data on year of birth, maternal age, marital status, and parity were complete. Birth weight was categorized as <1500 g, 1500–4999 g in 500-g categories, and ≥5000 g. The gestational age–specific z score (SD above or below the mean) of birth weight was calculated using Norwegian population standards (34), and the z score of birth length was calculated based on the same material. z Scores were categorized as ≤−3.00, −2.99–2.00, −1.99–0.50, −0.49–0.50, 0.51–2.00, 2.01–3.00, and >3.00. Gestational age was estimated from the reported last menstrual period and analyzed as completed weeks of gestation. Preterm birth was defined as gestational age <37 wk, 37–41 wk as term, and 42–44 wk as postterm. Maternal educational level (completed years) was categorized into low (≤10 y), medium (11–14 y), or high (≥14 y).

Data on adult height (cm) and weight (kg) were missing for 400 and 636 men, respectively. Height was analyzed as a continuous variable (1-cm increments). Quintiles and z scores for height were calculated based on data from the study cohort. We categorized body mass index (BMI), i.e. weight (kg)/height (m2), as <18.5, 18.5–24.9, 25.0–29.9, 30.0–39.9, and ≥40.0, consistent with the cutoff points proposed by the World Health Organization (35).

Statistics.

The relationship between birth size and mean intelligence test scores was explored by stratification and analysis of variance (crude and adjusted analyses); comparisons were expressed as mean differences with 95% confidence intervals (CIs) (Tables 1 and 2). For models in Table 2, all independent variables except height were treated as categorical. Because the relationship between height and intellectual performance is not purely linear (22), height was included with both linear and quadratic regression terms.

Table 1 Mean intelligence test scores with 95% CI among 317,761 drafted males by birth and maternal characteristics
Table 2 Mean intelligence test scores by gestational age, birth weight, and length

In Table 3, we applied the analytic strategy suggested by Lucas et al. (20) using z scores and adjusting for later size to interpret the relative importance of early and later body size; however, we emphasized the importance of birth characteristics. R2 was used to quantify the variance in intellectual performance. We used standard analysis of variance models for these analyses. We excluded the quadratic height-term because this did not contribute to the R2 estimation. z Scores with values outside ±4 SD were excluded.

Table 3 Multivariate linear regression analysis with estimated regression coefficients with SE and measure for explained variance (R2) of birth size measures on intelligence test score among 289,252 drafted males born at gestational age ≥37 wk with complete information on all variables

For dichotomous outcomes (Table 4), crude odds ratios (ORs) were calculated, and logistic regression analysis was used to adjust for potential confounding. Interactions were evaluated in stratified analyses and with specific interaction terms in the general linear or logistic models.

Table 4 ORs (95% CI) of intelligence test scores ≤3 among male conscripts by birth characteristics

In all analyses, maternal age, parity, maternal education, and BMI were treated as categorical (indicator) variables. SPSS software (version 11.0, SPSS, Chicago, IL) was used for the statistical analyses.

The Regional Committee for Medical Research Ethics, the Norwegian Board of Health, and the Norwegian Data Inspectorate approved the study.

RESULTS

In the study cohort, mean weight and length at birth was 3586 g (SD = 537) and 51.0 cm (SD = 2.2). Mean gestational age was 39.9 wk (SD = 1.9) with 4.5% preterm, 80.3% term, and 15.2% postterm births. At conscription, mean height was 179.9 cm (SD = 6.5), and mean BMI was 22.3 kg/m2 (SD = 3.1). Mean age was 18.7 y (SD = 0.7) with 95.3% aged 18–19 y.

Figure 1 presents the relationship between gestational age, birth size, and mean intelligence test score. Mean score increased with increasing gestational age from 28 wk (SD = 4.40) to 40–41 wk (SD = 5.27), but decreased thereafter for the highest gestational ages (5.10 at 44 wk) (Fig. 1A). Figure 1B and C show the relationship between adult intellectual performance and weight and length at birth, respectively, stratified by gestational age. There was a positive association for term births between adult intellectual performance and birth weight (Fig. 1B), as well as birth length (Fig. 1C), but with a tendency toward lower scores in the upper categories (Fig. 1B and C). The scores for preterm births were higher than for term births for birth weights <3000 g and birth lengths <50 cm. For example, for birth weights 2000–2499 g, mean score was 4.99 versus 4.84, respectively, for preterm and term births (p < 0.0005). In general, the association between birth size and intellectual performance was weaker for preterm compared with term births.

