Main

Corticosteroids, in particular DEX, have been widely used to prevent or treat CLD in preterm infants (13). However, there is growing clinical and experimental evidence that this therapy may adversely affect somatic growth and neurodevelopmental outcomes (48). Three recent retrospective studies have shown that postnatal HC treatment for CLD had less prominent or even absence of adverse effects on neurodevelopment compared with DEX therapy at school age (e.g. a reduction of cortical gray matter volume) (911). In addition, it has also been shown that HC-treated infants had significantly better school performance in comparison with their DEX-treated counterparts at 5–7 y of age (9). Therefore, it was deemed of interest to compare in detail the long-term effects of DEX and HC treatment on brain function during early development. Because controlled prospective and mechanistic studies in human material are limited, a plausible way to address these questions is using an experimental animal model that closely reproduces a clinical setting for corticosteroid treatment in preterm human neonates. In the present study, we used a clinically relevant rodent model to compare long-term effects of neonatal treatment with DEX and HC on hippocampal synaptic plasticity and associative memory in adolescent rats. Our results indicate that neonatal DEX, but not HC, treatment switched the direction of synaptic plasticity, favoring LTD over LTP, in the hippocampal CA1 region, as well as an impaired memory retention subjected to a one-trial passive avoidance learning task and that these alterations are highly correlated with an increase in the autophosphorylation of α isoform of Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II (αCaMKII) at threonine-286 and a decrease in the protein phosphatase 1 (PP1) expression.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Animals.

All procedures were performed in accordance with National Institutes of Health guidelines for animal research and approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at National Cheng Kung University. Within 6–8 h after delivery (d 0), pups were removed from their nests and six male pups were placed back with each dam. Pups were weaned at 21 d of age and remained group housed with littermates until experimentation at 5 wk of age.

Drug treatment.

Three treatment groups were assigned for each litter: saline (SAL)-, DEX-, and HC-treated animals. Pups in the DEX and HC groups received a daily i.p. injection of DEX or HC (Sigma Chemical Co., St. Louis, MO) between P1 and P3. DEX was given in tapering doses of 0.5 mg/kg on P1, 0.3 mg/kg on P2, and 0.1 mg/kg on P3. HC was given in tapering doses of 5 mg/kg on P1, 3 mg/kg on P2, and 1 mg/kg on P3. Doses of DEX and HC were selected based on published studies (7,8). Animals in the vehicle group received equivalent volumes of intraperitoneal injection of sterile SAL.

Hippocampal slice preparations and electrophysiology.

Hippocampal slices (400 μm thick) were prepared from 5-wk-old rats using standard procedures (8); the rats were allowed to recover for a minimum of 1 h and then were transferred to a submersion-type recording chamber continually perfused with 30°C–32°C oxygenated artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF) solution (in mM: NaCl, 117; KCl, 4.7; CaCl2, 2.5; MgCl2, 1.2; NaHCO3, 25; NaH2PO4, 1.2; glucose, 11). Extracellular field potential recordings were carried out with Axoclamp-2B amplifier (Molecular Devices, Union City, CA). The responses were low pass–filtered at 2 kHz, digitally sampled at 5–10 kHz, and analyzed using pCLAMP software (Version 8.0; Molecular Devices). Postsynaptic responses were evoked in CA1 stratum radiatum by stimulation of Schaffer collateral/commissural afferents at 0.033 Hz with a bipolar stimulating electrode. Field excitatory postsynaptic potentials (fEPSPs) were recorded with a glass pipette filled with 1 M NaCl (2–3 MΩ resistance) and the initial slope was measured. The long-term potentiation (LTP) was induced by high-frequency stimulation (HFS) at the test pulse intensity, consisting of two 1-s trains of stimuli separated by an intertrain interval of 20 s at 100 Hz. Long-term depression (LTD) was induced by low-frequency stimulation (LFS) delivered at 1 Hz for 15 min (900 pulses).

Passive avoidance training and testing.

One-trial passive avoidance learning task was selected as the tool for behavioral assessment to measure associative memory retention performance in rats from each group as described previously (8). Each rat was placed individually in the lighted compartment of an automated passive avoidance system (Ugo Basile, Comerio, Italy) and, after entering the dark compartment, was given a scrambled foot shock (0.5 mA for 2 s). The retention test was conducted 24 h later with the rat again being placed in the lighted compartment and subjected to the same protocol described above in the absence of foot shock. The latency (maximum 300 s) to enter the dark compartment was scored.

Preparation of postsynaptic density fractions.

