Main

Electrical brain stimulation, like intravenous administration of drugs of abuse, can establish arbitrary response habits (e.g., lever-pressing) in rats (see Wise 1996). Although there are many sites at which brain stimulation is rewarding, stimulation of the medial forebrain bundle (MFB) at the level of the lateral hypothalamus (LH) produces high response rates without evidence of aversive effects. Moreover, it is thought that the MFB is part of a final common path for the rewarding effects of brain stimulation at a variety of limbic sites (Wise 1996).

Many drugs of abuse—including morphine, cocaine, amphetamine, phencyclidine (PCP), and nicotine—potentiate the rewarding impact of MFB stimulation (see Wise 1996). When tested in the “curve-shift” variant of the brain stimulation reward (BSR) paradigm, these drugs increase the potency of the stimulation, causing leftward shifts in the functions that relate response strength to stimulation strength (Gallistel and Freyd 1987; Frank et al. 1988). Such shifts suggest additivity between the rewarding effects of the drug and the rewarding effects of the stimulation. Conversely, agents that attenuate drug reward (dopamine or opioid antagonists) attenuate BSR on their own, and block the ability of drugs of abuse to cause leftward shifts (Gallistel and Freyd 1987; Rompré and Wise 1989). Also, microinjections of drugs of abuse potentiate BSR in the same brain regions in which they are rewarding in their own right (Wise 1996). Together, these findings are often considered evidence that drug reward and BSR share common neural substrates.

Repeated exposure to drugs of abuse can alter their rewarding effects in rats. Pre-exposure to amphetamine, cocaine, and morphine facilitates the ability of these drugs to establish conditioned place preferences (Lett 1989). Similarly, rats pre-exposed to amphetamine or cocaine learn more rapidly to self-administer these drugs intravenously (Piazza et al. 1990; Horger et al. 1990). These findings suggest that drug reward undergoes sensitization (reverse-tolerance) with repeated intermittent treatment. However, in curve-shift variants of the BSR paradigm, similar regimens do not produce any change in the strength of the reward-related effects of these drugs: there is no evidence of sensitization (or tolerance) to the rewarding effects of cocaine (Frank et al. 1988; Bauco and Wise 1997), amphetamine (Wise and Munn 1993), morphine (Bauco et al. 1993), PCP (Carlezon and Wise 1993), or nicotine (Bauco and Wise 1994) with repeated intermittent treatment. These findings raise the possibility that pre-exposure to electrical stimulation of the MFB during BSR training causes adaptations that prevent further changes in the rewarding strength of the drugs.

Drug exposure causes changes in gene expression within brain reward systems, some of which may contribute to these alterations in drug sensitivity. Stimulants and opiates elevate expression of the immediate-early gene c-fos throughout the striatum, including the nucleus accumbens (Graybiel et al. 1990; Curran et al. 1996; Badiani et al. 1998; Kelz et al. 1999). Morphine and cocaine elevate expression of tyrosine hydroxylase (TH), the rate-limiting enzyme in dopamine synthesis, in the VTA (Beitner-Johnson and Nestler 1991).

Repeated intermittent exposure to these drugs also elevates expression of the AMPA receptor subunit GluR1 in the VTA (Fitzgerald et al. 1996; Churchill et al. 1999), which may explain increases in AMPA receptor function seen in dopamine neurons within this region after drug exposure (Zhang et al. 1997). Repeated cocaine elevates expression of the NMDA receptor subunit NMDAR1 in the VTA (Fitzgerald et al. 1996), and can elevate GluR1 in the nucleus accumbens (Churchill et al. 1999). In some cases, these changes in gene expression have been directly linked to changes in the motivational aspects of drugs of abuse. In the nucleus accumbens, expression of Fos-related proteins (i.e., ΔFosB) increases sensitivity to the rewarding effects of cocaine, whereas elevated GluR1 expression increases sensitivity to the aversive effects of cocaine (Kelz et al. 1999). In the rostral VTA, elevated GluR1 expression causes dramatic increases in the rewarding effects of morphine (Carlezon et al. 1997, 2000). Elevated GluR1 expression in the VTA might trigger a cascade of molecular changes that cause behavioral adaptations (see Self and Nestler 1995) because it favors the cellular influx of Ca2+ (Hollmann and Heinemann 1994; Seeberg et al. 1998), which plays an important role in many types of neuroadaptations (see Zucker 1999). Considering that sensitized behavioral responses can outlast elevated GluR1 expression in the VTA (Carlezon et al. 1997), it seems likely that this transient neuroadaptation contributes to the induction, rather than the expression, of sensitization. Indeed, there is considerable evidence that the induction of sensitization requires neuroadaptations in the VTA, whereas the expression of sensitization involves subsequent neuroadaptations in VTA target regions such as the nucleus accumbens (Kalivas and Stewart 1991; Wolf 1998).