Figure 1
figure 1

Mean intelligence test score with 95% CIs by gestational age (A), birth weight (B), and birth length (C) stratified by gestational age; 317,761 male infants, Medical Birth Registry of Norway, 1967–1979, linked with the Norwegian Conscripts Service, 1984–1999. Term = 37–41 wk gestational age, preterm = <37 wk gestational age. , preterm; term.

Table 1 presents mean intelligence test scores at conscription stratified by potential confounding factors. Maternal age, maternal educational level, and year of birth were positively associated with intellectual performance, whereas there was a negative association with maternal parity and mother being unmarried. There was an almost linear increase in intelligence test score by increasing adult height within each level of maternal education (Fig. 2A), whereas the association between intellectual performance and BMI was curvilinear, with declining scores at BMIs above the normal range (Fig. 2B).

Figure 2
figure 2

Mean intelligence test scores with 95% CIs among 317,761 drafted males, by height (A) and BMI (B) at conscription, stratified by three levels of maternal education. The Norwegian Conscripts Service (1984–1999) linked with Statistics Norway (1967–1998). , ≥14y; , 11–14y; , <11y.

Table 2 shows the associations of gestational age and birth size with intelligence test score. The reference groups were chosen based on the observed modes in Figure 1. Mean scores consistently decreased by lower gestational ages in both unadjusted analyses and analyses adjusted for maternal age, parity, maternal educational level, adult height, and BMI. The effects on intellectual performance were strong for all factors in Table 2, both unadjusted and adjusted (p < 0.0005). Analyses of the association between birth size and intellectual performance were complicated by the strong interaction between birth weight or birth length and gestational age on intellectual performance (p < 0.0005 for both, post hoc analysis), as described above. Thus, to separate the effects of growth retardation from immaturity, preterm births were excluded in analyses of the associations of birth weight and birth length with intellectual performance. Consequently, in Table 2, the low birth weight or short length categories are most likely growth-restricted babies. In crude analyses, intelligence test score decreased by lower birth weights and shorter birth lengths. To evaluate the contribution of later growth to this association separately from that of social factors, we made two sets of adjustments. First, adjustment for maternal age, parity, and maternal educational level only slightly changed the estimated intelligence test scores. Next, further adjustment for adult height and BMI resulted in marked attenuation of the associations. Additional adjustment for year of birth and marital status did not change the results. The decline in scores observed in Figure 1 for the highest categories of birth weight, as well as gestational age, remained after adjustment.

There was no statistical evidence of interaction on intellectual performance between birth weight and adult height or birth length and adult height, either between birth size and BMI or between birth size and maternal education (p > 0.05 for all).

Table 3 shows estimated regression coefficients (β) and the proportions of variation (R2) in intellectual performance explained by birth size (z scores). Again, preterm births were excluded due to the strong interactions between gestational age and birth size (Fig. 2). In this context, β represents the increase in intelligence by 1 SD increase in birth weight or length. β values for birth weight and birth length were 0.107 (SE = 0.003) and 0.116 (SE = 0.003), respectively, but were slightly reduced after inclusion of maternal education in the model [0.086 (SE = 0.003) and 0.097 (SE = 0.003), respectively]. However, after adding maternal age, parity, maternal education, adult height, and BMI to the model, the β values were strongly reduced [0.054 (SE = 0.003) and 0.041 (SE = 0.003), respectively]. Further adjustment with height as quadratic term did not influence the regression coefficients. Unadjusted β for adult height (1 SD) was 0.208 (SE = 0.005) and remained unchanged when either birth weight or birth length were included in the model, but was reduced when further considering maternal age, parity, and maternal educational level (data not shown).