Isolation of postsynaptic density (PSD) was performed according to the procedure of Wells and colleagues (12). Briefly, the microdissected CA1 regions were homogenized in ice-cold Ca2+, Mg2+-free buffer (50 mM HEPES, 100 mM NaCl, and 3 mM KAc, pH 7.4) with RNase inhibitor (15 U/mL), and the homogenate was centrifuged at 2000× g for 1 min. Supernatants were passed through two 100-μm nylon mesh filters, followed by a 5-μm pore filter. The filtrate was centrifuged at 1000× g for 10 min and then gently resuspended in same buffer at a protein concentration of 2 mg/mL.

Western blot.

For each experimental group, homogenates from at least three slices were pooled. The microdissected subregions were lysed in ice-cold Tris-HCl buffer solution (pH 7.4) containing a cocktail of protein phosphatase and proteinase inhibitors (50 mM Tris-HCl, 100 mM NaCl, 15 mM sodium pyrophosphate, 50 mM sodium fluoride, 1 mM sodium orthovanadate, 5 mM EGTA, 5 mM EDTA, 1 mM phenylmethylsulfonyl fluoride, 1 μM microcystin-LR, 1 μM okadaic acid, 0.5% Triton X-100, 2 mM benzamidine, 60 μg/mL aprotinin, and 60 μg/mL leupeptin) to avoid dephosphorylation and degradation of proteins and ground with a pellet pestle (Kontes Glassware, Vineland, NJ). Samples were sonicated and spun down at 15,000× g at 4°C for 10 min. The supernatant was then assayed for total protein concentration using Bio-Rad Bradford Protein Assay Kit (Hercules, CA). Each sample of tissue homogenate or each PSD fraction was separated in 10% sodium dodecyl sulfate–polyacrylamide gel electrophoresis gel. After the transfer on nitrocellulose membranes, blots were blocked in buffer solution containing 5% milk and 0.1% Tween-20 in phosphate-buffered saline (124 mM NaCl, 4 mM KCl, 10 mM Na2HPO4, and 10 mM KH2PO4, pH 7.2) for 1 h and then blotted for 2 h at room temperature with the monoclonal antibody that recognizes phosphorylated αCaMKII at threonine-286 site (1:2000; Affinity Bioreagents, Golden, CO) or protein phosphatase 1 (PP1; 1:1000; Upstate Biotechnology). It was then probed with horseradish peroxidase–conjugated secondary antibody for 1 h and developed using the electrochemiluminescent immunoblotting detection system (Amersham Biosciences, Buckinghamshire, UK), according to manufacturer's instructions. The immunoblots using phosphorylation site–specific antibodies were subsequently stripped and reprobed with an antibody that recognizes αCaMKII (1:1000; Affinity Bioreagents). Immunoblots were analyzed by densitometry using Bio-profil BioLight PC software.

Data analysis.

All data are expressed as means ± SEM. We used a two-way repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) to compare LTP and LTD among groups with treatment as the intersubject variable and the time course (time point after HFS or LFS) as the intrasubject variable. Fisher's protected LSD post hoc test was used for pairwise comparisons after ANOVA. Western blot data were analyzed using an unpaired t test. Because passive avoidance performance on training and retention did not assume a normal distribution, these data were analyzed using one-way repeated-measures ANOVA, followed by a nonparametric Mann-Whitney U test. Number of animals used is indicated by n. Probability values of p < 0.05 were considered statistically significant. All data analyses were performed using SPSS commercially available statistical analysis software (SigmaStat Version 2.0; Chicago, IL).

RESULTS

Effects of neonatal DEX and H treatment on the induction of LTP and LTD.

To determine the effects of neonatal DEX and HC on long-term synaptic plasticity, we analyzed LTP and LTD in the CA1 region of the hippocampus. In slices from SAL-treated rats, conditioning HFS induced a robust LTP (50 min after HFS: 155 ± 11%, n = 6) (Fig. 1 A). There was a striking difference in the effect of DEX or HC treatment on LTP induction. Neonatal DEX, but not HC, treatment impaired LTP induction (DEX, 117.8 ± 6.3%, n = 6; HC, 149.6 ± 7.8%, n = 6). During the 60-min recording time after HFS, a two-way repeated-measures ANOVA of fEPSP slope showed a significant interaction among groups and time course (F2,15 = 7.158, p < 0.01). LTD was induced by applying LFS at 1 Hz for 15 min. As shown in Fig. 1B, slices from DEX-treated but not SAL- or HC-treated rats showed a reliable LTD (50 min after the end of LFS: SAL, 96.3 ± 4.2%, n = 6; DEX, 63.6 ± 5.8%, n = 6; HC, 94.7 ± 4.9%, n = 6). There was a significant interaction among groups and time course on fEPSP slope (F2,15 = 39.87, p < 0.001).

