Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Mind matters: placebo enhances reward learning in Parkinson's disease

Abstract

Expectations have a powerful influence on how we experience the world. Neurobiological and computational models of learning suggest that dopamine is crucial for shaping expectations of reward and that expectations alone may influence dopamine levels. However, because expectations and reinforcers are typically manipulated together, the role of expectations per se has remained unclear. We separated these two factors using a placebo dopaminergic manipulation in individuals with Parkinson's disease. We combined a reward learning task with functional magnetic resonance imaging to test how expectations of dopamine release modulate learning-related activity in the brain. We found that the mere expectation of dopamine release enhanced reward learning and modulated learning-related signals in the striatum and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. These effects were selective to learning from reward: neither medication nor placebo had an effect on learning to avoid monetary loss. These findings suggest a neurobiological mechanism by which expectations shape learning and affect.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: Instrumental learning task.
Figure 2: Behavioral results for the gain and loss conditions (n = 18).
Figure 3: vmPFC value activity (n = 15).
Figure 4: Striatal prediction error activity (n = 15).

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Goetz, C.G. et al. Placebo influences on dyskinesia in Parkinson's disease. Mov. Disord. 23, 700–707 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Goetz, C.G. et al. Placebo response in Parkinson's disease: comparisons among 11 trials covering medical and surgical interventions. Mov. Disord. 23, 690–699 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. McRae, C. et al. Effects of perceived treatment on quality of life and medical outcomes in a double-blind placebo surgery trial. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 61, 412–420 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Benedetti, F. et al. Placebo-responsive Parkinson patients show decreased activity in single neurons of subthalamic nucleus. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 587–588 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. de la Fuente-Fernández, R. et al. Expectation and dopamine release: mechanism of the placebo effect in Parkinson's disease. Science 293, 1164–1166 (2001).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Lidstone, S.C. et al. Effects of expectation on placebo-induced dopamine release in Parkinson disease. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 67, 857–865 (2010).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Voon, V. et al. Mechanisms underlying dopamine-mediated reward bias in compulsive behaviors. Neuron 65, 135–142 (2010).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. de la Fuente-Fernández, R. et al. Dopamine release in human ventral striatum and expectation of reward. Behav. Brain Res. 136, 359–363 (2002).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Schweinhardt, P., Seminowicz, D.A., Jaeger, E., Duncan, G.H. & Bushnell, M.C. The anatomy of the mesolimbic reward system: a link between personality and the placebo analgesic response. J. Neurosci. 29, 4882–4887 (2009).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Zubieta, J.K. & Stohler, C.S. Neurobiological mechanisms of placebo responses. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 1156, 198–210 (2009).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Pessiglione, M., Seymour, B., Flandin, G., Dolan, R.J. & Frith, C.D. Dopamine-dependent prediction errors underpin reward-seeking behavior in humans. Nature 442, 1042–1045 (2006).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Sutton, R.S. & Barto, A.G. Reinforcement Learning: an Introduction (Bradford Book, 1998).

  13. Barto, A.G. Reinforcement learning and dynamic programming. in Analysis, Design and Evaluation of Man-Machine Systems, Vol. 1,2 (ed. Johannsen, G.) 407–412 (1995).

  14. Daw, N.D. in Trial-by-trial data analysis using computational models. Decision Making, Affect, Learning: Attention and Performance XXIII, Vol. 1 (eds. Delgado, M.R., Phelps, E.A. & Robbins, T.W.) 3–38 (Oxford University Press, 2011).

