Main

Thyroid hormone (TH) is essential for normal brain development (1). A lack of TH during fetal or early postnatal life is associated with specific brain damage. This includes abnormal neuronal proliferation and migration (2,3), decreased dendritic densities and synaptic profiles (4), impaired synaptic transmission (5), and reduced myelination (6). TH acts by regulating specific brain genes (7) through the formation of a nuclear receptor complex (8), which serves to up- or down-regulate specific genes (9) via a variety of signaling mechanisms (10,11). Because TH-regulated gene activity varies spatially and temporally in the brain (12), different types of deficits will follow from a loss of TH at different stages of development (13,14).

In the retina, TH is needed (1517) for the differentiation of cones versus rods (18,19), specific cone subtypes (20), and retinal oligodendrocyte precursor cells (21) and for the production of essential proteins (22,23). Animal models demonstrate that when TH is lacking at a particular time prenatally, visual development and functioning will be impaired (20). Although comparable evidence is not available on humans, visual processing deficits in children with prenatal or neonatal TH insufficiencies (24,25) suggest that the human visual system may also be vulnerable to an early lack of TH insufficiency.

Even though negative feedback regulation of the fetal thyroid by the hypothalamus and pituitary does not commence until midgestation (26), the presence of TH receptors in human fetal brain tissue in the first trimester (27) suggests that TH is probably needed well before the fetal thyroid is functional. TH of maternal origin is evident in coelomic fluid from the start of pregnancy (28), and by the end of pregnancy, maternal TH may still account for as much as 30% of the fetal TH supply (29). Maternal hypothyroidism, congenital hypothyroidism (CH), and preterm birth, which represent conditions involving an insufficient TH supply, allow one to assess the impact of insufficient TH on the developing visual system. In maternal hypothyroidism, the effects are most salient in early pregnancy, when the fetus's own TH supply is not yet available (30). Correlations between maternal TH levels in early pregnancy and the child's subsequent outcome signify that adequate levels of maternal TH are necessary for normal offspring neurodevelopment (31,32). Because children with CH do not produce enough TH on their own and the mother's thyroid cannot fulfill all of their third-trimester TH needs, they may experience TH insufficiency in the latter part of gestation (33). Also, because these children lack TH postnatally until replacement hormone takes effect (34), they may manifest further cognitive deficits (35). Children who are born preterm, who have a severed maternal thyroid supply before their own glands are fully competent (36), may exhibit low TH levels at birth, and these remain low or even decline ex utero (37,38). Low TH levels in preterm infants have also been associated with later suboptimal neurodevelopment (39,40).

In this article, we describe how a lack of TH during pregnancy or early life affects children's contrast sensitivity and visual acuity development. These core visual abilities were chosen because of their different developmental trajectories (41), and they are differentially compromised in certain pediatric conditions (4244). We hypothesized that because maternal hypothyroidism, CH, and preterm birth each involve a different time period of TH insufficiency, they may have a different impact on contrast sensitivity and visual acuity development.

METHODS

Three groups of infants with TH insufficiency and typically developing control subjects were tested at 3, 4.5, and 6 mo (corrected) age at The Hospital for Sick Children (HSC; Toronto, Ontario, Canada). Testers were masked to infant group status. Prior to testing, informal consent was obtained from all parents who received a full debriefing of procedures in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The Research Ethics Board at HSC formally approved all procedures.

Participants.

From an original sample of 102 infants tested once minimally, 74 infants who participated in all three sessions were considered for this study. All of their mothers had a normal pregnancy, delivery, and, except for the preterm group, a full-term pregnancy. All children were free of neonatal or birth complications or neonatal disease (except for hypothyroidism in the CH group). From a total of 74 infants who fit these criteria, an additional 13 were eliminated because mothers reported taking medications for other illnesses during pregnancy (e.g. fluvoxamine for depression; n = 6), children took medications for illnesses in infancy (n = 4), and technical difficulties (e.g. software malfunction) or children were fussy and noncompliant (n = 3). The final sample size of 61 infants included 13 offspring of hypothyroid women (HYPO), 12 with CH, 16 who were born preterm (PREM), and 20 control subjects.

