Main

Our understanding of the neuronal mechanisms involved in sensory and cognitive processes has advanced considerably with the advent of modern imaging methods. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) makes use of the blood oxygenation level–dependent (BOLD) signal to visualize brain activity. The BOLD signal relies on the paramagnetic properties of deoxyhemoglobin (HbR) as a concentration-dependent, endogenous contrast agent. The increase in neuronal activity induced by a stimulus is quickly followed by a vascular response. Increasing neuronal activity leads to an increase in cerebral metabolic rate of oxygen consumption (CMRO2), which in turn increases the rate at which oxyhemoglobin is converted to HbR. The increase in the local HbR concentration is offset by the vascular response, resulting in an increase in the magnetization signal, commonly referred to as the BOLD response.

The noninvasive nature of fMRI makes it an ideal tool for imaging brain activity in children to follow maturational changes and to obtain an insight into the development of the human CNS (14). Performing and interpreting functional imaging data of the immature brain presents scientists with two main difficulties. The first is the difficulty of finding a normal cohort. The second and perhaps more important difficulty is that children, particularly young children, more often than not have to be measured during natural or induced sleep (2,3,58).

The influence on the BOLD signal of testing children under natural and induced sleep has been the subject of several investigations. The conclusions drawn from these unfortunately are still contradictory. Some reported that the incidence of negative BOLD responses increases during natural sleep (2,5) or induced sleep (9), suggesting an increase in the HbR concentration as a result of an attenuation of the vascular response. Others reported that the sedative agent reduces the neuronal activity (6,7,10). The above studies have shown clear differences in the BOLD response during natural or induced sleep, but its origin is still uncertain. If fMRI measurements are to assist our understanding of the functional changes resulting from cerebral maturation, then it is necessary to determine which factors change during sleep and how these manifest themselves in the BOLD response.

We investigated whether the anesthetic agent Sevoflurane altered the BOLD response in visual cortex by attenuating the neuronal activity or the vascular response. The main obstacle that needed to be overcome was that the neuronal activity and the vascular response seem to be linked. Increasing the neuronal activity increases the cerebral blood flow (CBF), and vice versa. What was needed was a set of stimuli with either the identical levels of neuronal activity or identical CBF responses. In their seminal work, linking the CBF response in visual cortex to stimulus frequency, Fox and Raichle (11) used a flashed and reversing stimulus to overcome the confounding effects of stimulus frequency and mean stimulus intensity. On the CBF response of the flashed and reversing stimulus at the different frequencies, the authors concluded, “The response curves of the local blood flow change as a function of stimulus rate demonstrate the near identity of the regional response, despite differences between the two stimulus modalities in luminance, pattern, distance from retina and degrees subtended in the visual field.” The flashed and reversing stimuli induce identical CBF responses in visual cortex.

We used a flashed and reversing checkerboard as shown in Figure 1. Both stimuli subtended the same visual angle and so projected on the same area of visual cortex. Because of the topographic organization of the visual cortex, the reversing checkerboard will activate all of the visual, cortical area onto which it projects, whereas the flashed checkerboard will activate only half of the area. Doubling the area of visual cortex being activated doubles the number of cells being activated, which in turn doubles the CMRO2. Both stimuli induced identical increases in CBF so that doubling the CMRO2 doubles the oxygen extraction fraction, which doubles the local HbR concentration. If the anesthetic agent Sevoflurane attenuates the neuronal activity more than the CBF, then the local HbR concentration would decrease and the BOLD signal amplitude increase. If the neuronal activity is reduced less than the vascular response, then the local HbR concentration would increase and the BOLD signal amplitude decrease. The question that we aimed to address is whether the neuronal metabolic activity (CMRO2) or the CBF is more strongly altered when children are measured during Sevoflurane anesthesia.

Figure 1
figure 1

The four different checkerboard stimuli used in our investigation. (A and C) The flashed checkerboard. (B and D) The reversing checkerboard.

METHODS

Subjects.

We performed fMRI on 10 children (five boys and five girls). The children who were measured under anesthesia were measured as part of a diagnostic MRI session that had to be performed under anesthesia because clinical indications necessitated it. Because these children usually displayed or were suspected of a medical condition warranting a diagnostic MRI, it was important to set strict inclusion and exclusion criteria. We excluded children with metabolic anomalies or micro-, macro-, or hydrencephaly, children had experienced hypoxic asphyxia, and children who had abnormalities of the visual system or had a tumor in the cortex located posterior to the central sulcus. Table 1 shows the clinical indications of the children who were measured under anesthesia in our study.

