Main

The hormone prolactin has typically been associated with processes of lactation. However, prolactin has also been shown to have a broad range of functions (13). An area of particular attention has been prolactin as a stress-response hormone. Studies in biologic psychiatry have documented a rise in serum prolactin levels after physical and psychological stress (48).

Serum prolactin has been considered a peripheral indicator of central dopaminergic function, because the neurotransmitter dopamine provides the primary tonic inhibitory influence on prolactin release from the anterior pituitary (9,10). Increased dopamine release inhibits prolactin secretion, as can reduced dopamine reuptake through decreased dopamine transporter (DAT) number or function. Among the family of dopamine receptors, D2 is the primary one associated with the inhibition of prolactin release (9). A complex system has been demonstrated to regulate prolactin and includes other neurotransmitter systems (e.g., serotonin) (10,11).

We became interested in prolactin in the course of research on iron deficiency, a common nutritional problem that may affect as many as half of the world's infants (12). Of the neurotransmitter systems shown to be altered by iron deficiency in animal studies, dopamine has been studied the most. There is substantial evidence that several aspects of dopaminergic function are altered by iron deficiency (13,14). Early studies showed that iron deficient rats had reduced D2 receptor number and function in the striatum and a behavioral profile similar to that observed with the use of dopamine receptor blocking agents (15). As predicted by these changes, serum prolactin levels and liver prolactin-binding sites were increased in iron deficient rats (16,17). Recent animal studies have suggested more complex dopamine system alterations, including dopamine transporter down-regulation and increased extracellular dopamine levels in the striatum (13).

We previously explored the question of dopaminergic alterations in human iron deficiency by assessing serum prolactin levels in 12- to 23-mo-old infants who participated in a study of the effects of iron deficiency anemia on behavior and development (7,18). Serum was available from the blood obtained for iron status measures before and after three months of iron treatment. Developmental testing preceded venipuncture at both time points. Therefore, serum prolactin levels may have reflected the infant's response to both events. We did not find a significant relationship between infant iron status and serum prolactin levels in that study. However, we found that a high serum prolactin level was associated with the behavioral profile of infants with iron deficiency anemia – wary and hesitant behavior during developmental testing. In addition, serum prolactin levels (high or normal) were surprisingly stable over the two assessments.

In the present study, we obtained serum prolactin levels in these same children in early adolescence. While the heightened serum prolactin levels in infancy were related to the wary and hesitant behavioral profile typical of infants with chronic iron deficiency and not iron status per se (5), we believed that a relationship between iron status and altered serum prolactin remained possible. One reason was that affective differences such as wary and hesitant behavior have been observed in every human study of iron deficiency that included such measures in infancy (14). In addition, animal studies have consistently documented dopaminergic alterations with iron deficiency anemia and have documented hesitant behavior after early iron deficiency as well (13,14,19). Based on the association of elevated serum prolactin with the behavioral profile typical of iron deficiency in infancy and the information available from animal studies, we predicted that having chronic iron deficiency in infancy would be associated with elevated serum prolactin levels after a stressor in early adolescence.

METHODS AND MATERIALS

Subjects.

The original cohort consisted of 191 healthy, full-term infants who were enrolled at 12–23 mo of age (between 1983 and 1985) for a study of the behavioral and developmental effects of iron deficiency (18). In infancy, the children resided in a peri-urban, lower-middle-class community in Costa Rica. Infant iron status ranged from moderate iron deficiency anemia to iron sufficiency. Moderate anemia was defined as Hb ≤ 100 g/L and mild anemia was defined as Hb 101–105 g/L. Iron deficiency was defined as serum ferritin ≤ 12 μg/L, and either erythrocyte protoporphyrin > 100 μg/dL packed red blood cells or transferrin saturation ≤ 10%. Iron sufficiency was defined as Hb ≥ 120 g/L and no abnormal iron measures. Infants with Hb < 120 g/L and any degree of iron deficiency (defined above) were given either IM iron (calculated to increase Hb to 125 g/L), or two daily oral doses of ferrous sulfate (3 mg/kg, one dose by project personnel) for a period of three months. Infants who had Hb ≥ 120 g/L and were either iron deficient or iron depleted (serum ferritin < 12 μg/L being the only abnormality) also received the oral iron treatment.