  15. Daw, N.D., O'Doherty, J.P., Dayan, P., Seymour, B. & Dolan, R.J. Cortical substrates for exploratory decisions in humans. Nature 441, 876–879 (2006).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Hare, T.A., O'Doherty, J., Camerer, C.F., Schultz, W. & Rangel, A. Dissociating the role of the orbitofrontal cortex and the striatum in the computation of goal values and prediction errors. J. Neurosci. 28, 5623–5630 (2008).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Levy, D.J. & Glimcher, P.W. The root of all value: a neural common currency for choice. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 22, 1027–1038 (2012).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Plassmann, H., O'Doherty, J.P. & Rangel, A. Appetitive and aversive goal values are encoded in the medial orbitofrontal cortex at the time of decision making. J. Neurosci. 30, 10799–10808 (2010).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. McClure, S.M., Berns, G.S. & Montague, P.R. Temporal prediction errors in a passive learning task activate human striatum. Neuron 38, 339–346 (2003).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. O'Doherty, J.P., Dayan, P., Friston, K., Critchley, H. & Dolan, R.J. Temporal difference models and reward-related learning in the human brain. Neuron 38, 329–337 (2003).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Daw, N.D. & Doya, K. The computational neurobiology of learning and reward. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 16, 199–204 (2006).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Rushworth, M.F., Mars, R.B. & Summerfield, C. General mechanisms for decision making. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 19, 75–83 (2009).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Chowdhury, R. et al. Dopamine restores reward prediction errors in old age. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 648–653 (2013).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Schönberg, T., Daw, N.D., Joel, D. & O'Doherty, J.P. Reinforcement learning signals in the human striatum distinguish learners from nonlearners during reward-based decision making. J. Neurosci. 27, 12860–12867 (2007).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Schönberg, T. et al. Selective impairment of prediction error signaling in human dorsolateral but not ventral striatum in Parkinson's disease patients: evidence from a model-based fMRI study. Neuroimage 49, 772–781 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Behrens, T.E., Hunt, L.T., Woolrich, M.W. & Rushworth, M.F. Associative learning of social value. Nature 456, 245–249 (2008).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Li, J., Delgado, M.R. & Phelps, E.A. How instructed knowledge modulates the neural systems of reward learning. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 108, 55–60 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Niv, Y., Edlund, J.A., Dayan, P. & O'Doherty, J.P. Neural prediction errors reveal a risk-sensitive reinforcement-learning process in the human brain. J. Neurosci. 32, 551–562 (2012).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Kirsch, I. Response expectancy as a determinant of experience and behavior. Am. Psychol. 40, 1189–1202 (1985).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Wager, T.D. et al. Placebo-induced changes in FMRI in the anticipation and experience of pain. Science 303, 1162–1167 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Pessiglione, M. et al. Subliminal instrumental conditioning demonstrated in the human brain. Neuron 59, 561–567 (2008).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Schmidt, L., Palminteri, S., Lafargue, G. & Pessiglione, M. Splitting motivation: unilateral effects of subliminal incentives. Psychol. Sci. 21, 977–983 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Benedetti, F. How the doctor's words affect the patient's brain. Eval. Health Prof. 25, 369–386 (2002).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Fournier, J.C. et al. Antidepressant drug effects and depression severity: a patient-level meta-analysis. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 303, 47–53 (2010).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  35. Kirsch, I. & Sapirstein, G. Listening to Prozac but hearing placebo: a meta-analysis of antidepressant medication. Prev. Treat. 1, 2a (1998).

    Google Scholar 

  36. Bódi, N. et al. Reward-learning and the novelty-seeking personality: a between- and within-subjects study of the effects of dopamine agonists on young Parkinson's patients. Brain 132, 2385–2395 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Frank, M.J., Seeberger, L.C. & O'Reilly, R.C. By carrot or by stick: cognitive reinforcement learning in parkinsonism. Science 306, 1940–1943 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Palminteri, S. et al. Pharmacological modulation of subliminal learning in Parkinson's and Tourette's syndromes. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 106, 19179–19184 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Maia, T.V. Reinforcement learning, conditioning, and the brain: successes and challenges. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 9, 343–364 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Gold, J.M. et al. Negative symptoms and the failure to represent the expected reward value of actions: behavioral and computational modeling evidence. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 69, 129–138 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Bamford, N.S. et al. Heterosynaptic dopamine neurotransmission selects sets of corticostriatal terminals. Neuron 42, 653–663 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Wang, W. et al. Regulation of prefrontal excitatory neurotransmission by dopamine in the nucleus accumbens core. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 590, 3743–3769 (2012).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  43. Bamford, N.S. et al. Dopamine modulates release from corticostriatal terminals. J. Neurosci. 24, 9541–9552 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Colloca, L. et al. Learning potentiates neurophysiological and behavioral placebo analgesic responses. Pain 139, 306–314 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Voudouris, N.J., Peck, C.L. & Coleman, G. The role of conditioning and verbal expectancy in the placebo response. Pain 43, 121–128 (1990).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Kordower, J.H. et al. Disease duration and the integrity of the nigrostriatal system in disease. Brain 136, 2419–2431 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Cools, R., Barker, R.A., Sahakian, B.J. & Robbins, T.W. Enhanced or impaired cognitive function in Parkinson's disease as a function of dopaminergic medication and task demands. Cereb. Cortex 11, 1136–1143 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Frank, M.J., Seeberger, L.C. & O'Reilly, R.C. By carrot or by stick: cognitive reinforcement learning in parkinsonism. Science 306, 1940–1943 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Shohamy, D., Myers, C.E., Geghman, K.D., Sage, J. & Gluck, M.A. L-dopa impairs learning, but spares generalization, in Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychologia 44, 774–784 (2006).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Shohamy, D., Myers, C.E., Grossman, S., Sage, J. & Gluck, M.A. The role of dopamine in cognitive sequence learning: evidence from Parkinson's disease. Behav. Brain Res. 156, 191–199 (2005).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Benedetti, F. et al. Placebo-responsive Parkinson patients show decreased activity in single neurons of subthalamic nucleus. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 587–588 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Benedetti, F. et al. Electrophysiological properties of thalamic, subthalamic and nigral neurons during the anti-parkinsonian placebo response. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 587, 3869–3883 (2009).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  53. Starkstein, S.E. et al. Reliability, validity, and clinical correlates of apathy in Parkinson's disease. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 4, 134–139 (1992).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Spielberger, C.D. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory: Bibliography, 2nd edn. (Consulting Psychologists Press, 1989).