Figure 1
figure 1

Neonatal DEX, but not HC, treatment impairs LTP induction but facilitates LTD induction in the hippocampal CA1 region. (A) Time course of the slope of the fEPSP before and after HFS (arrow) for hippocampal slices obtained from neonatal SAL- (), DEX- (•), and HC- () treated rats. (B) Time course of the slope of the fEPSPs before and after LFS for hippocampal slices obtained from neonatal SAL- (), DEX- (•), and HC- () treated rats. (C) Previous administration of RU38486 (▪), but not RU28318 (□), completely prevented the block of LTP induction by DEX treatment. (D) LFS failed to induce LTD in slices from DEX-treated rats, which administrated RU38486 (▪). Administration of RU28318 did not significantly alter the induction of LTD by LFS in slices from DEX-treated rats (□). (E) Time course of the slope of the fEPSP before and after HFS for hippocampal slices obtained from neonatal RU28318- () or RU38486- (♦) treated rats. (F) Time course of the slope of the fEPSP before and after LFS for hippocampal slices obtained from neonatal RU28318- () or RU38486- (♦) treated rats. The superimposed fEPSPs in the upper portion illustrate respective recordings from example experiments taken at the time indicated by number. Horizontal bar denotes the period of delivery of LFS. Dash lines show level of baseline.

Consistent with a key mechanistic role for glucocorticoid receptor activation in mediating the effects of DEX on hippocampal plasticity, the specific glucocorticoid receptor antagonist RU38486 (40 mg/kg, i.p. injection 30 min before DEX), completely prevented the block of LTP (154.3 ± 8.5%, n = 6) and facilitation of LTD induction (93.5 ± 5.8%, n = 6) by DEX treatment (Fig. 1C, D). Conversely, previous administration of the specific mineralocorticoid receptor antagonist RU28318 (10 mg/kg) did not significantly affect the effects of DEX on LTP (121.2 ± 6.3%, n = 4) and LTD (66.4 ± 5.7% of baseline; n = 4). However, neither neonatal RU28318 nor RU38486 treatment alone affected the induction of LTP (RU28318, 151.5 ± 9.3%, n = 4; RU38486, 154.5 ± 9.1%, n = 4; p > 0.05 when compared with slices from saline treatment rats) or LTD (RU28318, 96.1 ± 5.3%, n = 4; RU38486, 94.5 ± 6.3%, n = 4; p > 0.05 when compared with slices from saline treatment rats) (Fig. 1E, F).

In previous studies (8), we established that the effects of neonatal DEX treatment on LTP and LTD induction are correlated with an increase in the autophosphorylation of αCaMKII at threonine-286 in hippocampal PSD fraction and a decrease in the PP1 expression and activity in hippocampal CA1 homogenate. Thus, we also compared the long-term effects of neonatal DEX and HC treatment on the basal levels of phosphorylated αCaMKII at threonine-286 and PP1 protein expression. As previously shown (8), neonatal DEX treatment led to an increase in the αCaMKII autophosphorylation and a decrease in PP1 when compared with SAL treatment group (Fig. 2A, B). No significant effects of HC treatment on the levels of αCaMKII autophosphorylation and PP1 proteins were seen. Inhibition of glucocorticoid receptor with RU38486 also specifically prevented the effects of DEX on the αCaMKII autophosphorylation and PP1 protein (Fig. 2C, E). In contrast, prior administration of RU28318 did not significantly affect the effects of DEX. Neither neonatal RU28318 nor RU38486 treatment alone caused a significant change in the levels of αCaMKII autophosphorylation or PP1 protein (Fig. 2D, F).

Figure 2
figure 2

Neonatal DEX, but not HC, treatment leads to an increase in the levels of αCaMKII autophosphorylation but a decrease in PP1 protein expression. (A) Representative blots showing that higher phosphorylation of αCaMKII at threonine-286 in PSD from DEX-treated rats as compared with SAL- and HC-treated rats. Group data showing the normalization of phosphorylation of αCaMKII to the nonphosphorylated form was determined in each group. (B) Representative blots showing the levels of PP1 in the hippocampal CA1 homogenates from neonatal SAL-, DEX-, and HC-treated rats. Group data showing the normalization of PP1 to β-actin was determined in each group. (C) RU38486 pretreatment prevented the up-regulation of αCaMKII autophosphorylation by DEX, while RU28318 had no effect. (D) Representative blots and group data showing the level of αCaMKII autophosphorylation in PSD from neonatal SAL-, RU28318-, and RU38486-treated rats. (E) RU38486 pretreatment specifically blocked the reduction of PP1 protein levels by DEX. (F) Representative blots and group data showing the level of PP1 protein in the hippocampal CA1 homogenates from neonatal SAL-, RU28318-, and RU38486-treated rats. *p < 0.05, compared with SAL-treated group, unpaired t test.

