Main

Traditional antipsychotics, such as haloperidol or fluphenazine, induce purposeless oral chewing movements in rats when they are administered chronically (Clow et al. 1980; Ellison et al. 1987; Waddington 1990; Glenthoj and Hemmingsen 1989). These dyskinesias appear gradually, and they occur in only some of the animals. Because characteristics of these neuroleptic-induced rat movements parallel certain features (although not all) of the human neuroleptic-induced dyskinetic syndrome tardive dyskinesia (TD), they are often used in its study.

General criteria for using an animal preparation as a model for a human condition have previously been proposed (McKinney and Bunney 1969) and refined (Weiss and Kilts 1995). Scientists have stressed that the most critical elements for comparison are similarities in etiology, phenomenology, biochemistry, and pharmacology between the animal and the human condition. The etiology of these rat oral dyskinesias (i.e., the chronic antipsychotic treatment) matches the etiology of TD, as do some of the demographic aspects of the rat movements, such as slow symptom onset, delayed offset, and jerky quality (Tamminga et al. 1990; Waddington 1990). Moreover, whereas traditional neuroleptics cause these purposeless movements in rats and tardive dyskinesia in humans, clozapine fails to produce dyskinesias in either rats or humans (Gunne et al. 1982; Kakigi et al. 1995).

The difficulty of distinguishing types of antipsychotic-induced movements in rats, along with the limited oral distribution of the movements in rats, and disagreements about the anticholinergic suppressability of the rat movements, have maintained a proper skepticism about using this preparation without reservation as an animal model of human TD. In this report, we have attempted to expand the pharmacology of these rat dyskinesias by testing whether two of the new antipsychotic drugs, olanzapine and sertindole, which have known low acute motor side effect profiles in humans, would induce purposeless oral chewing in rats.

Olanzapine is a new antipsychotic drug developed by Lilly Laboratories, Inc., a thio[1,5]benzo-diazepine (Bymaster et al. 1996). It has potent antipsychotic actions in schizophrenic psychosis, comparable to traditional neuroleptics, and is active against both positive and negative symptoms (Beasley et al. 1996). One of the unusual characteristics of olanzapine is its low potency in producing Parkinsonian side effects and akathisia in humans. Olanzapine is a clozapine-like drug with respect to its chemical structure, its broad receptor affinity profile (dopamine D1-D5, serotonin 5HT2A, 5HT2C, 5HT6,1α noradrenergic, muscarinic cholinergic, and histamine H1); moreover, it shows selective action on the mesolimbic (A10) versus the nigrostriatal (A9) dopamine neurons, with chronic dosing (Skarsfeldt 1995), fails to stimulate Fos protein in dorsal striatum (Robertson and Fibiger 1996), and does not cause catalepsy.

Sertindole is a new antipsychotic drug jointly developed by Abbott Laboratories and Lundbeck A/S, a phenyl indole derivative (Sanchez et al. 1991). Like olanzapine, it has potent antipsychotic actions in schizophrenic psychosis, on both positive and negative symptoms (Zimbroff et al. 1997). Similarly, one of the unusual characteristics of sertindole is its low potency in producing Parkinsonian side effects and akathisia in humans. The drugs is clozapine-like in its selective action on A10 (mesolimbic), compared to A9 (nigrostriatal) dopamine neurons (Skarsfeldt 1992); indeed, this was the selection principle used prospectively in its design. Consistent with this anatomic selectivity, sertindole fails to stimulate Fos protein in dorsal striatum (Fink-Jensen and Kristensen 1994), and does not cause catalepsy at relevant doses (Sanchez et al. 1991). Despite this, its chemical structure and receptor profile differ from that of clozapine, with a considerably narrower receptor affinity profile (the dopamine D2 family, serotonin 5HT2A, 5HT2C, and α1 noradrenergic receptors).

Based on the low incidence of Parkinsonism and akathisia with each of these antipsychotic drugs in clinical trials (Beasley et al. 1996; Zimbroff et al. 1997), and therefore, the potential that either drug might have a low incidence of TD with long-term use (Kane et al. 1986), we hypothesized that their potency in causing oral dyskinesias in rats with chronic dosing would also be low.