Table 1 Clinical indicators that led to a diagnostic MRI being prescribed

The children who were tested awake were recruited from a pool of normal, healthy children. The mean age of the children who were tested under anesthesia was 8.3 y (range, 7.1–10.2 y); that of the children who were tested awake was 9.1 y (range, 6.7–11.6 y). The testing procedure was approved by the hospital's ethics committee. Informed parental and child consent was obtained in each case.

Anesthetic management.

All anesthetic procedures were carried out by the hospital's anesthetist. The hospital guidelines on performing of MRI under anesthesia were adhered to at all times. Each patient was administered an oral dose of Midazolam (Dormicum, 0.5 mg/kg; maximum dose of 10 mg). This is a short-acting sedative and is used routinely to prepare patients for anesthesia. Anesthesia was initiated using Sevoflurane administered via a breathing mask. Nitrous oxide was avoided so as not to have to contend with the influence of two anesthetic agents. Tracheal intubation followed. The muscle relaxant pancuronium bromide (Pavulon, 0.1 mg/kg) was administered. Breathing and anesthesia were thereafter controlled via a mechanical ventilator. Throughout the entire MRI session, all vital signs including rectal body temperature, respiratory rate, heart rate, blood pressure, blood oxygen saturation, and expiratory CO2 were monitored constantly by the anesthetic team. Expiratory CO2 levels were kept between 4.8 and 6.0%.

The depth of anesthesia is measured using the mean alveolar concentration (MAC). One MAC is defined as the concentration of a specific anesthetic agent at which half of the patients still respond to a noxious stimulus (e.g. surgical incision) (12). We performed our fMRI measurements with an anesthetic level of 0.5 MAC using the anesthetic agent Sevoflurane in an oxygenair mixture. This level of anesthesia is considered light (1214). In children who were measured under anesthesia, the eyelids were kept open using eyelid retractors made of titanium. Before applying the lid retractors, a short-term, local analgesic agent (Alocaine; Alcon Pharmaceuticals, Mumbai, India) was administered to the eye. This agent has no influence on the pupil or the lens. The conjunctiva and cornea were protected using a clear gel (Viscotears; Novartis, Switzerland). In applying the gel, we ensured that it sealed the eye and that it set to an even surface to minimize optical distortion of our image. We waited until the end-tidal, expiratory concentration of Sevoflurane corresponded to 0.5 MAC before commencing our fMRI measurements. In the time required for the anesthesia level to stabilize at 0.5 MAC, the clinic performed a diagnostic MRI measurement so as not to unduly delay the examination of the child. After the anesthetist signaled that the level of anesthesia was at 0.5 MAC and stable, the functional measurements commenced. The fMRI measurements did not exceed 15 min. At the end of the fMRI measurements, the eyelid retractors were removed and the protective gel was washed out. The eyes were then taped shut, as is customary for the institute when diagnostic MRI is being performed under anesthesia. The children who were tested awake were instructed to keep their eyes in the middle of the radial checkerboard and avoid large eye movements.

Apparatus.

Measurements were performed on a 2 Tesla Bruker Tomikon S200 whole-body scanner (Bruker-Medical, Ettlingen, Germany) using a surface coil operated in cross-coil mode. This coil had been modified by affixing a surface-coated mirror to it in such a way that an image projected onto a translucent screen at the back of the magnet could be reflected to the subject's eyes. The coil was positioned so that it was most sensitive to signals from the occipital lobe. Inspecting Figure 1 reveals a signal intensity loss in the anterior part of the brain. This is the result of the signal loss caused by our use of a surface coil. The advantage of using a surface coil is that we had a higher sensitivity in the region of the visual cortex.

Functional imaging was performed using a single-shot, gradient-echo, EPI sequence with the following parameters: repetition time, 2000 ms; effective echo time, 58 ms; spectral width of 100 kHz; and a tip angle of 90 degrees. We also used a fat-suppression pulse with a 350-Hz bandwidth. A package of 14 slices with a thickness of 4.5 mm and a gap of 1 mm was placed horizontally through the calcarine sulcus so that the lowest slice coincided with the base of the occipital lobe. We ensured that the slice package was positioned in such a way that the entire posterior occipital cortex, including striate cortex, was included. The field of view measured 25 × 25 cm with a matrix size of 128 × 64 pixels, resulting in a spatial resolution of 2.0 × 4.1 mm. The EPI images were then reconstructed to a matrix size of 128 × 128 elements, including zero filling.