The HYPO group was recruited from data sheets for the period from April 1999 to October 2000 of the Motherisk Clinic, which provides antenatal counseling on the risks of maternal illnesses, medications, and substances of abuse to the fetus. All mothers had called this clinic to inquire about hypothyroidism predating (n = 10) or acquired during (n = 3) pregnancy. Although all were treated with l-thyroxine, their dosages were generally insufficient because all had raised TSH levels in the first and/or second trimester (45), and a few also had elevated TSH levels throughout pregnancy. The CH group consisted of infants who were identified through the Ontario newborn screening program between January 2000 and December 2001 on the basis of a TSH value >20 mIU/L (mean = 150.5 mIU/L; range = 20–400) at 2–3 d of life (46). Except for two children who were treated by a local pediatric endocrinologist, all were followed in the Endocrine Clinic at HSC and received an initial replacement therapy dose of ≈10 μg/kg l-thyroxine at a median age of 9 d (range = 6–25 d). Technetium scanning identified five with athyreosis, five with an ectopic gland, and two with dyshormonogenesis. The PREM group was born between September 2000 and May 2001 at 32–35 wk gestation in the level II nursery of Sunnybrook and Women's College Health Sciences Centre in Toronto. All were screened to be free of any gross neonatal complications (e.g. prematurity, bronchopulmonary dysplasia, intraventricular hemorrhage). Control subjects had normal TSH levels at birth and were born to women with no signs of hypothyroidism during pregnancy. For eight control infants, mothers were recruited from a local obstetrician, who measured their TSH levels at 12 wk of pregnancy (range = 1.15–2.14 mIU/L). The remaining 12 control infants were born between May and December 2000 at Sunnybrook and Women's College Health Sciences Centre and were recruited from the level I neonatal nursery monthly birth lists, which had been previously screened by a neonatologist (E.A.) to be free of neonatal or perinatal complications. Of the families listed, ≈25% who lived in the central Toronto area were invited to participate, and of these, ≈20% agreed to participate in the study.

Sweep visual evoked potentials.

Visual evoked potentials (VEPs) (47) were measured using the International 10–20 system (48). Active electrodes were placed over the infant's scalp at O1, OZ, and O2, and at CZ (reference) and Pz (ground). The analogue electroencephalograph signal was amplified using a Grass Model 12 Data Acquisition System. Analogue to digital signal conversion, data acquisition, and stimulus presentation all were controlled using the PowerDiva (49) software system. The sweep VEP technique estimated contrast sensitivity and grating acuity by tracking the amplitude of the steady-state evoked response to a black-and-white sine wave (striped) grating, which increased in contrast or spatial frequency. Thresholds were determined by performing a linear regression over a range of increasing or decreasing response values to zero amplitude.

During testing, each infant sat on his or her parent's lap 70 cm from a 17” (43 cm) monochrome video monitor. Black and white gratings changed in contrast or spatial frequency over the course of a 10-s interval. Gratings were phase-reversed 12 times per second (modulation frequency = 6 Hz) and subtended a visual angle of 21° by 21°. Mean luminance was 105 cd/m2. A small toy, which was dangled ≈2 cm from the screen, served to direct the infant's attention to the monitor.

For each infant, three to five thresholds were obtained per session. Sweep ranges were age appropriate (50), and order of conditions was randomized. To be scorable, all evoked responses had to meet the following criteria: 1) average amplitude of the response frequency exceeded noise by a factor of three (51); 2) phase of response was constant or synchronized with stimulus onset, or phase could lag progressively as spatial frequency increased or as contrast decreased (52); and 3) the peak amplitude of the signal was significantly above zero at the p < 0.05 level using the circular T2 statistic (52), which tests the consistency of the amplitude and phase of the averaged responses.

For deriving the dependent variables, thresholds were fitted with a negative exponential function (41) y = ce−ax, where y equals contrast sensitivity and x equals spatial frequency (Fig. 1). Four variables were obtained per session: 1) peak sensitivity (c) or the highest log contrast sensitivity; 2) the high spatial frequency roll-off (a) or rate at which contrast sensitivity is reduced as spatial frequency increases; 3) coefficient of determination (COD) or the fit of the negative exponential model; and 4) grating acuity (acuity limit of the contrast sensitivity function) obtained in the 80% spatial frequency condition. Because the COD distribution was heavily skewed, this variable was transformed (53) whereby y = 2arcsine(√p), where p was the infant's COD score.

Figure 1
figure 1

Sample contrast sensitivity function.

Eye examination.

Eye examinations involved undilated retinoscopy using the Mohindra technique, an approach that shows good correlation to dilated measurement (54). Mean sphere equivalent and cylindrical error were measured at one of the infant's three visits. Fundus ophthalmoscopy showed no abnormality in any infants.

Data analyses.

VEP data were analyzed using a mixed-model repeated-measures ANOVA with groups as the between-subjects variable and age as the within-subjects variable, with repeated-measures analyses using the Greenhouse and Geisser adjustment. Significant group main effects were analyzed using pairwise F tests with controls. T tests served to compare subgroups of infants.

RESULTS

Demographic information.

Groups did not differ in sex distribution (Table 1) or maternal or socioeconomic status (data not shown). Except for CH, no differences in newborn TSH levels were seen. For the subgroup of HYPO mothers with TSH information, significantly elevated levels (p < 0.01) compared with control subjects were observed.

Table 1 Demographic characteristics of clinical and control groups (SDs)

Between-group analyses.

Mean log peak contrast sensitivity scores ranged from 1.75 to 2.2 log units. ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of Group (F = 3.62, df = 3,57, p = 0.018), which reflected the reduced peak contrast sensitivities for HYPO and CH relative to control subjects (F = 9.36, df = 1,32, p = 0.003; and F = 4.08, df = 1,31, p = 0.04, respectively). For HYPO, the difference was ≈0.20 log units and for CH was 0.15 log units. There were no effects of age or group × age interaction. Figure 2 shows that after 3 mo of age, peak contrast sensitivity values did not change.

Figure 2
figure 2

Mean peak contrast sensitivity values by group and age. Results are presented as log peak sensitivities in cycles per degree. HYPO (n = 13; ▪); CH (n = 12; ); PREM (n = 16; ); and control subjects (n = 20; □).

Table 2 presents the groups' roll-off and visual acuity parameters. For roll-off, only age was significant (F = 4.68, df = 1.9, 110, p < 0.01), which reflected a generalized increase at higher spatial frequencies. This effect was somewhat larger between 4.5 and 6 mo of age than between 3 and 4.5 mo (means across groups: 3 mo = −0.59 ± 0.03; 4.5 mo = −0.61 ± 0.04; 6 mo = −0.52 ± 0.04). An effect of age was also observed for visual acuity (F = 22.9, df = 1.9, 111.2, p < 0.001), reflecting a linear increase in acuity between 3 and 6 mo of age (F = 41.5, df = 1,57, p < 0.001). Although the group main effect and interactions were not significant for PREM, these infants showed a trend toward mildly reduced visual acuity (p = 0.065) at 6 mo of age (see Table 2).

Table 2 Mean roll-off parameter and grating acuity by age in clinical and control groups

For COD, all effects were significant. The age effect (F = 5.16, df = 1.9, 113, p < 0.007) reflected a linear increase in r2 with age (F = 6.27, df = 1,57, p < 0.015). The group effect (F = 3.29, df = 3,57, p < 0.03) and group × age interaction (F = 2.2, df = 5.9, 113.2, p < 0.048) reflected the lower COD scores by HYPO and CH (but not PREM) relative to controls (see Fig. 3). This effect was particularly evident at 3 mo of age (HYPO: F = 4.37, df = 1,32, p < 0.04; CH: F = 18.19, df = 1,31, p < 0.001). The poorer fit of the data to the curve for HYPO and CH groups reflected their reduced contrast sensitivity at the low spatial frequency end of the function (data not shown). These results therefore signify that the data of 3-mo-old children in the HYPO and CH groups did not adequately fit the exponential model of the contrast sensitivity function. A similar trend was also noted at 4.5 mo for the CH group (F = 3.11, df = 1,31, p < 0.08).