Conversely, little is known about how BSR affects gene expression. Rewarding MFB stimulation elevates c-fos expression in various brain regions, including the VTA, nucleus accumbens, peduncopontine nucleus, locus coeruleus, septum, and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (Arvanitogiannis et al. 1997; Flores et al. 1997; Hunt and McGregor 1998). In the VTA, c-fos expression does not appear to occur in TH-positive (dopaminergic) neurons (Hunt and McGregor 1998), and it desensitizes with repeated exposure to the stimulation (Nakahara et al. 1999). However, it is not known if these changes have behavioral relevance. Moreover, the effect of BSR on expression of other proteins with established behavioral relevance (i.e., GluR1 in the VTA) has not been studied.

The goal of the present study was to determine if the effects of repeated intermittent exposure to rewarding brain stimulation has the same effects on the expression of GluR1, GluR2, NMDAR1, and TH as does repeated intermittent exposure to cocaine and opiates. We report here that there are important differences in the molecular consequences of these two types of rewarding stimuli. The differences, particularly in the case of GluR1 expression, may explain why the rewarding effects of drugs of abuse do not sensitize with repeated testing in the BSR paradigm.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Sixteen male Sprague-Dawley rats (350–450 g; Charles River Laboratories) were used. Rats were individually housed in standard plastic cages within a colony room on a reverse 12 hr light: 12 hr dark cycle (lights off at 7:00–19:00), with free access to food and water. Each rat was anesthetized with pentobarbital (50 mg/kg, i.p.), given atropine sulfate (0.25 mg/kg, s.c.) to reduce bronchial secretions, and implanted with a monopolar, stainless steel electrode (Plastics One) aimed at the right MFB, at the level of the lateral hypothalamus (3.0 mm posterior to bregma, 1.6 mm lateral from midsaggital suture, and 7.5 mm below dura) (Paxinos and Watson 1986; see Stellar et al. 1988). Skull screws (which served as the ground) and the electrode were secured to the skull with dental acrylic. All procedures were conducted in accordance with the NIH Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals.

One week after surgery, rats in the Experimental (E) group were trained on a continuous reinforcement schedule (FR1) to lever press for trains of 0.25 sec square-wave monopolar cathodal pulses (0.1 msec pulse duration), at a set frequency of 100 Hz. Stimulation was delivered by Stimtek stimulator-microcontrollers. For each rat, the minimal stimulation current (316–398 μA) that produced reliable responding with no signs of aversion (turning away from the lever, vocalization, and/or seizures) was used. All rats pressed reliably by the end of this 1 hr training session. On each of the following nine days, rats were allowed to self-stimulate on an FR-1 schedule of reinforcement for 1 hr. Sessions began with illumination of a house light in the test chamber, and non-contingent delivery of a single train of priming stimulation. Control (C) animals were placed in the same operant chambers for 1 hr each day for 10 consecutive days, but did not receive any stimulation. These procedures were conducted during the dark phase of the rats' light cycle.

To quantify the amount of stimulation that each rat received during each session, charge (Q) was calculated according to a minor modification of the formula Q = INd, where Q = charge (μC), I = Current (μA), N = number of pulses in a 0.25 sec train, and d = pulse duration (0.0001 sec) (see Gallistel 1978). This formula combines all stimulation parameters into a single representative value for each stimulation site per subject. With this method, the excitability of the brain tissue under the tip of the electrode can be assessed and compared among many brain regions supporting BSR, as well as for the subregions within a specific area of interest (see Bielajew and Trzcinska 1998). Thus Q is an index of the amount of rewarding stimulation that each rat receives. Usually, Q has been used to reflect the characteristics of sites supporting BSR in mapping studies, where only a limited number of rate-frequency curves can be obtained because of performance deficits, seizures, etc. Since no rate-frequency curves were collected in the present study, Q was calculated by multiplying current, pulse duration, number of pulses (which in a 0.25 sec train was 25 for 100 Hz stimulation) and the number of reinforcements per day to obtain a final average value per subject.

Rats were killed by decapitation 24–26 hr after the final brain stimulation (or control) session, and brains were placed immediately in ice-cold artificial cerebrospinal fluid (aCSF) buffer. A Stoelting tissue slicer was used to obtain 1.0 mm-thick coronal slices of brain containing the rostral-central aspects of the VTA (4.8–5.8 posterior to bregma), and a blunted 15 gauge needle was used to collect bilateral tissue punches. Tissue from each rat was homogenized in 250 μl of 1% SDS, and protein content was determined by the method of Lowry et al. (1951). Tissue from each sample (15 μg) was then subjected to one-dimensional SDS-polyacrylamide gel electrophoresis on 7.5% polyacrylamide resolving gels, and transferred electrophoretically onto polyvinylidene difluoride (Millipore) membranes for western immunoblot analysis (Western Star immunodetection system; Tropix) (see Carlezon et al. 1997).

Separate blots were conducted for each glutamate receptor subunit protein (GluR1: Chemicon, 1:1000; GluR2/3: Upstate Biotechnologies, Inc., 1:333; NMDAR1: Pharmingen, 1:1000), but TH (Chemicon, 1:1000) was analyzed on each membrane. The GluR2/3 antibody recognizes GluR2 almost exclusively, even though it is directed against a portion of the protein with some homology with GluR3 (see Prince et al. 1995). Equivalent protein loading was confirmed by amido black staining of the membranes. Levels of immunoreactivity were quantified using computer-assisted densitometry (MCID, Imaging Research Inc.), and between-group differences in relative optical density (signal minus background) for each protein were analyzed using Student's t-tests.

The portion of the brain anterior to the VTA (containing the MFB at the level of the LH) was placed in 4% paraformaldehyde solution. Sections (40 μM) were stained with cresyl violet to confirm electrode placements.

RESULTS

One rat (C6) in the C (non-stimulated) condition was euthanized before the completion of the experiment because its electrode became dislodged. No tissue was collected from this rat.

All rats in the E (stimulated) group rapidly learned to press, indicating that the brain stimulation was rewarding. The average daily Q received by each rat was 2939.4 (±257.6, S.E.M.) μC (Table 1) , which is well within the range of values obtained in previous studies (Bielajew and Trzcinska 1998). Electrode placements for each rat in the E group were located within the MFB at the level of the LH (Figure 1). Electrode placements for each rat in the C group were similarly situated in the MFB, at sites that would likely support self-stimulation (this was impossible to test in the present experiment).

Table 1 Average Daily Charge (Q) for Each Rat Exposed to Rewarding MFB Stimulation for 10 Days, and Group Mean
Figure 1
figure 1

The locations of LH sites for the control (C) and experimental (E) and groups. Distance posterior to bregma (mm) is indicated on the right. The circles (gray for C and black for E groups) localize the electrode tips, and the alphanumeric identifies the individual subjects. Although all electrodes were implanted in the right hemisphere, the groups are depicted separately for clarity (schematics from software [Neurographics—The Rat Brain] based on Paxinos and Watson 1986)

Repeated intermittent exposure to rewarding MFB stimulation (1hr/day × 10 days) dramatically and selectively decreased GluR1 expression in the VTA (t13 = 2.70, p < .02) to 49% of control levels (Figure 2A). On the same blot on which GluR1 expression was significantly decreased, TH expression was unchanged (t13 = 0.10, p = .91, n.s.) (Figure 2B). Rewarding MFB stimulation did not alter GluR2 (t13 = 1.04, p = .37, n.s.) (Figure 2C) or NMDAR1 (t13 = 0.60, p = .58, n.s.) expression (Figure 2D).

Figure 2
figure 2

Protein immunoblots of VTA. Data are presented as percentage (mean ± SEM) of protein expression in non-stimulated controls; —, non-stimulated; +, stimulated. Rewarding MFB stimulation decreases GluR1 expression (A) without affecting TH expression (B). The same two lanes from the same membrane are shown in (A) and (B). The stimulation did not alter expression of GluR2 (C) or NMDAR1 (D). **p < .02, Student's t-test

There was no correlation between Q (μC) and levels of GluR1 expression in the VTA (relative optical density) (F1,6 = 0.64, p = .45, n.s.) (Figure 3).