Stimuli.

Our stimuli consisted of two checkerboard pairs as shown in Figure 1. Each radial checkerboard subtended a visual angle of 36 by 36 degrees at a viewing distance of 1.13 m. We presented both a high-luminance contrast (black and white) and a low-luminance, high-chromatic contrast (red and blue) checkerboard pair.

Each checkerboard pair consisted of a flashed and a reversing checkerboard. The white elements of a checkerboard represented half of the area of the stimulus. All elements of the reversing checkerboard inverted their luminance contrast, whereas only half of the elements (the white elements) did so in the flashed checkerboard. We used a reversal frequency of 1 Hz, with each reversal lasting 500 ms. This is slower than the frequency used in other studies. In adults, a temporal frequency of 8 Hz leads to the largest CBF response (11,1517), whereas the maximum increase in CMRO2 has been observed at 4 Hz (11,1518). Our reason for choosing this frequency is that the lower temporal and spatial frequencies have a better response by the immature visual system (1923).

The luminance of the stimulus elements was measured using a luminance metre (LS 110; Minolta, Osaka, Japan). The luminance of the white elements was 144 cd/m2; that of the black background was 0.4 cd/m2. The luminance contrast ratio was 365:1. The luminance of the blue elements was 5.4 cd/m2; that of the red background was 29.3 cd/m2. The luminance contrast ratio was 5.5:1.

Procedure.

Each measurement started with 10 dummy scans, intended to bring the spin system into a steady state. These scans were immediately discarded. The actual measurements started with 10 control scans (20 s), during which the subject viewed a static image of the checkerboard stimulus. Ten stimulation scans (20 s) followed, during which either the flashed or the reversing checkerboard was presented. This cycle of control and stimulation period was repeated five times. The presentation order of the stimuli was randomized between subjects.

Data analysis.

We used the commercial package MEDx 3.4 (Medical Numerics Inc., Sterling, VA, U.S.A.) to analyze our functional data. We followed the procedure described of Boynton et al. (24) and divided our data analysis into two independent steps. First, we located the activated picture elements or voxels using the standard approach for fMRI studies. A motion correction using the automated image registration (25) was applied. This was followed by an intensity normalization, which adjusted the mean of the data to a value of 1000. Furthermore, a multilinear correction for baseline drift was applied. Activated voxels were determined using a cross-correlation analysis. The activated voxels detected were then subjected to a Bonferroni correction to correct for multiple testing. The correction threshold was obtained by convolving the uncorrected threshold (p = 0.05) with the number of voxels in a mask restricting the correction to the subject's head. The x, y, and z coordinates of each corrected, activated voxel was noted. These coordinates were then used to extract the time series data. We identified those voxels located within the calcarine sulcus and selected only these for our analysis.

On the basis of the time series data, we determined the change in HbR concentration by calculating the change in the spin relaxivity (ΔR2*) between the baseline and each time point (2629). We defined our baseline signal (Sb) as the average, transverse magnetization signal of the 10 control scans of the first cycle. We calculated ΔR2* using ΔR2* = (−1/TE) × ln(St/Sb), where TE is the echo time, St is the mean transverse magnetization signal at a specific time point, and Sb is the baseline signal (29). A ΔR2* value of 0 is indicative of no change in the HbR concentration between the time point and the baseline. A positive ΔR2* value is associated with a negative BOLD response as it indicates an increase in the concentration of HbR compared with the baseline. A negative ΔR2* value is associated with a positive BOLD response as it indicates a decrease in the concentration of HbR from the baseline. The ΔR2* amplitude is proportional to the change in HbR concentration from the baseline.

RESULTS

We examined the extent of activation as measured by the number of activated pixels and the change in the local concentration of HbR using the ΔR2*. Because of the number of subjects tested and because previous studies have shown that the luminance contrast and the chromatic contrast stimuli yielded the same result (30), we combined the data from the luminance and chromatic contrast stimuli. Unless otherwise stated, all statistical analyses were performed using SPSS 10 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, U.S.A.). The data were analyzed using repeated measures of the general linear model with stimulus and state of consciousness as factors. For brevity, we provide only details of the statistical analyses that yielded a p ≤ 0.05. We included ETA2 as a measure of the size of the effect independent of sample size.

Positive BOLD response.

