Abstract
The uniquely enlarged noses of male proboscis monkeys are prominent adornments, and a sexually selected male trait. A recent study showed significant correlations among nose, body, and testis sizes and clear associations between nose size and the number of females in a male’s harem. However, to date, the analyses of other common male traits, i.e., canines, are lacking. Whereas male nose size had a positive correlation with body size, we unexpectedly found a negative correlation between body and canine sizes. We explain this by an interaction between sexual and natural selection. Larger noses in males may interfere with the use of canines, thereby reducing their effectiveness as weapons. Additionally, longer canines are opposed by natural selection because the larger gape it imposes upon its bearer reduces foraging efficiency, particularly in folivores. This unique case of decoupling of body and canine size reveals that large canines carry an ecological cost.
Introduction
Darwinian evolutionary theory explains many male-specific characteristics as a consequence of sexual selection1. Large canines in some mammals, including primates, are a typical example of a weapon used in contests with conspecific male rivals2. By contrast, ornaments are not involved in combat and instead act as a partner attraction signal (ultimately determined by female choice). However, some male-specific traits are neither weapons nor ornaments, and serve as a badge of male social status, advertising competitive potential with respect to other males1,3, particularly as observed for social rank-dependent masculine traits in some primate species4. Additionally, such badges of status may be related to animal social recognition5; status badges may be beneficial in large/complex societies like primate multilevel societies, in which two or more levels of organization are recognizable. A status badge would therefore provide a useful solution in lieu of individual identification for acquiring an understanding of the relative dominance of conspecifics6,7.
Proboscis monkeys (Nasalis larvatus) are a typical sexually dimorphic primate with male-specific enlarged noses that are prominent adornments, which have been linked with their sexually competitive social system of one-male groups, suggestive of a multilevel social system8. Recently, we reported significant positive correlations among nose, body, and testis size as well as a clear association between nose size and both nasalized acoustic signals and the number of females in harem groups; this supports the hypothesis that larger noses audiovisually advertise a male’s fighting ability with conspecifics9, and thus serve as status badges rather than ornaments.
To date, an analysis of another common sexually selected male trait, i.e., canine size, is lacking. Sexually dimorphic male traits (e.g., body mass, nose, testis, and canines) develop after sexual maturation, primarily in response to endocrine changes in the levels of androgens such as testosterone10,11,12. Comparisons of these male traits would enable us to study the relationships among weapons, badges, ornaments, and testes. In this study, we compared canine size among previously investigated wild proboscis monkeys9. Notably, proboscis monkey harem groups (and bachelor groups) aggregate on a regular basis at their sleeping sites, and female dispersal between the groups is typical13 in their multilevel society8, creating the potential for both female choice and male–male competition for females. Therefore, male canines are expected to serve as important weapons for mate competition or defense, especially because canine size generally reflects the strength of intra-sexual selection more directly than body size in interspecific comparisons2,14. Together with the evaluations by measuring the body traits, we attempted to test whether the development of canine size is replicated by a mathematical simulation with minimal assumptions, and search for the plausible hypothesis to explain the multiple sets of the biological properties of the proboscis monkeys that are often exposed to the natural and sexual selections.
Results
Relationships among physical traits
The best-fitting model included both male nose and canine size, although the fit of the model including only canine size was almost as good (i.e., ΔAICc < 2.0: Table 1). Whereas nose size was positively correlated with body mass, the model with canine size was unexpectedly negative (Fig. 1). As the values for the young/old adult males were within the range of the ones for the “averaged” adult males (Fig. 1), we, therefore, believe that age/wear of each individual male would not contribute greatly to the relationship between body size and canine size. However, due to the limited sample size, further analysis using a larger data set would be necessary to reconfirm these results in a future study. Conversely, for females, the best-fitting model only included nose size, which was positively associated with body mass. The second-best model was the null model (Table 1). The difference in the regression slopes and intercepts for body size vs. nose size between the sexes was not significant (slope: t = −1.41, P = 0.17; t = 0.28; intercept: P = 0.78). However, the difference in the regression intercept for body size vs. canine size was significant (t = 8.28, P < 0.0001), although the slopes were not different (t = −0.33, P = 0.74). Previous results illustrated that secondary sexual proboscis monkey male traits reflect both pre- and post-copulatory competition; specifically, larger noses serve as both “female attraction” and “badge of status,” and simultaneously, larger testes reflect sperm competition9. Body size also contributes to both male–male competition and female attraction.
Nose size was positively and significantly correlated with body mass for both sexes a, though a significant negative correlation between body and canine size was found only in males but not in females b. Noted that sex and estimated age/class category is indicated in different shape and colour symbol, respectively.
Numerical simulation
Figure 2 shows the simulated results of developmental growth curves for harem alpha status males, non-alpha status males, and females with various cost and termination time parameters. Our simulations revealed that higher costs caused by canine development negatively influenced body mass growth in males. Male body mass also varied depending on the termination time of canines, i.e., canine size, whereas female body mass was relatively independent of canine size. In the case of no or weak costs attributable to canine development, body mass for both sexes was independent of the canine growth termination time, but under the assumption of a large cost for canine development, canine size had a clear negative influence on body mass, particularly in males (Fig. 3).
The cost parameters were set to 0.01 (weak), 0.5 (moderate), or 0.99 (high). The termination of canine development (represented as “t_1” in figures) was set to 0.51 (immediate termination), 0.75 (middle), or 0.99 (maximum canine growth) for males and 0.51 (immediate termination), 0.625 (middle), or 0.74 (maximum canine growth) for females.
Discussion
The results presented in this study can be interpreted in two ways. The minimal interpretation is that canine size does not contribute to the fighting ability of harem-holding males, unlike the findings in other primate species forming harem societies2. Battles featuring fatal physical attacks using canines are commonly observed in typical harem species such as hanuman langurs (Semnopithecus entellus)15 or howler monkeys (Alouatta seniculus)16. Nonetheless, the benefit of a larger body size in proboscis monkey males is clear. Indeed, branch shaking is commonly used to keep groups apart8, and in chases without physical contact, males repeatedly exhibit such displays accompanied by leaps between trees13,17. Additionally, among proboscis monkey males, nose size, which is highly correlated with body size, determines the formant frequencies of loud vocalizations, which may decide contests without any direct physical competition, or at least direct biting9,13,18. Thus, a larger body, which contributes to male athleticism, would allow more effective displays of fighting ability, whereas nose size would be a more suitable proxy to represent fighting ability because it is easier to assess via vocalization as opposed to visual examination. Consequently, males might invest their physiological resources to develop nose, body, and testis size more than canine weapons, in line with the trade-off phenomenon recently reported in other harem primates, e.g., Dunn et al.19.
Contrarily, whereas females do not compete through sound as do males, they unexpectedly exhibited the same nose size/body size correlation, suggesting that female nose size is linked to the effect on males; positive selection on larger male nose size may be driving larger noses in females. Female noses are probably sufficiently small to avoid interfering with their activity (e.g., feeding, moving, social behaviors, etc.), and thus, selection toward sex-differentiated expression does not occur. In other respects, it has been found that secondary sexual characteristics, i.e., ornaments or weapons, have a positive allometric slope concerning body size in a number of animal species20,21, a positive correlation between nose size and body size in either sex would just be a natural phenomenon and requires no complex such explanation.
However, the significantly negative correlation between male canine and body size supports an alternative interpretation; namely, it suggests an ecological cost of large canines, as suggested for the taxonomic variation in canine size observed across primate lineages22,23. The prominent nose may interfere with effective canine bites, thereby reducing their efficacy as weapons. Moreover, given their reduced effectiveness, natural selection may actually favor smaller canines because larger canines increase gape, which reduces bite force and thus mastication efficiency22. This foraging cost may be especially severe in large-sized folivores, such as proboscis monkeys and gorillas23. Thus, a negative correlation might evolve in the rate of post-adolescent growth of the size of the body frame and that of canines. We therefore predict that in proboscis monkeys, body-size dependent displays are more effective at greater distances than the facial displays commonly observed in most other primates24.