METHODS

Male Sprague–Dawley albino rats were divided into six treatment groups: Water ad libitum, n = 10; haloperidol, 1.5 mg/kg/day, n = 10; sertindole 0.5 mg/kg/day, n = 9; sertindole, 2.0 mg/kg/day, n = 10; olanzapine 0.5 mg/kg/day, n = 10; and olanzapine 2.0 mg/kg/day, n = 10. Because the doses of all the drugs are given in drinking water, they are administered gradually over the 24-hour period, not in a bolus. The dose of haloperidol was selected, because it has previously been used in our studies and reliably produces purposeless chewing movements (CMs). Moreover, we have previously demonstrated that this dose of haloperidol (1.5 mg/kg/day) produces rat drug plasma levels of 6.8 ± 1.1 ng/ml, which are in the clinical antipsychotic range (Gao et al. 1997). Plasma levels in this range in humans are associated with an incidence of TD of 5% per treatment year (Kane et al. 1986).

For the new neuroleptics, we attempted to pick two doses of each that were in the range of this dose of haloperidol based on clinical potency and animal pharmacology. The recommended clinical dose ranges of haloperidol, olanzapine, and sertindole for schizophrenic psychosis are 8 to 16 mg (VanPutten et al. 1991), 10 to 20 mg (Beasley et al. 1996), and 12 to 24 mg (Zimbroff et al. 1997), respectively, with more uncertainty about the latter two dose ranges than that for haloperidol. Oral bioavailability in rat is substantial for all three drugs. The dose range selected for the new antipsychotics (0.5 and 2.0 mg/kg/day) spanned this range relative to haloperidol; moreover, plasma drug levels of all three drugs were analyzed in rat trunk blood.

Each drug was administered in drinking water continuously for 6 months. The two test drugs, sertindole and olanzapine, were separately dissolved in a minimum volume of glacial acetic acid. Each solution was diluted with distilled water and adjusted to pH 5.5 to 6.0, using 10 N sodium hydroxide, to give stock solutions of 33.3 mg/100 ml. The stocks were diluted every 7 days to 3.3 mg/100 ml drug solution for the high-dose groups, and to 0.833 mg/100 ml drug solution for the low-dose groups. Rats were housed two per cage; drug dosage was calculated (weekly initially; monthly in last months) by the amount of water drunk, assuming equal drinking between the two animals. Body weight was monitored. Haloperidol dosing in this strategy has been previously verified by us (Gao et al. 1997); preclinical scientists familiar with the pharmacology of olanzapine and sertindole verified the dosing strategy for the two new drugs (J. Arnt, personal communication; R. Lahti, personal communication). Drug concentration in the drinking water was adjusted as needed, through monitoring water intake, to maintain the target daily dose. After 6 months of continuous drug administration, rats were sacrificed by decapitation. Trunk blood was collected at sacrifice for measurement of plasma drug levels.

The animal preparation utilized here has been previously examined in detail in this laboratory. Under the conditions of this experiment in our laboratory, male Sprague–Dawley rats have a gradual onset of movements with haloperidol treatment. The rate of CMs after onset of haloperidol treatment, and the offset of movements, and the effects a single 5-mg acute dose of atropine on the CMs are given in Table 1 (Tamminga et al. 1990). Both Long–Evans animals and Wistar rats behaved differently from Sprague–Dawley rats with respect to these characteristics. After this initial descriptive experiment, we have applied this same metholodology in several subsequent experiments to test the pharmacologic characteristics of this animal paradigm (Shirakawa and Tamminga 1994; Kakigi et al. 1995; Gao et al. 1994; Gao and Tamminga 1996). We assume that the careful duplication of this animal preparation procedure within our laboratory will reproduce the behavioral characteristics of this paradigm.