All infants with moderate iron deficiency anemia corrected their anemia with 3 mo of iron treatment. However, 64% of the infants with moderate IDA still had altered iron status measures after iron treatment (e.g., erythrocyte protoporphyrin > 100 μg/dL packed red blood cells or transferrin saturation ≤ 10%). In addition, 17 infants with hemoglobins >100 g/L also had altered iron status measures after iron treatment. Previous analyses have shown that the severity of iron deficiency and response to iron treatment combined are related to CNS outcomes. The entire group of subjects who had moderate iron deficiency anemia in infancy and those who had higher Hb levels but persisting abnormalities of iron status measures after iron treatment were similar in having poorer long-term behavioral and developmental outcomes (20,21). Both groups of infants had evidence of chronic iron deficiency. Infants with moderate IDA had chronic, severe iron deficiency since anemia is a late manifestation of iron deficiency. The infants who had persisting alterations of iron measures had lower hemoglobins and higher erythrocyte protoporphyrin levels at study entry than those infants whose iron measures completely responded to iron treatment. It is important to note that unmodified cow milk was typically introduced in the first few months of life in this population and that subjects were identified as ID at 12–23 mo in the original study. Thus, the iron deficiency was likely to have been chronic. Previous studies have compared the children with evidence of chronic ID in infancy to children who had good iron status in infancy. The good iron status comparison group consisted of infants who were iron sufficient (as described above) at study entry and/or after iron treatment (other than those with moderate IDA) (20,21). The present study uses the same grouping approach.

Of the original infant cohort, 167 (87%) participated in a re-evaluation of behavioral and developmental status in early adolescence (11 to 14 y of age) between 1994 and 1995 (21). Serum samples for prolactin were available for 159 of these children. The average age at follow up was 12.3 ± 0.7 y (range 10.9–13.7 y). There were 70 females and 89 males. Iron status during infancy did not differ significantly for children who participated in this follow-up and those who did not. The project pediatrician obtained signed informed consent from parents and assent from the child. The study protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of the University of Michigan, the Hospital Nacional de Ninos, Costa Rica, and the Office of Protection from Research Risks, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD.

Procedures.

Sequential blood samples for prolactin levels were collected in conjunction with a venipuncture to determine iron status in early adolescence. To facilitate repeated blood sampling, an angio-catheter was placed at venipuncture by the project pediatrician who was blind to infant iron status group. Venipuncture and catheter placement occurred between 7:30 and 8:00 h to minimize variation of serum iron and prolactin levels related to normal diurnal patterns (22,23). The timing of the subsequent samples was guided by the physiology of serum prolactin response to stress. Previous studies in adults have documented an increase in serum prolactin levels within 5–10 min of a stressful event, a peak between 20–30 min, then a decline by 45–60 min (24). We collected the first sample at venipuncture for catheter placement (the stressor), and sequential samples were planned at 15 min, 30 min, and 45 min thereafter. Between the initial and second samples, all children had a complete physical examination that included an assessment of pubertal stage by standard protocol (25). After the second sample, the children had a break during which they rested, read and had a snack. The children had no other activities between the third and fourth samples. All blood samples for prolactin measurement were immediately centrifuged and the sera separated. Sera were stored at –20°C until assay in duplicate for prolactin concentration (mIU/L) using standard double-antibody RIA kits from ICN Pharmaceuticals, Costa Mesa, California. The assays were completed within 3–4 y of collection. Inter-assay variability was 15% and the intra-assay variability was 13%.

Our overall measure of pubertal development was pubic hair Tanner stage (1–5). This measure correlated highly with all other ratings of pubertal development (axillary hair, breast or genital development, and menstruation status (all values r > 0.70)), and thus, could be applied to both boys and girls. Children were categorized as follows: Tanner stage 1—less mature; Tanner stage 2 and 3—intermediate mature; and Tanner stage 4 and 5—more mature.

Statistical analysis.