Contrary to the significant negative slope for the male canine size–body size correlation, we only found a weak negative correlation for the female slope. Considering lower incidence of agonistic interaction, either using canines and leaping between trees, with weak or absence of linearity in hierarchy among females within groups13,25, such canine size–body size correlation may be less prominent in females than that in males. Although the difference of the slopes between the sexes was not significant, possibly because of the limited sample size, the significant sex difference in the intercept for body size vs. canine size suggests that canine size has a different relationship with body mass between males and females. The difference in the sex-differentiated pattern between noses and canines would confirm the hypothesis that large canines interfere with effective biting in males, making them less effective as weapons and thus leading to negative selection on canine size in the largest males.
Concerning the mechanism underlying the trade-off among body mass, nose size, and canine size, we predict that canine growth would actually exhibit a negative correlation possibly with testosterone levels, opposite to the common and ancestral situation19. To test a part of this “costly canine hypothesis”, we conducted preliminary mathematical simulations considering body mass development with a growth constrained by canine size. Echoing our empirical findings, the negative relationship between body and canine size was observed only in males when we assumed a large cost of canine development (Fig. 3); noting that the model did not consider the possible basal factors such as testosterone level which might fundamentally determine the maturation patterns of the sexual traits, due to no available data for the initial parameter proposals. Our study, however, raises interesting questions regarding the developmental mechanism that produces a negative correlation among adult body, nose size, and canine size. Future detailed studies focusing on the behavior and development system of proboscis monkeys are therefore required.
Methods
Ethics statement
Permission to capture and handle proboscis monkeys was granted by the Sabah Biodiversity Centre (permit JKM/MBS.1000-2/2 JLD.3 (73)) and was carried out in accordance with the current laws of Malaysia, Sabah Wildlife Department’s Standard Operation Procedures on Animal Capture, Anaesthesia and Welfare, the Weatherall report26, and the guidelines for non-human primates as described by Unwin et al.27. Once an risk assessment was conducted on the target individual and its surrounding sleeping site area, darting was performed by veterinarians experienced in the capture and anesthesia of wildlife using Zoletil 100 (Tiletamine + Zolazepam; 6 ± 10 mg/kg), Anaesthesia and the vital signs were monitored throughout the procedure28, and once the procedure was complete, each animal was given a prophylactic dose of Alamycine LA (20 mg/kg) and Ivermectine (0.2 mg/kg) as a preventative measure to assist in the post-anesthesia recovery.
Data collection
Between July 2011 and December 2016, we captured 28 free-ranging adult proboscis monkeys in the Lower Kinabatangan Floodplain, Sabah, Borneo, Malaysia (5°18′N to 5°42′N and 117°54′E to 18°33′E). To reduce the impact of capturing on the animal’s social system, we captured all study subjects during the night28. While animals were anaesthetized, we performed in situ measurements for their body parts using a scale (body mass) and caliper (nose size and canine length). In this study, we additionally included data for maxillary canine length to those on body mass and nose size (length × width) for the same 18 males obtained by Koda et al.9 as well as data for 10 females’ canine length, body mass, and nose size. Note that canine length refers to the apex to base measurement (the height of the crown), and the maxillary canine length was measured because of its larger size and greater importance in behavioral displays and weaponry29. All study subjects were adults, i.e., 18 harem-holding males and 10 females including one pregnant and two lactating individuals (see Supplementary Table 1 for further detailed study subject information). The veterinarian in the sampling team attempted in situ age/class estimation for the study subjects based on the three categories of wear level of their molars, i.e., low: young adult; medium: adult; high: old adult, although the estimation was rather rough due the difficult in situ condition in the forest at night, e.g., limited tools, light, and time the animal remained anaesthetized.