Table 1 Characteristics of Haloperidol (Hal)-Induced CMs (# per 2 min): Time to Onset, Offset; Atropine Effect (Tamminga et al. 1990)

Purposeless oral movements were assessed as previously described (Gunne et al. 1982; Tamminga et al. 1990). The CMs were counted for two 5-minute periods by two trained raters blind to treatment group, with their ratings averaged; they were assessed at baseline, then monthly with treatment, and twice in the final treatment month. The two raters were the same throughout the study. The two end ratings were averaged for the final score at study end. Where ratings differed by more than 10%, a third rating was done later, and all three were averaged. Rat CMs were analyzed in two ways: by a simple average of rat CM scores across treatment groups; and by a dichotomy into high-CM and low-CM groups. We have identified a score of 8 CMs/5 min by mixture analysis as the optimal criterion for dichotomization into high and low CMs (Hashimoto et al. 1998), and applied this criterion prospectively. We have previously argued that the high-CM group may be TD-like.

Trunk blood was collected at the time of sacrifice, and the plasma was frozen at −20°C until analysis. Haloperidol was quantified using a modification of the method of Bianchetti and Morselli (1978), using chlorohaloperidol as the internal standard and simultaneously determining reduced haloperidol in the laboratory of Thomas Cooper (Nathan Klein Institute). Olanzapine levels were analyzed by Lilly Research Laboratories using high-performance liquid chromatography (HPLC) with electrochemical detection (Method LC-FID-PM-P-21, 1995). Sertindole levels were analyzed by Abbott Laboratories using an ethyl acetate: hexane (80:20) extract of the samples followed by tandem mass spectrometric detection of the reconstituted extract (Assay S96-002, NB 26262, 1995).

A one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with Dunnett's post-hoc test was used to analyze the data. First, the placebo group was used as the control group, and then the haloperidol group was the designated control group for the Dunnett's analysis. Because of the potential influence of outliers, the analyses were repeated with nonparametric comparisons. The results were the same. In addition, a chi-square analysis was carried out with the frequency data with significance tested using the two-tailed Fisher's exact test. Effect sizes were calculated for each test drug group in comparison to haloperidol.

RESULTS

Incidence of Purposeless Chewing Movements

After 6 months of treatment, water alone did not result in any “high” CM animals in the control animals; whereas, haloperidol induced these oral movements in 60% of the animals. The two doses of olanzapine both induced a 10% incidence of high CMs. The two doses of sertindole induced a 0% (lower dose) and 10% (higher dose) incidence of high CMs in the treated animals, respectively (Figure 1 ). The chi-square analysis of all six groups comparing high versus low CMs was significant at p = .00184. Individual comparisons between the test drug dose groups and haloperidol using chi-square analysis showed a strong trend for a difference between each dose of olanzapine and haloperidol and the lower dose of sertindole and haloperidol (each comparison, p = .057); whereas, the higher dose of sertindole was significantly different from haloperidol (p = .011). The effect sizes for the difference between each test drug group and haloperidol are moderate-to-large and are specified in Table 2.

Figure 1
figure 1

The percentage of animals in each treatment group with high levels of purposeless chewing movements (>8 CM/5 min) is shown in the open columns and keyed on the left vertical axis. The open circles represent the individual levels of CM for each individual rat in each treatment group and are keyed on the right vertical axis. In the haloperidol-treated animals, both the percentage high CM and individual CM were higher than the control group. Neither measure was different from control in the any of the other treament groups

Table 2 Plasma Drug Concentrations

Rates of Purposeless Chewing Movements and Plasma Drug Levels

Average rates of CMs for all treatment groups are given in Table 2 and graphed individually in the figure. Using ANOVA, CM rates in the haloperidol group differed from placebo (p < .05), but CM rates of the study drugs did not differ from placebo at either dose level using Dunnett's post-hoc test. Sertindole at its lower dose produced significantly fewer CMs than haloperidol (p < .05).

Plasma drug concentrations in each drug treatment group are indicated in Table 2. The average haloperidol plasma concentration in the rat plasma is within the human therapeutic range for haloperidol (4–16 ng/ml). The same is true for olanzapine (10–20 ng/ml) and for sertindole (30–100 ng/ml), although the therapeutic range of each of these drugs is less well established.