  55. Movement Disorder Society Task Force on Rating Scales for Parkinson's Disease. The Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS): status and recommendations. Mov. Disord. 18, 738–750 (2003).

  56. Sutton, R.S. & Barto, A.G. Reinforcement Learning: an Introduction (Bradford Book, 1998).

  57. Lindquist, M.A., Spicer, J., Asllani, I. & Wager, T.D. Estimating and testing variance components in a multi-level GLM. Neuroimage 59, 490–501 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Mazaika, P., Hoeft, F., Glover, G.H. & Reiss, A.L. Methods and software for fMRI analysis of clinical subjects. Organ. Hum. Brain Mapp 475 (2009).

  59. Hare, T.A., O'Doherty, J., Camerer, C.F., Schultz, W. & Rangel, A. Dissociating the role of the orbitofrontal cortex and the striatum in the computation of goal values and prediction errors. J. Neurosci. 28, 5623–5630 (2008).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Pessiglione, M., Seymour, B., Flandin, G., Dolan, R.J. & Frith, C.D. Dopamine-dependent prediction errors underpin reward-seeking behavior in humans. Nature 442, 1042–1045 (2006).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Behrens, T.E., Hunt, L.T., Woolrich, M.W. & Rushworth, M.F. Associative learning of social value. Nature 456, 245–249 (2008).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  62. Niv, Y., Edlund, J.A., Dayan, P. & O'Doherty, J.P. Neural prediction errors reveal a risk-sensitive reinforcement-learning process in the human brain. J. Neurosci. 32, 551–562 (2012).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. Li, J., Delgado, M.R. & Phelps, E.A. How instructed knowledge modulates the neural systems of reward learning. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 108, 55–60 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Maldjian, J.A., Laurienti, P.J., Kraft, R.A. & Burdette, J.H. An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas–based interrogation of fMRI data sets. Neuroimage 19, 1233–1239 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank neurologists P. Greene, R. Alcalay, L. Coté and the nursing staff of the Center for Parkinson's Disease and Other Movement Disorders at Columbia University Presbyterian Hospital for help with patient recruitment and discussion of the findings, N. Johnston and B. Vail for help with data collection, M. Sharp, K. Duncan, D. Sulzer, J. Weber and B. Doll for insightful discussion, and M. Pessiglione and G.E. Wimmer for helpful comments on an earlier version of the manuscript. This study was supported by the Michael J. Fox Foundation and the US National Institutes of Health (R01MH076136).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

D.S. and T.D.W. planned the experiment. L.S., T.D.W. and D.S. developed the experimental design. L.S. and E.K.B. collected data. L.S. analyzed data. D.S. and T.D.W. supervised and assisted in data analysis. L.S., E.K.B., T.D.W. and D.S. wrote the manuscript.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Tor D Wager or Daphna Shohamy.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Integrated supplementary information

Supplementary Figure 1 Experimental and task design.