Figure 3
figure 3

Mean COD scores by group and age. Because the COD distribution was heavily skewed, this variable was transformed, whereby y = 2arcsine(√p), where p was the infant's COD score. HYPO (53) (n = 13; ▪); CH (n = 12; ??); PREM (n = 16; ??); and control subjects (n = 20; □).

Within-group analyses.

To determine whether factors within the HYPO and CH groups contributed to reduced peak contrast sensitivities, we used a median split procedure. Infants in the HYPO group were stratified into subgroups on the basis of mothers' mean TSH values (range: 3.28–7.83 mIU/L) during the first and/or second trimester of pregnancy. The more severe group had mothers with TSH values between 5.53 and 7.83 mIU/L, and the less severe subgroup had mothers with values <5.53 mIU/L. ANOVA revealed a significant group × age interaction (F = 3.94, df = 2,22, p < 0.05) but no independent effects of group or age with the interaction reflecting an increase with age in peak sensitivity in the less severe group and the opposite trend for the more severe group (see Fig. 4). A similar set of analyses on COD scores revealed no effects, suggesting that the fit of the negative exponential curve is insensitive to severity of maternal thyroid disease. Finally, an identical analysis stratifying those with available data in the HYPO group according to maternal free thyroxine levels during pregnancy yielded no significant group differences.

Figure 4
figure 4

Contrast sensitivities by severity of maternal hypothyroidism. ▪, more severe group (n = 7); ??, less severe (n = 6) group.

The CH group was stratified by the child's initial hypothyroidism severity, with the more severe subgroup consisting of six infants with either athyreosis or TSH >200 mIU/L and the less severe subgroup with a lingual gland or dyshormonogenesis and having TSH values <100 mIU/L. A one-tailed t-test revealed that these groups differed significantly in log peak sensitivity, reflecting the lower scores at 6 mo of age of the more severe CH subgroup (1.8 versus 2.08; t = 2.62, df = 11, p < 0.05) (Fig. 5). There were no differences in COD scores (Fig. 5).

Figure 5
figure 5

Contrast sensitivities by severity of congenital hypothyroidism. ▪, more severe group (n = 6); ??, less severe group (n = 6).

Refractive Error.

To determine whether contrast sensitivity effects could be explained by optical factors, we also compared groups as to sphere equivalent and cylindrical errors. Refractive errors (Table 3) were not different among the groups. Values are comparable to those reported in another study of typically developing infants.

Table 3 Refractive error results of clinical and control groups

DISCUSSION

Present findings support our hypothesis of differences from normal in contrast sensitivity and visual acuity development in children of hypothyroid mothers, children with CH, and children who were born preterm at low risk. We found that compared with typically developing children, 3- to 6-mo-old infants whose mothers had preexisting or de novo hypothyroidism in pregnancy or who themselves had CH showed reduced contrast sensitivity at low spatial frequencies. In contrast, children who were born preterm (between 32 and 35 wk gestation) had normal contrast sensitivity but showed a trend toward weaker visual acuity. Children of hypothyroid women had large contrast sensitivity deficits when mothers had high TSH levels in the first half of pregnancy, as did children who had CH and an absent thyroid gland or very elevated TSH levels at birth. In both groups, the fit of the contrast sensitivity function was abnormal at 3 mo of age (and persisted until 4.5 mo in the CH group), suggesting an additional transient loss of sensitivity at the lowest spatial frequencies.