The children in the chronic ID group (n = 46) were compared with the children who had had good iron status in infancy (the comparison group, n = 113) for age, sex, pubertal development, time of serum prolactin samples, and serum prolactin concentrations using t-test for continuous and normally distributed variables, the Mantel-Haenzael χ2 test for ordered categorical variables, and the Fisher's exact test for dichotomous variables. Serum prolactin levels at each time point were then compared by infant iron status controlling for propensity score, pubertal status and first serum sample prolactin concentration using linear mixed models. We used a propensity score to reduce bias due to group differences in background characteristics and other factors. Propensity scores have been increasingly used in epidemiologic and medical research as an index of the risk (propensity) for the condition of interest (26). The propensity score, consisting of relevant risk factors, is used as a control variable in analyses of outcome. In this study, the condition of interest was iron status in infancy (chronic ID versus the comparison, good iron status group). Based on our previous work, factors that could influence the probability of iron status group were included in the propensity score using multivariate logistic regression. The factors included sex, birth weight, infant feeding history, age at initial enrollment, mother's IQ, socioeconomic status, family structure, and quality of the home environment. The area under the receiver operating characteristic curve (c-statistic) for the propensity score model in this study was 0.84, indicating excellent discrimination between iron status groups. The propensity score was divided into quintiles and incorporated as a stratified control variable. A linear mixed effects model growth curve analysis was used to account for correlations among observations due to sequential measurements taken on the same individual. The initial level of prolactin after a stressor affects the range of the response curve thereafter (e.g., lower initial levels constrain the amount of possible decline) (27). Therefore, this was controlled in this analysis. We assumed a spatial autocorrelation structure for the covariance matrix where the strength of the correlation between observations was a function of the time interval between measurements. Separate covariance structures were estimated for the chronic ID and good iron status groups due to unequal variances. All statistical tests were two-tailed with alpha levels set at 0.05. Statistical analyses were conducted using SAS Version 9.1 (28).

RESULTS

In early adolescence, no child was anemic and only 2% (n = 3) had biochemical evidence of iron deficiency (21). The characteristics of the children who had chronic ID in infancy and those who had good iron status before and/or after iron therapy (comparison group) are described in Table 1. There were significantly more males in the chronic ID group and, at the time of assessment, these adolescents were less mature than the comparison group. The actual times of the serum prolactin samples were recorded and found to vary somewhat by individual. However, there was no significant difference for the mean sample times when compared by iron-status group (Table 1).

Table 1 Subject characteristics and sample collection times by infant iron status

Serum prolactin levels were compared by sex and pubertal status. In this early adolescent period, males had significantly lower serum prolactin levels than females at each sample time (Table 2). In addition, the less mature adolescents had significantly lower serum prolactin levels at each sample time than those who were more mature (Table 3).

Table 2 Serum prolactin concentrations at each sample time by sex
Table 3 Serum prolactin concentrations by pubertal status

Comparing serum prolactin concentrations at each time point by infant iron status, the chronic ID group had significantly lower levels at 15 min after venipuncture (unadjusted values, Table 4). After controlling for propensity score, pubertal status and the first prolactin sample concentration in the linear effects model, group differences were not significant (adjusted values, Table 4).

Table 4 Unadjusted and adjusted serum prolactin levels by infant iron status group

The change in serum prolactin between samples 2 (15 min) and 3 (30 min), and between samples 3 and 4 (50 min) were compared within and between infant iron status groups using the linear effects model (Table 5). The pattern of serum prolactin response differed for chronic ID versus the good iron status comparison group. The adolescents that had chronic ID in infancy had significantly less decline between 15 and 30 min than the good iron status comparison group. The iron status groups did not differ for amount of serum prolactin change between 30 to 50 min after venipuncture.

Table 5 Change in serum prolactin concentrations by infant iron status group

DISCUSSION

This follow-up of early adolescents who had participated in a study of iron deficiency in infancy provided a unique opportunity to assess long-term alterations in the response of serum prolactin to stress in relation to infant iron status. Based on the association between high serum prolactin levels and the behavioral profile of iron deficiency in infancy and the effects of iron deficiency on the dopamine system in animal studies, we had hypothesized that children who had chronic ID in infancy would show elevated serum prolactin levels 10 y later. The adolescents who had chronic ID in infancy did not show the predicted pattern. Instead, they demonstrated a more prompt decline of serum prolactin shortly after venipuncture, compared with those who had good iron status in infancy.