(a) Experimental design. Each patient was scanned 3 times: off drug, on placebo, and on drug. Off drug and placebo scan sessions were counterbalanced across patients (order 1: 8 patients; order 2: 10 patients). Scan sessions lasted for 1 h and were separated by a 1 h break. Placebo and dopaminergic medication were crushed into orange juice and administered 30 min before the respective scan session. (b) Task structure. The outcome of a trial could be a gain of $10, nothing ($0), or a loss of $10. Two cue pairs, a gain and a loss cue pair, were randomly intermixed. Within the gain cue pair, optimal choices led to a gain of $10 with a probability of.75 and to nothing ($0) with a probability of.25. Within the loss cue pair, optimal choices led to nothing ($0) with a probability of.75 and to a loss of $10 with a probability of.25.

Supplementary Figure 2 Partial correlations between on drug and placebo controlling for the effects of off drug (n = 18).

Partial correlations between on drug and placebo controlling for effects of off drug for reward learning (left) (r = 0.34, p = 0.08) and motor symptoms (right) (r = 0.59, p = 0.01). On drug and placebo expressed as residuals after confounds due to off drug effects were regressed out.

Supplementary Figure 3 BOLD responses to choices and feedback in the vmPFC and the ventral striatum (n = 15).

(a) Parameter estimates for choices (correct vs. incorrect) from the vmPFC region of interest in the gain and loss condition for off drug (gray), placebo (blue), and on drug (black) treatments. (b) Parameter estimates for feedback (correct vs. incorrect) from the ventral striatum region of interest in the gain and loss condition for the off drug (gray), placebo (blue), and on drug (black) treatments. Error bars represent within subject standard errors.

Supplementary Figure 4 Ventral striatum responses to components of the prediction error (n = 15).

Parameter estimates (betas) from the ventral striatum at time of outcome for the two components of prediction error – expected value and reward – in the off drug (gray), placebo (blue) and on drug (black) treatments. Error bars represent within subject standard errors.

Supplementary Figure 5 Reaction times during learning across conditions and treatment.

Average reaction time in seconds for the gain (left) and loss condition (right) in the off drug (gray), placebo (blue), and on drug (black) treatments. The reaction time curves depict how fast patients chose the optimal choice cue. Error bars represent within-subject standard errors.

Supplementary Figure 6 Behavioral results for the subgroup of patients scanned with fMRI (n = 15).

Percentage of observed optimal choices binned across bocks of 8 trials (left), smoothed (middle), and modeled optimal choices (right) in the gain (top) and loss (bottom) conditions for the off drug (gray), placebo (blue), and on drug (black) treatments. The learning curves depict how often patients chose the 75% rewarding cue (t11 = 5.2, p < 0.001) during the gain condition and the 75% nothing cue (t11 = 4.1, p < 0.01) during the loss condition. Error bars represent within-subject standard errors.

Supplementary Figure 7 Observed and modeled behavioral results for the scanned patients (n = 15).

Percentage of observed behavioral choices (dots) and modeled optimal choices (solid lines) across trials for off drug (gray), placebo (blue), and on drug (black) treatments. The learning curves depict how often patients chose the 75% rewarding cue (t11 = 5.2, p < 0.001) during the gain condition and the 75% loss cue (t11 = −4.1, p < 0.01) during the loss condition. The modeled learning curves represent the probabilities of choice on each trial, as predicted by the RL model.

Supplementary Figure 8 Direct comparisons of value and prediction error responses across treatments.

Statistical parametric maps (SPMs) are superimposed on the average structural scan. (a) SPMs are masked for the vmPFC ROI, defined a priori by MNI = [−1, 27, −8]) from Hare et al. 2008, at p < 0.05 uncorrected. (b) Whole brain activations for value at p < 0.05 uncorrected. (c) SPMs are masked for the ventral striatum ROI, defined a priori by MNI = [−10, 12, −8]) from Pessiglione et al. 2006, at p < 0.05 uncorrected. (d) Whole brain activations for prediction error at p < 0.05 uncorrected.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Text and Figures

Supplementary Figures 1–8 and Supplementary Tables 1–2 (PDF 795 kb)

Supplementary Methods Checklist

(PDF 530 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Schmidt, L., Braun, E., Wager, T. et al. Mind matters: placebo enhances reward learning in Parkinson's disease. Nat Neurosci 17, 1793–1797 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3842

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3842

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing