This page has been archived and is no longer updated

 
August 26, 2013 | By:  Sarah Jane Alger
Aa Aa Aa

Some City Birds Are Changing Their Tune

The human world population has climbed to over 7.1 billion people and for the first time ever, more than half of us live in an urban area. Urban areas are spreading and more animals are either getting pushed out or are becoming urbanized in the process. Birds are among the many species we are used to seeing and hearing in our cities, but how exactly are they being affected by urban spread?

Urban areas have dramatically different ecologies compared to their rural counterparts: the loss of native vegetation decreases many available habitats and food sources while the increase of architecture and human food waste provides new habitats and food sources. The prevalence of pavement, metal and glass raises temperatures. And for animals that use sound to communicate, urban environments have much louder background noise to compete with. Many ecologists and animal behaviorists have looked at aspects of how bird behavior, survival, and population dynamics are affected by the sounds of urbanization, but the results have been highly variable. Dr. Hans Slabbekoorn from Leiden University in the Netherlands recently reviewed the evidence to find patterns and make sense of it all.

The rapid environmental changes of urbanization typically cause declines and local extinctions of native species. However, among birds, some species have been found to decline while others persist, and still others newly arrive from other areas. This may sound like overall there is no net effect, but the reality is that the pattern of which species go, stay, or move in creates a phenomenon called the "urban homogenization of avifauna" (In other words, most cities have lots of the same bird species). Some species, like wood pigeons, rock doves, European blackbirds, European starlings and house sparrows have been notoriously successful at exploiting the resources of urban environments and have moved in and out-competed many of the native species. Compared to the species getting pushed out, these urban-dwelling species tend to have proportionally large brains, a wide range of diet and nesting choices, and low fear of humans. But one of the greatest factors that predicts a species' ability to adapt to an urban environment is its behavioral flexibility.

This great tit clearly has a low fear of humans.

Image by Paweł Kuźniar at Wikimedia.

.

Acoustic communication is very important to most bird species. They use it to defend territories, attract mates, stay in touch with their partners and offspring, and know when to feed their offspring. If these communication signals are drowned out by traffic, construction, industry, and noisy people, you can see how this could reduce their ability to successfully reproduce, which would in turn reduce population sizes. This is exactly what the data shows for most (but not all) bird species that have been studied.

City noises are generally louder at lower frequencies (also known as pitch) than at higher frequencies. Not all birds sing or call at the same frequencies, so we would expect birds that rely more on lower frequencies to communicate to be more adversely affected by city noise. The data shows us that species that have the lowest frequency songs are in fact the most negatively affected by urban noise.

Within some species, such as great tits, song sparrows, and silvereyes, populations that live in urban areas tend to sing at higher frequencies than populations that live in nearby wooded areas. Furthermore, the louder the urban noise, the higher the frequency these birds tend to use for their songs. But not all species make this adaptive shift. A recent comparative study found that song frequency increases with urban noise levels especially among species that learn their song from other group members. This ability to learn songs gives species an advantage when it comes to rapidly adjusting behavior in a rapidly changing environment. Among some species (like great tits and house finches), animals can do this at the individual level. That is to say, when the urban noise levels increase, an individual bird can shift to using more and longer high frequency songs, or can increase the frequency of their low frequency notes within a song.

Young house finches learn their songs from the adults around them.

Image by Susan Rachlin of U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service Northeast Region (USFWS) at Wikimedia.

.

Some species may also make this shift at the population level. Many bird species learn their songs from others in the group. If others in the group are singing higher frequency songs, or if learners cannot hear the lower frequency notes, then the next generation are going to learn to sing at higher frequencies. They will pass that higher frequency song on to their offspring in a phenomenon called cultural evolution. If the ability to learn and adjust song frequencies has genetically derived features, then this process can also be influenced by genetic evolution.

We still have a lot left to learn about how birds adjust their songs and more generally, how animals adjust their behavior in response to urbanization. Most of the studies out there are looking at correlations, so we can't say for sure the cause of these patterns. More experiments are needed to determine exactly how urban noise is changing the birdsongs around us. Are you up for the challenge?

--

Further reading:
Slabbekoorn, H. Songs of the city: noise-dependent spectral plasticity in the acoustic phenotype of urban birds, Animal Behaviour, 85, 1089-1099 (2013). DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.01.021

Francis, C.D. and Chadwick, M.A. What makes a species synurbic? Applied
Geography, 32, 514-521 (2012). DOI: 10.1016/j.apgeog.2011.06.013

Ríos-Chelén, A. A., Salaberria, C., Barbosa, I., Macías Garcia, C. & Gil, D. The
learning advantage: bird species that learn their song show a tighter adjustment
of song to noisy environments than those that do not learn. Journal of
Evolutionary Biology, 25, 2171-2180 (2012), DOI: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2012.02597.x

Halfwerk, W. & Slabbekoorn, H. A behavioural mechanism explaining noise-dependent frequency use in urban birdsong. Animal Behaviour, 78, 1301-1307 (2009). DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.09.015

Bermudez-Cuamatzin, E., Ríos-Chelén, A. A., Gil, D. and Garcia, C. M. Experimental evidence for real-time song frequency shift in response to urban noise in a passerine bird. Biology Letters, 7, 36-38 (2010). DOI: 10.1098/rsbl.2010.0437


Image Credits:
Sich_plustender_Star.JPG by 4028mdk09 at Wikimedia Commons.

Parus_major_in_flight_01.jpg by Paweł Kuźniar at Wikimedia Commons.

Carpodacus_mexicanus_-John_Heinz_National_Wildlife_Refuge_at_Tinicum,_Pennsylvania,_USA-8.jpg by Susan Rachlin of U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service Northeast Region (USFWS) at Wikimedia Commons.

5 Comments
Comments
August 29, 2013 | 05:14 PM
Posted By:  Sarah Jane Alger
Thanks for the references, Kenneth! I was aware of the breadth of research on experimentally-caused hearing loss in birds. But the reference on self-induced hearing loss from 1875 is really cool!
August 29, 2013 | 11:49 AM
Posted By:  Kenneth Schneider
Here is an entertaining reference on hearing loss in birds dating back to 1875. Bird hunters take notice:

http://www.jstor.org/discover/10.2307/4079207?uid=3739656&uid=2&uid=4&uid=3739256&sid=21102563205051

More serious stuff--

ONCE DEAF, BIRD BRAINS CAN’T KEEP A TUNE:

http://www.futurity.org/once-deaf-bird-brains-can%E2%80%99t-keep-a-tune/

Actually, birds can regenerate ears damaged by loud noises, and this has important research implications for treatment of human hearing loss. There are quite a few references-- see:

http://depts.washington.edu/drrpt/research/hearingloss.htm

http://www.deafnessresearch.org.uk/content/news-and-media/latest-news/-the-regenerative-power-of-birds--could-it-work-for-hearing-loss-in-humans/
August 29, 2013 | 02:21 AM
Posted By:  Sarah Jane Alger
Wow! Great comments!

Kenneth: I have been studying starling song for many years now and although we have caught them at airports, I have never considered loss of hearing... as far as I know, no one has. Definitely an avenue for new research!

Ilona: Also a great question and again, I don't think it has been looked at. It would be hard (but not impossible) to tease apart the effects of sound degradation from increased vigilance.
August 27, 2013 | 06:41 PM
Posted By:  Kenneth Schneider
Starlings that nest on the jetways at DFW Airport are exposed to extreme noise, yet they successfully rear their young. Why don't they lose their hearing? Given the importance of vocalization and hearing, how do they maintain normal behavior in this environment.? Fascinating!
August 27, 2013 | 05:35 PM
Posted By:  Ilona Miko
Hi Kate, Nice post. You've hit on a favorite topic of mine-how urban environments affect biology. I wonder if the changes you describe about birds shifting to higher frequencies affects how birds move about and group, since higher frequencies tend to have less impact at longer distance (amplitude degrades over a distance at greater rate than low frequencies do). Are there any data about changes of bird distribution or clustering in urban environments? Clustering more closely, mating more closely?
Blogger Profiles
Recent Posts

« Prev Next »

Connect
Connect Send a message

Scitable by Nature Education Nature Education Home Learn More About Faculty Page Students Page Feedback



Blogs