Dopamine, a central factor in the production, release and reuptake of prolactin, is the primary direct tonic inhibitor of prolactin secretion (9,10). Of three major hypothalamic neuronal populations, tuberoinfundibular dopminergic neurons (TIDA) are considered the primary source of dopamine for inhibiting prolactin secretion (10). Iron deficiency early in life in rats has been shown to alter striatal dopamine regulation, (13,14) but the TIDA system has not been directly studied in the context of iron deficiency. The hypothalamic-TIDA dopamine system is thought to differ from dopaminergic systems in other brain areas and there are species-specific differences (10). However, increased serum prolactin levels and liver prolactin-binding sites have been observed in iron deficient rats (16,17).

The potential biologic mechanisms that underlie our findings of altered prolactin regulation years after a period of chronic iron deficiency are unknown and must be considered cautiously. If early life iron deficiency affected the TIDA dopamine system, one might expect decreased dopaminergic inhibition and higher prolactin levels while iron deficient. Although we did not demonstrate this in infancy, (7) recent findings that infants of anemic mothers with or without malnutrition had elevated cord blood prolactin levels provides support for this mechanism early in life (29). In the present study, we observed an altered serum prolactin pattern in response to stressor more than 10 y after ID in infancy. The pattern is consistent with a swifter response of dopaminergic inhibition. Decreased DAT number or function, as recently observed in the striatal system of iron-deficient rats, could allow extracellular dopamine to accumulate and inhibit prolactin release (14). In addition, chronically high prolactin levels in infancy might lead to fundamental alterations that increased the number of TIDA neurons, with a corresponding increase in prolactin inhibition over time (30). Alterations in other aspects of prolactin regulation are also possible, (e.g., serotonin) (31).

Recent research indicates that early life experiences can result in fundamental neurobiologic changes. The development of CNS regulatory mechanisms for prolactin has been demonstrated to be dynamic, (3234) and in one rodent study, early postnatal enrichment of maternal care (handling) was associated with lowered serum prolactin response to fearful situations later in life (35). Studies in humans with mental health conditions, such as bipolar disorder and bullemia nervosa, have noted blunted prolactin responses after pharmacologic challenge and serum prolactin responses correlated with the degree of early life trauma. In these conditions, altered serotonin biology was proposed as the indirect mechanism leading to altered dopamine and serum prolactin levels (31,36). Investigations of other neurohormonal systems are also relevant. Rodent studies of the developing hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis and the pattern of corticosterone response to stress have shown that the timing and duration of early stressful experiences affects the direction of the stress-response system long-term, resulting in exaggerated or blunted corticosterone response patterns (37,38).

There were limitations in evaluating serum prolactin responses in the children in this study. In infancy, serum prolactin levels did not differ significantly by sex or age (7). However, as these children entered adolescence, sex and pubertal stage became important factors as observed by others (5,39). We statistically controlled for propensity score (which included sex) and pubertal status, but a stronger design would be to assess children with more equal distribution by group for sex and pubertal status. Another important limitation in this study was that the venipuncture (the stressor of interest) and the first serum sample occurred simultaneously. Both groups showed declining prolactin concentrations thereafter indicating that the peak response in these young adolescents may have occurred in anticipation of, and during the venipuncture, in contrast to the initial rise post-venipuncture observed in adults. It is possible that the chronic ID group subjects had more anticipatory distress that resulted in a peak serum prolactin response even earlier than the good iron status comparison group. An earlier peak before catheter placement could explain the differences in decline of serum prolactin over time. In addition, it is important to recall that more of the chronic ID group adolescents had been rated as wary and hesitant during developmental testing in infancy. This raises the possibility of enduring behavioral differences that might underlie the difference in serum prolactin response. Future studies could explore this further by a repeated sampling approach with a stress after reaching baseline prolactin levels and behavioral measures. Finally, other factors such as dietary tryptophan intake were not monitored in these children and might have affected serum prolactin levels (40).

In summary, serum prolactin levels have not previously been assessed years after a period of iron deficiency in infancy. Our results show an altered serum prolactin response pattern in adolescents who had chronic ID in infancy. We suggest this finding reflects fundamental alterations in the regulation of serum prolactin due to effects of early iron deficiency on the dopamine system.