Effects of neonatal DEX and HC treatment on one-trial passive avoidance learning.

As a final test, we compared the long-term effects of neonatal DEX and HC treatment on memory retention of one-trial passive avoidance learning task (a form of contextual fear conditioning). No significant between-group differences in performance were noted during the training, but on the day of testing, performance of DEX-treated rats was poorer [i.e. they entered the darkened chamber in which they were previously shocked more rapidly (23.5 ± 5.2 s; n = 8) than SAL- (64.8 ± 7.5 s; n = 8) and HC- (57.6 ± 6.7 s; n = 8) treated rats] (Fig. 3A). The effect of DEX on memory retention performance was completely prevented by the administration of RU38486 (61.3 ± 7.2 s; n = 6), but not by RU28318 (24.9 ± 4.9 s; n = 6) (Fig. 3B). Neither neonatal RU28318 (67.5 ± 7.5 s; n = 5) nor RU38486 (56.3 ± 9.2 s; n = 5) treatment alone significantly affected the memory retention performance (Fig. 3C).

Figure 3
figure 3

Neonatal DEX, but not HC, treatment results in an impaired memory retention for one-trial passive avoidance task at 5 wk of age. (A) Vertical bar plots representing the latencies to enter the dark component on retention testing in rats neonatally received SAL (open column), DEX (coarse oblique line column), or HC (filled column) treatment. (B) Vertical bar plots representing the latencies to enter the dark component on retention testing in rats that neonatally received, SAL (open column), RU28318 + DEX (coarse oblique line column) or RU38486 + DEX (filled column). (C) Vertical bar plots representing the latencies to enter the dark component on retention testing in rats that neonatally received SAL (open column), RU28318 (coarse oblique line column) or RU38486 (filled line column). *p < 0.05, in comparison with SAL-treated group, in a nonparametric Mann-Whitney U test after one-way ANOVA.

DISCUSSION

Long-term neurodevelopmental outcomes after neonatal corticosteroid treatment have become a determinant factor against the use of this therapy in preterm infants. Although evidence is now increasing that treatment of preterm infants with DEX is associated with an increased risk of impaired neuromotor skills and cognitive function in later life (46), it is uncertain whether these abnormalities are linked to the use of corticosteroids in general or just to DEX only. In the present study, besides the confirmation of previous work demonstrating that neonatal DEX treatment alters hippocampal synaptic function in later life (7,8), we show the novel observations that neonatal HC treatment using a regimen similar to that used in preterm infants has no long-term effects on either hippocampal synaptic plasticity or contextual fear memory formation. These findings are in agreement with the neurodevelopmental outcome data of recently published comparison studies of postnatal DEX and HC treatment in preterm infants (911) with favorable outcomes for HC treatment.

A key question is why neonatal treatment with HC can result in quite different neurodevelopmental outcomes compared with DEX. As DEX has a high glucocorticoid-mediated activity (13), low mineralocorticoid-mediated activity and long biologic half-life compared with HC, one potential possibility is that these discrepant outcomes arise from the differential activation of glucocorticoid receptors by these treatments. The present study confirmed the role of glucocorticoid receptor activation in mediating the long-term effects of neonatal DEX treatment by demonstrating that DEX-induced alterations of hippocampal synaptic plasticity and contextual fear memory formation were specifically prevented by the glucocorticoid receptor antagonist RU38486.

In line with previous findings (7,8), we found that, compared with SAL- and HC-treated rats, DEX-treated rats exhibited an increased basal αCaMKII autophosphorylation at threonine-286 in hippocampal PSD fraction. It has been shown that threonine-286 phosphorylation is sufficient to convert the αCaMKII to the high affinity form (14). Thus, it is possible that the greater level of autophosphorylated αCaMKII binds a greater amount of Ca2+/calmodulin, which in turn leads to systematic shift in favor of LTD at all submaximal levels of Ca2+ influx triggered by LFS. In fact, it has been shown that transgenic mice overexpressing a mutated form of αCaMKII with autonomous activity display a downward shift of the frequency response curve, favoring LTD over LTP induction in the hippocampal CA1 region (15). Because the increase in αCaMKII autophosphorylation observed in DEX-treated rats is correlated with a reduction of PP1 level, it is likely that neonatal DEX treatment may provide a unidentified mechanism for decreased PP1 production, which renders the αCaMKII persistently hyperphosphorylated (8).

In conclusion, our works supports the notion that neonatal HC treatment has no long-term adverse effects on hippocampal function compared with DEX treatment. These findings are of clinical importance because it is now difficult to avoid the use of corticosteroids in neonatology and perinatology to fight the problems of CLD. Although HC may instead of DEX for the prevention or treatment of CLD, further prospective, longitudinal follow-up studies are necessary to evaluate this therapeutic approach.