Figure 2 depicts the location of the activated voxels as projected onto an anatomic MR image of a child measured awake and a child measured under anesthesia. We noted that the extent of activation was larger for the flashed than the reversing stimulus (FWilks = 10.874, df = 1,20, p = 0.005, ETA2 = 0.437). There was a significant interaction between the state in which the children were tested (FWilks = 12.664; df = 1,20, p = 0.003, ETA2 = 0.475).

Figure 2
figure 2

The location of the activated voxels to our flashed and reversing checkerboard stimuli in a child who was measured awake and a child who was measured under Sevoflurane anesthesia. The number of activated voxels may be smaller in the child who was measured under anesthesia, but it is still evident that the presentation of the flashed checkerboard induced a larger number of activated voxels than the reversing checkerboard.

The children who were measured awake yielded a larger number of activated voxels than the children who were measured under anesthesia. This is illustrated in Figure 3A. Examining the ΔR2* for the flashed then the reversing checkerboard stimulus, we found a lower HbR concentration for the former than the latter (FWilks = 4.761, df = 1,12, p = 0.05, ETA2 = 0.281). There was no significant interaction between the size of the ΔR2* change and whether the children were tested awake and under anesthesia (Fig. 3B).

Figure 3
figure 3

The mean number of activated voxels and the peak ΔR2* for the positive BOLD response. (A) The extent of activation using the mean number of activated voxels to the flashed and the reversing checkerboard.▪, Children who were measured awake; □, children measured during anesthesia. (B) The peak ΔR2* associated with the flashed and the reversing checkerboard stimulus.▪, Children who were measured awake; □, children who were tested under anesthesia. The error bars indicate the SEM. The closer the mean peak ΔR2* value is to 0, the smaller the difference in the HbR concentration is compared with the baseline value.

Negative BOLD response.

We found no statistically significant differences in either the extent of activation (Fig. 4A) or the peak ΔR2* (Fig. 4B) between the children who were tested awake and under anesthesia. Finally, we found no statistically significant correlation between the number of activated voxels with a positive BOLD response and the number of activated voxels with a negative BOLD response.

Figure 4
figure 4

The mean number of activated voxels and the peak ΔR2* for the negative BOLD response. (A) The extent of activation using the mean number of activated voxels to the flashed and the reversing checkerboard.▪, Children measured awake; □, children who were measured during anesthesia. (B) The peak ΔR2* associated with the flashed and the reversing checkerboard stimulus. ▪, Children who were measured awake; □, children who were tested under anesthesia. The closer the peak ΔR2* value is to 0, the smaller the difference in the HbR concentration is compared with the baseline value. The bars indicate the SEM.

DISCUSSION

Our study has shown that, although the extent of activation was affected considerably by the light level of Sevoflurane anesthesia used, the BOLD signal amplitude was not. If the BOLD signal amplitude was not altered, then neither the neuronal metabolic activity nor the vascular response could have been adversely influenced by light anesthesia. The results from fMRI under light anesthesia therefore are comparable to those obtained from the awake child. In the succeeding sections, we discuss some of the most important issues.

Role of consciousness in visual processing.

Because one group of children in our study was measured awake and the other under light anesthesia, it is important to examine the possible role of consciousness on our measurements. There is evidence that the lower visual areas are not entirely driven by the retinal input alone and that feedback projections also influence the neuronal activity. Our study revealed a close agreement in the BOLD signal amplitude between the awake and the anesthetized group to our stimuli. We interpret this as indicating that the BOLD response in our study was almost entirely due to the activation of a mechanism driven by the retinal input. Having a mechanism that is unaltered by consciousness may not provide any insight into the contribution of the cognitive development to visual perception. Our study instead provides a means of understanding the development of the hard-wired side of visual perception. Once this aspect is understood, the contribution made by the cognitive development in visual perception can be more readily distinguished from the more mechanical aspect.

Defining neuronal activity.

Neuronal activity has traditionally been equated with the electrical discharge activity of neurons. fMRI using the BOLD signal detects changes in the HbR concentration within a region of cortex that can be as small as 1 mm3. This small volume, however, will contain millions of cells. The oxidative metabolism within this volume will depend not only on the electrical discharge activity of a group of cells but also and probably more so on the number of cells being activated.

Influence of our stimuli on the CBF.