DISCUSSION

These data show that, in contrast to the traditional antipsychotic drug haloperidol, the two new antipsychotics, olanzapine and sertindole, do not produce oral dyskinesias in rats in the high-CM range, when administered over 6 months. Neither new drug produced CM rates significantly different from placebo, and their incidence of high CMs was significantly different from haloperidol or showed a strong trend for this effect (p = .057); moreover, the effect sizes of the test drug group comparisons to haloperidol are substantial. The motor response produced by these two new drugs mimics results with chronic clozapine administration in rats (Gunne et al. 1982; Kakigi et al. 1995). It is not entirely surprising that these two drugs demonstrate the same action on oral dyskinesias as clozapine, based upon their similar regional depolarization blockade of dopamine neurons, Fos activation pattern, and clinical motor side effect profile, despite their very different receptor affinity profiles (Bymaster et al. 1996; Sanchez et al. 1991).

The preclinical characteristic shared by olanzapine and sertindole that may predict their low motor side effect profile in human studies is their anatomic selectivity demonstrated in the depolarization blockade model (Grace et al. 1997) and using Fos protein activation (Robertson et al. 1994). Both olanzapine and sertindole relatively selectively affect dopamine neurons that innervate the limbic and frontal cortices (the A10 dopamine neurons) and leave relatively unaffected those dopamine neurons involved in the striatum (the A9 dopamine neurons) (Skarsfeldt 1995). Although the full meaning of this model has been debated, the preclinical data indicate a preferential corticolimbic localization of drug action and a limited action in regions of striatum associated primarily with motor function. We, as well as others, have proposed that this selective action is the basis for clozapine's low incidence of acute motor side effects and tardive dyskinesia in humans (Gao et al. 1997). Moreover, as with clozapine, in the nonhuman primate, neither olanzapine nor sertindole causes acute dystonic reactions in the neuroleptic-primed monkeys at dose levels comparable to human treatment doses (Casey 1996). The primate data suggest that, at effective antipsychotic doses, clozapine, olanzapine, and sertindole do not act strongly enough on CNS motor pathways to produce acute motor side effects; therefore, these drugs might lack late motor side effects as well.

Clinical data from long-term studies with olanzapine suggest that treatment-emergent dyskinesias may occur at a significantly lower rate with olanzapine than with haloperidol. Several blinded extension protocols attached to Lilly's multicenter olanzapine versus haloperidol efficacy studies included 904 previously neuroleptic-treated schizophrenic individuals participating for 6 to 150 weeks (median drug exposure with haloperidol was 203 days; with olanzapine, 237 days). At study start, participants did not evidence any dyskinesias; at study end, 4.6% of the group of 197 haloperidol-treated patients demonstrated treatment-emergent dyskinesias by the strictest criteria, and 1% of the 707 olanzapine-treated subjects demonstrated the dyskinesias (Tollefson et al. 1997). Thus, the rate of “treatment-emergent” dyskinesias in rats with olanzapine as compared to haloperidol is consistent with this piece of clinical information about the relative incidence of neuroleptic-induced dyskinesias with olanzapine in humans.

Based not on the receptor profile of these two drugs, but on their preclinical pharmacologic property of selective A10 dopamine neuronal action and distribution of Fos protein activation, we predicted a low incidence of oral dyskinesias in rats with chronic treatment. We have confirmed this prediction; moreover, the low dyskinesia rate in rats with both new drugs correlates with a low rate of acute motor side effects with acute human use and with early clinical information of low treatment-emergent dyskinesia incidence with olanzapine.

These pieces of data, considered together, can support considerable optimism about the low motor side effect profile of these two new antipsychotic drugs in human use. Definitive clinical studies have already demonstrated their low acute Parkinsonism profile. The rat oral dyskinesia study reported here predicts a low TD incidence with their chronic use, to the extent that this rat condition can be seen to model human TD. Future prospective TD incidence studies with olanzapine and sertindole will test this prediction.