Figure 3
figure 3

Lack of correlation between the daily amount of charge (Q) (mean ± SEM) received by each rat and VTA levels of GluR1 expression (mean optical density ± SEM) for each rat in the stimulated group. Relative optical density was calculated for each subject by subtracting background from signal, and Q was obtained from Table 1. Dashed line represents mean optical density of non-stimulated group

DISCUSSION

Repeated intermittent exposure to rewarding MFB stimulation decreases GluR1 expression in the VTA, without any effect on expression of GluR2, NMDAR1, or TH. This effect of BSR on GluR1 expression is opposite to that caused by intermittent exposure to cocaine and morphine, which is known to increase GluR1 expression in the VTA (Fitzgerald et al. 1996; Churchill et al. 1999). Thus, these two types of rewarding stimuli (drugs and BSR) cause fundamentally different neuroadaptations in the brain reward system despite the observations that they share common neural substrates (i.e., the mesocorticolimbic dopamine system) and they have common effects on behavior (e.g., both stimulate activity and establish lever-pressing). These data suggest that the neural mechanisms of drug reward and brain stimulation reward are at least partially non-overlapping (see Arvanitogiannis et al. 1997; Hunt and McGregor 1998).

The downregulation of GluR1 expression appears correlated only with whether the rats received any rewarding stimulation, rather than with the amount of rewarding stimulation that each rat received. If decreased GluR1 expression was a non-specific effect of the stimulation itself, it would be expected that higher amounts of stimulation received (Q) would result in less GluR1 expression (optical density) in the VTA. This correlation was not demonstrated by the methods that we employed in the present studies. Because we used the minimal amount of stimulation that would support responding for each rat (see Methods), the rewarding effect of the stimulation was qualitatively similar for all of the rats, whereas the amount of stimulation received (Q) differed among the rats.

The lack of correlation between the amount of MFB stimulation received and the downregulation of GluR1 in the VTA raises the possibility that, instead, we might find a stronger correlation between decreased GluR1 expression and the actual rewarding value of the brain stimulation if we used more quantitative measurements to define reward strength. This question would be best addressed in future studies using the curve-shift variant of the BSR paradigm, in which it is possible to define the actual rewarding value of the stimulation with great precision. In the curve-shift paradigm, the rats are trained extensively over a wide range of stimulation parameters, producing essentially a “dose-response” analysis of the rewarding efficacy of the brain stimulation (each stimulation parameter represents a “dose”). Protein expression in the VTA could be compared among groups of rats self-administering low, intermediate, and high “doses” of rewarding MFB stimulation. It is important to note that the stimulation parameters that have low, intermediate, and high rewarding efficacy are different for each rat, and depend upon the exact placement of the electrode within the MFB; an amount of stimulation that supports maximal rates of responding in one rat may support only low rates in another rat. Therefore it is critical to show, as we have in the present studies, that protein expression in the VTA is not correlated merely with the amount of MFB stimulation received. Thus the present studies provide a rationale for performing more complex and labor-intensive studies involving the curve-shift paradigm.

Relative levels of GluR1 and GluR2 expression in the dopaminergic neuron-rich VTA are important because the subunit composition of AMPA receptors controls their function. High expression of GluR1 favors the formation of Ca2+-permeable (GluR1-homomeric) AMPA receptors (Hollmann and Heinemann 1994; Carlezon et al. 1997; Seeberg et al. 1998), which presumably increase sensitivity to the excitatory (depolarizing) effects of glutamate (see Neve et al. 1997). Conversely, high expression of GluR2 favors the formation of Ca2+-impermeable (hetereomeric) AMPA receptors, since this subunit contains a motif that blocks Ca2+ conductance (Hollmann and Heinemann 1994).