The overall R2 of intellectual performance explained by either weight or length at birth was 0.003 or 0.004, respectively (Table 3). Adding maternal age, parity, maternal education, adult height, and BMI increased R2 to 0.120 for both. Thus, 12% of the variance in intellectual performance was explained by these factors in addition to birth size.

Table 4 shows the ORs for low intelligence test scores associated with birth characteristics, with adjustment for maternal age, parity, maternal educational level, adult height, and BMI. Compared with those born at 39–41 wk, infants born at gestational ages 26–29, 30–33, and 34–36 wk, had an OR for low score of 2.19 (95% CI: 1.77–2.70), 1.44 (95% CI: 1.31–1.58), and 1.21 (95% CI: 1.15–1.27), respectively. In addition, the OR was increased among postterm infants (OR = 1.09; 95% CI: 1.06–1.12). These ORs were similarly elevated after adjustment. Table 4 also shows the ORs for low score associated with z scores for birth weight (excluding preterm births). The crude ORs ranged from 1.75 (95% CI: 1.53–2.02) for z scores ≤−3.00 to 1.17 (95% CI: 1.14–1.20) for z scores between −1.99 and −0.50. These ORs were reduced after adjustment for maternal age, parity, and maternal educational level but were still increased even after further adjustment for current body size. Large infants (z score for birth weight >3.00) had a slightly elevated risk of low score (1.22; 95% CI: 1.00–1.48) when all factors were included in the model. The results for z scores for birth length were essentially similar to that observed for birth weight.

DISCUSSION

Birth weight, birth length, and gestational age are associated with intellectual performance, even after adjustment for social factors and adult body size; however, the associations are weak.

The strengths of this study are the large sample size, the population-based design, and the high degree of follow-up; by linking national registers, 94% of the birth cohort was traced until age 18 y. However, infants born preterm or with low birth weight were more likely lost to follow-up and not drafted because the proportions of infant deaths and disability were higher than among term and normal weight infants (21). Also conscripts who had missing data on intellectual performance had slightly lower birth weight (3559 g), birth length (50.9 cm), and gestational age (39.8 wk) compared with the study cohort. Hence, a selection bias toward a healthy population might be possible. This may affect the validity regarding intellectual performance. For preterm and low birth weight infants, not attending the draft board may be associated with lower intellectual performance, implying that the observed mean scores would be higher than the true values. Thus, if data on those lost to follow-up had been available, the observed associations probably would have been strengthened. However, despite loss to follow-up, we observed low scores for the lowest gestational age and birth size categories, as expected according to previous studies using somewhat different types of data and design (611,13,19). Furthermore, our data compare with other Scandinavian studies using military draft data from practically the same time period and including males only (6,7,19). Still, the restriction to male participants may limit the external validity of our findings.

In our study, data on gestational age and birth length were available, thereby providing a better measure of prenatal growth than indicated by birth weight alone. Gestational age in the Medical Birth Registry was based on self-reported information about the last menstrual period because ultrasound dating was not recorded in this cohort of births. Although infants with z scores for birth weight (or birth length) by gestational age outside 4 SD were excluded in the linear models (34), misclassification is still likely.

The intelligence test used in our study correlates r = 0.73 with the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, which is somewhat lower than in the study by Sørensen et al. (r = 0.8) (7). Still, to assess intellectual ability and suitability for military service, this standardized test has been considered appropriate and applied unchanged as part of the draft board examination, compulsory for every able young man in Norway since the early 1950s.

More direct measures of maternal intelligence and socioeconomic status, important predictors of offspring's intellectual outcomes (13,14), were unavailable. Although maternal educational attainment is a strong proxy measure of intelligence (2,4), residual confounding may still be present. Maternal education also served as an indicator of socioeconomic status, together with maternal age, marital status, and parity. In general, education is found to be more strongly and consistently associated with health and disease than is income or occupation (36). Still, residual confounding by socioeconomic status is likely. Unfortunately, neither data on postnatal factors, such as nutrition, psychological stimulation, childhood illness, and lifestyle, nor data for evaluating the possible influence of stress hormones or growth factors as suggested by other researchers (9,10,18), were available in our registries.

The association of birth size with intellectual performance was weak, as summarized in a recent review (18). Most previous studies on the association between birth weight and intellectual performance do not consider the contribution of current size (79,11,14,16,17), whereas others have shown an effect of later growth in terms of height (6,10,25). Some studies did not control for maternal education (68,12,25) or gestational age (9,10). Although it was weak, we observed a prenatal influence on intellectual performance independent of the contribution of social factors and adult size, consistent with Richards et al. (10), but in contrast to Pearce et al. (25). This may reflect biological processes occurring during fetal growth that affect adult intelligence, as suggested by the fetal programming hypothesis (37). Conversely, genetic or environmental factors may affect prenatal growth as well as later growth and intelligence, without going through the programming pathway.

It has been suggested that postnatal growth has a greater influence on intelligence than fetal growth (25). Also, Martyn et al. (38) noted that prenatal growth seems to be less important than genetic factors and environmental influences in postnatal life in determining adult cognitive performance. Lucas et al. (20) have suggested that adult size must be taken into consideration when examining early life exposures and outcomes in later life. However, there is some discussion about whether adjusting for variables that may be on the causal pathway is appropriate (39,40). We found that the association between birth size and intellectual performance was attenuated after adjustment for adult size, while the influence of adult height on intellectual performance was almost unchanged. Our interpretation is that adult height dominated the effects of birth size, slightly more for birth length than for birth weight. Neither adult weight nor BMI had a similar attenuating effect. Birth size and adult height are measures of growth that are strongly correlated (21). In contrast, birth size is less correlated with maternal education (8). However, adjustment for current body size may be wrong. For example, if fetal growth, including brain growth, is impaired and insulin resistance is induced to prepare for shortage of food, then adult obesity may be an epiphenomenon to the association between birth weight and intellectual performance, and adjusting for current body size may be misleading. In our study, if adult height (and BMI) is on the causal pathway between birth weight and intellectual performance, adjustment for adult height may introduce a bias because adult height and intellectual performance may have unmeasured common causal factors (e.g. childhood nutrition). Such a bias may lead to an underestimation of the association between birth size and intellectual performance. Hence, the strong influence of adult height on the association between birth size and intellectual performance could be explained by overadjusting for factors on the causal pathway. Conversely, these factors are strongly linked to maternal education (Fig. 2); thus, adjusting for maternal education will probably close this backdoor path and the assumed bias will be reduced.

Only a small part of the variation in intellectual performance was due to birth size (R2 = 0.3%–0.4%). Even the full model (including birth size, adult size, and social factors) explained only 12%, and most of the variation was explained by maternal education (R2 = 8.4%). Similar results have been reported in previous studies (8,14,24). Maternal education appeared to be an important confounder when evaluating prenatal influences on offspring's intellectual performance, possibly by inequalities in social conditions, as well as by the influence of maternal genes.

Gestational age is relevant in the interpretation of birth weight as an indicator of prenatal growth. Consistent with a recent Swedish study (19), we observed an interaction between birth size and gestational age on intellectual performance (Fig. 1). In our study, approximately 40% of the preterm infants had a birth weight <2500 g compared with 1% of term infants. For infants with birth weight 2000–2499 g, those born preterm actually performed better than those born at term. This result may seem surprising, but these births represent a low birth weight in term births, but not in preterm births. The study by Bergvall et al. (19) seems to provide similar results, although they do not compare term and preterm births directly. It has been suggested that the importance of birth weight on neurodevelopmental outcome is lower than that of gestational age (19). The interpretation of our finding may be that compared with term births, birth size in preterm infants is less predictive of the intellectual potential. In preterm infants, attained birth size may have been influenced by complications in pregnancy leading to preterm birth, whereas at term, small size is more likely to reflect growth retardation.

We found a decline in scores among postterm births, as observed in the study by Record et al. (26). Also, there was a slight decrease in intelligence test score at the highest birth weights, as observed in two previous studies (7,8). These results are a supplement to previous studies, but must be interpreted carefully in clinical settings and cannot be applied to the history of any one child.

Although these historical data do not allow us to speculate on the biological pathways, our conclusion is that the positive association between birth size and intellectual performance is influenced by later growth, but still intellectual performance is dominated by maternal education.