We were not able to include a CBF measurement for ethical reasons. Measuring the CBF response would have doubled the time required to complete our fMRI session or would have involved subjecting the children to a positron emission tomography measurement to obtain an accurate assessment of the CBF response (31). Instead, we relied on the published positron emission tomography data of Fox and Raichle (11), demonstrating that the CBF response to a flashed and a reversing stimulus is identical. Also, our finding that the ΔR2* was larger for the flashed than the reversing checkerboard is consistent with the prediction by Buxton (32) for stimuli with identical CBF responses but different CMRO2 loads. These observations confirm that our checkerboard stimuli did induce identical CBF responses.

Neuronal activity and the BOLD response.

In previous fMRI investigations, the electrical activity of a neuronal population has been manipulated by varying the stimulus contrast (2628,3336) or stimulus frequency (11,1518). In our study, the flashed and reversing checkerboards had the same luminance contrast and the same stimulus frequency. The neuronal electrical discharge activity associated with the two stimuli should therefore be identical. The reversing checkerboard, however, activated twice as many neurons as the flashed checkerboard. This enabled us to examine the influence of on the BOLD response. This being the case, the CMRO2 oxygen requirements resulting from an increase in the CMRO2 would have to be drawn from the identical blood volume. Increasing CMRO2 would increase the oxygen extraction fraction and so increase in the concentration of HbR in the venous blood.

We first consider the voxels with a positive BOLD response as these represent the standard BOLD response reported in most fMRI studies. Figure 3B shows that the peak ΔR2* was larger during the presentation of the flashed than the reversing checkerboard. This finding shows that the local HbR concentration reached a lower level during the flashed than during the reversing checkerboard. This is consistent with our view that the CMRO2 was lower during the flashed than during the reversing checkerboard stimulus, whereas the CBF response was identical for the two stimuli.

The negative BOLD response is harder to interpret as we found no difference in either the extent of activation (Fig. 4A) or the peak ΔR2* (Fig. 4B) to the flashed and the reversing checkerboard. This suggests that the negative BOLD response is not related to either CMRO2 or CBF but to some other factor.

Influence of anesthesia on the positive BOLD response.

We first discuss the extent of activation as indicated by the number of activated voxels. The children who were measured under light anesthesia yielded fewer activated voxels than the children who were measured awake. This indicated to us that anesthesia may have lowered the absolute number of activated voxels but that the relative difference between the stimuli was preserved. However, the flashed checkerboard still yielded the larger extent of activation than the reversing checkerboard in both groups of children (Fig. 3A). We found no difference in the peak ΔR2*, so the change in the local HbR concentration to our stimuli was the same for the children who were tested awake and under Sevoflurane anesthesia. This suggests that the anesthetic agent Sevoflurane at a concentration of 0.5MAC did not alter CMRO2 or interfere with CBF.

A reduction in the extent of activation during anesthesia but a preservation of the CMRO2 and the CBF response reflects the idea that conscious processing depends on synchronized activity across several neuronal nets (3742). The loss of consciousness results in a loss of synchronized activity across different neuronal nets, reducing the extent over which synchronized activation takes place while leaving the neuronal activity in the core net unaffected.

Influence of anesthesia on the negative BOLD response.

The origin of the negative BOLD response is still not completely understood, but it is the result of an increase in the local HbR concentration. A number of studies have noted that the negative BOLD response increases in neonates compared with infants and young children (7), if subjects are measured during natural sleep (2,5) or sleep resulting from the administration of a sedative agent (6,9). We were interested in examining whether the light level of Sevoflurane anesthesia used in our study increased the incidence of a negative BOLD response. There was no difference in either the extent of activation or the CMRO2 between the children who were tested awake and under light anesthesia. The negative BOLD response does not seem to be influenced in any way by anesthesia. The difference in finding with respect to the negative BOLD response between our investigation and previous studies may be due to our having used an anesthetic agent (Sevoflurane) rather than a sedative agent.

Performing fMRI measurements under anesthesia.

The extent of activation can best be thought of as the window through which we can measure the neuronal activation, whereas the peak ΔR2* represents the neuronal metabolic activity. Provided that the window is not lost, the CMRO2 and the CBF are comparable to that seen in children who are measured awake. Performing fMRI measurements on children under Sevoflurane anesthesia will yield a BOLD signal amplitude that is a reliable indicator of the underlying neuronal activity. With the necessary care and precaution, it is possible to examine specific aspects of cerebral maturation using fMRI and compare the results between young children who were measured under light Sevoflurane anesthesia and older children who were measured awake. Whether this applies to other anesthetic agents and thus can be generalized will require further investigation.