Repeated exposure to drugs of abuse selectively elevates GluR1 expression in the VTA (Fitzgerald et al. 1996; Churchill et al. 1999), and this change may explain, at least in part, the sensitized electrophysiological responses of VTA dopamine neurons seen in drug-exposed rats (Zhang et al. 1997). Furthermore, selective elevations in GluR1 expression in the rostral VTA (the predominant region probed in the present studies) by viral-mediated gene transfer can increase responsivity to the rewarding effects of morphine (Carlezon et al. 1997, 2000), establishing causal relations between elevated GluR1 expression in this region and sensitization. Drug-related elevations in GluR1 expression in the VTA, a region known to be involved in the induction of sensitization (Kalivas and Stewart 1991; Wolf, 1998), may themselves be sufficient to explain sensitization (Carlezon et al. 1997; Zhang et al. 1997), or they may lead to Ca2+-dependent adaptations (see Self and Nestler 1995) in the VTA or other regions that also contribute to changes in drug sensitivity (Kalivas and Stewart 1991; Wolf 1998).

The present data strengthen the hypothesized association between elevated GluR1 expression in the VTA and sensitization of reward. Decreased GluR1 expression without changes in GluR2 expression would be expected to favor formation of GluR2-containing, Ca2+-impermeable AMPA receptors in the VTA. Considering that increased GluR1 and/or Ca2+ flux in the VTA seems critical for the induction of sensitized drug responses (Carlezon et al. 1997; see also Wolf 1998), our data may provide an explanation for why repeated intermittent administration of drugs of abuse do not produce sensitization when tested in variants of the BSR paradigm that are sensitive to changes in reward strength (e.g., the “curve-shift” variant).

Acute administration of most drugs of abuse potentiates the rewarding strength of MFB stimulation (see Wise 1996). This effect indicates additivity between the rewarding effects of the stimulation and the rewarding effects of the drugs, and suggests that the drugs themselves are rewarding. However, the magnitude of the acute reward-related effects of cocaine (Frank et al. 1988; Bauco and Wise 1997), amphetamine (Wise and Munn 1993), morphine (Bauco and Wise 1993), PCP (Carlezon and Wise 1993), and nicotine (Bauco et al. 1994) does not change progressively with repeated intermittent drug treatment in the BSR paradigm. [There is a report in which brain stimulation pre-sensitizes rats to the locomotor-stimulating effects of amphetamine (Ben-Shahar and Ettenberg 1994), but this work involved direct rather than transynaptic stimulation of the VTA, which might result in different neuroadaptations.]

The lack of sensitization to drug reward in BSR procedures that use MFB stimulation is in stark contrast to findings in place conditioning and intravenous drug self-administration paradigms (Lett 1989; Piazza et al. 1990; Horger et al. 1990), which indicate that drug reward undergoes sensitization as the result of repeated exposure. This inconsistency among reward paradigms raises the possibility that pre-exposure to electrical stimulation of the MFB during BSR training causes adaptations that prevent further changes in the rewarding strength of the drugs. Our present data suggest that MFB stimulation causes a change (decreased GluR1 expression in the VTA) exactly opposite to that necessary for the induction and/or expression of sensitized responses to the rewarding effects of drugs of abuse (elevated GluR1 expression). Thus when given in combination with MFB stimulation, repeated intermittent drug administration may never result in the elevations in VTA GluR1 expression that are required to trigger sensitized responses to the rewarding effects of the drugs. This interaction can be addressed in future studies in which GluR1 expression is analyzed in the VTA of rats that receive drugs and rewarding MFB stimulation concurrently.

The molecular explanation for differences between the effects of rewarding MFB stimulation and drugs of abuse on GluR1 expression in the VTA is unknown. One important distinction between drugs and BSR is that drugs have pharmacological actions at receptor and/or transport proteins within brain reward circuitry, whereas the effects of BSR depend upon the electrically-stimulated release of endogenous transmitters. This difference would appear to be of little relevance, however, since stress (like drugs of abuse) also increases GluR1 expression in the VTA via release of endogenous transmitters (Fitzgerald et al. 1996). Regardless, the ability of rewarding MFB stimulation to decrease GluR1 expression selectively—at least among the proteins probed—in the VTA may offer a valuable method with which to address directly the role of VTA GluR1 in the behavioral, molecular, and electrophysiological correlates of sensitization. There are currently no described methods, other than antisense oligonucleotides injected directly into the VTA, with which to selectively decrease GluR1 expression in this region. Conceivably, the ability of rewarding MFB stimulation to decrease GluR1 expression in the VTA may ultimately provide a working molecular model of protection against sensitization to the incentive-motivational properties of drugs and, by extension (see Robinson and Berridge 1993), resistance to addiction.