These findings therefore signify that adequate levels of TH are needed throughout pregnancy to ensure normal contrast sensitivity development, particularly at low spatial frequencies. Because the maternal hypothyroid group had the strongest deficit of the three clinical groups, adequate TH seems especially necessary during early pregnancy. Children with severe CH also showed reduced contrast sensitivity (but to a lesser degree than HYPO), indicating that TH is additionally needed during the third trimester for proper development. The normal maternal TH contribution in the CH group was not enough to offset their fetal thyroid hormone insufficiency because many children had elevated TSH levels at birth. Children with less severe CH had primarily postnatal hypothyroidism and attained normal contrast sensitivity functioning, suggesting that contrast sensitivity does not seem to be vulnerable to a postnatal lack of TH.

Preterm infants who were born between 32 and 35 wk gestation had typical contrast sensitivity development but had mildly weaker visual acuity. Rooman et al. (37) suggested that the fetal thyroid is able to assume adequate function and has less need for maternal supplementation after 33 wk gestation. Because even the youngest preterm infants in our study (i.e. 32 wk gestation) showed no deficits, these infants likely had sufficient TH for the normal development of contrast sensitivity. Indeed, “low-risk” preterm infants have better acuity relative to age-matched term infants (55), possibly as a result of their greater visual experience. Because all groups displayed age-appropriate refractive error, observed differences in acuity cannot be attributed to optical factors.

According to Norcia et al. (41), peak contrast sensitivity is adult-like by 3 mo of age and improves little over the remainder of the first year. Our findings on typically developing infants are similar, showing no major changes in peak contrast sensitivity between 3 and 6 mo of age. However, there were contrast sensitivity changes in the HYPO and CH groups between 3 and 6 mo of age. In both groups, those with the most severe conditions showed contrast sensitivity reductions at 6 mo, with the less severe infants showing no deficit or some recovery. This finding suggests that only the severely affected infants have persisting deficits, whereas those who are less severely affected manifest only delays in development.

Animal studies have shown that TH is necessary for the development of primary visual structures—the retina, magnocellular neurons in the lateral geniculate nucleus of the thalamus, and the primary visual cortex (19)—with timing being earlier for retina than for thalamus and for thalamus than for cortex (13). Given that TH is important for neurodevelopmental processes along this pathway, a lack of TH during these periods would disrupt such development (13,14). Because the HYPO group had the most compromised contrast sensitivity, this suggests that the effects of TH loss on development was likely disrupted at retinal and/or thalamic levels. Milder deficits in the CH group suggests cortical disruption. Unfortunately, because the VEP represents a cortical response to visual stimuli (56) and reflects processing along the entire pathway, it cannot specify the neuroanatomic levels of a deficit. Further testing using electroretinograms and developmental visual marker tasks (57,58) may serve to identify affected neuroanatomic sites.

Although this is the first study of its kind to compare models of human TH insufficiency at a basic functional level, it is limited for several reasons. Our small sample sizes may have reduced the ability to detect differences on variables other than peak contrast sensitivity and so not allow for in-depth analysis of within-group differences. A median split procedure that was used to stratify infants into more versus less severely affected subgroups did not permit determination of the TSH levels that define impairment or the critical period for deficits to occur. Because most mothers were under the care of family doctors and clinical community endocrinologists, who measured TSH mainly and at varying intervals and frequencies, we were not able to control when exactly thyroid hormones were measured. Subanalyses that are based on TSH values may not be ideal because TSH does not predict later infant outcome as well as does free thyroxine (48). Finally, because the sweep VEP is not ideal for estimating results of individual subjects, conclusions about timing effects for specific cases were limited.

Nevertheless, our results do demonstrate that adequate levels of TH are necessary throughout pregnancy for normal human contrast sensitivity development. Although the critical period extends throughout gestation, insufficient TH during early pregnancy produces a strong and persisting contrast sensitivity deficit. The clinical relevance of present findings awaits further testing (currently ongoing) that will determine specific cognitive dysfunctions. Given previous findings that showed suboptimal visuospatial and visuomotor abilities in thyroid-insufficient infants, these particular abilities may be dependent on normal contrast sensitivity functioning.

CONCLUSION

In conclusion, present findings suggest that TH is definitely necessary for human visual development, and some effects of TH loss during pregnancy may manifest in structures that are critical for visual processing.