Data analysis
We used a linear regression model to establish whether body mass (response variable) was related to other physical properties such as nose and canine size (explanatory variables). To obtain a linear dimension comparable to the canine dimension, body mass and nose size were cube root- and square root-transformed, respectively, to generate response and explanatory variables. The variance inflation factors were 1.00 for both nose size and body mass, indicating that collinearity among independent factors would not affect the results30. We examined a set of models with all possible combinations of the explanatory variables and ranked them according to the corrected version of the Akaike information criterion (AIC) for small sample sizes, called AICc31. Following guidelines published for wildlife research, we selected models with ΔAICc ≤ 2, where ΔAICc = AICc−minimum AICc within the candidate model set31. Analysis of covariance served to compare the slopes and intercepts of regression lines between the sexes. We performed these analyses using R ver. 3.3.2 (ref. 32).
Mathematical model
The aim of the simulations was to examine our hypothesis regarding the positive/negative correlations between body mass and canine size. Earlier work distinguished the subadult class from fully adult males based on nasal maturation as well as a fully developed body size9. In other words, two developmental stages likely exist among sexually mature males. Therefore, before the acquisition of harem status, subadult males reach a limit in body mass, which cannot increase without a status change. By contrast, in females, the development of body mass and canines is basically similar to that of males before the acquisition of harem status. In this study, we therefore assumed a two-stage model of male development, i.e., a first stage with primary development of body and canine size for both males and females and a second stage that only applies to males and only after the acquisition of harem status.
Primary development of body and canine size
Body mass m develops under the assumption that the growth rate depends on the body mass at the time, until reaching its developmental limit K within a fixed time T, i.e.,
where a = const is the shape factor determining the sigmoidal curve of development. This is based on a previously proposed basic model for body mass development20,33 but with our modification to simplify the constraint of K. By contrast, the canines develop in the later stage of body mass development, and thus, we supposed that larger canines would reduce feeding efficiency, resulting in a growth rate reduction for body mass as follows:
where cz is the reducing factor determined by the cost factor \(c \in [0,1)\) and the size of canines z and cs < 1 always holds. For model simplification, the canines start to develop at the time t0 and linearly develop until t1 ≤ T as follows:
where α > 0 is the growth rate of canines. Consequently, the differential equation of body mass in \(t \in [0,T],T \, > \, t_1\) is
where \(cz_1: = cz_{t = t_1} = c\alpha (t_1 - t_0)\), because proboscis monkeys likely increase their body mass until t = T, with the constant of the reducing factor, after terminating canine growth (t = t1). Note that \(0 \le z \le \alpha (T - t_0)\) holds; therefore,
also holds.
In the case of t < t0, i.e., before the onset of canine eruption, the equation is a simple logistic equation; therefore, the solution is
where A0 is the constant determined by the initial values of m(0) = m0 > 0 as follows:
Once the canines begin to develop, i.e., in the case of t0 ≤ t ≤ t1, the differential equations are solved as follows:
Then, after terminating canine development (t = t1), we found that the solution is simply
Note that the following equations must be satisfied:
Consequently, the basic models for body mass are
and those for canine size are
Rank-dependent secondary development of body and nose size
Our previous study suggested that males who acquire harem alpha status develop their noses as a badge of status in coordination with body mass9. In this study, we supposed that alpha status males can dominate both copulation opportunities and foraging resources because harem groups with larger males better defend resources from bachelor groups generally consisting of smaller males34. Therefore, such alpha status males would possibly continue their body mass growth after completing their primary developmental process, whereas males who fail to acquire harem status terminate body mass growth. Then, the differential equations of the body mass in the secondary development for harem males are equivalent with Eq. (4), i.e.,
whereas those for non-harem males are
where t2 is the termination time of the body mass growth. This equation is solved in the same manner as Eq. (8), namely
where \(\beta := \frac{{a(1 - c\alpha t_1 + c\alpha t_0)}}{{t_2 - T}}\). Based on our findings of allometric development of ornaments and body size in free-ranging specimens20,33, we supposed that nose size is primary determined by body mass.
Numerical simulations
Overview of aims
We conducted the numerical simulations with the aim to clarify the mechanism by which the termination time of canine development t1 determines the final results of body mass and canine size. We simulated the growth pattern based on several deterministic parameters, considering the developmental evidence for proboscis monkeys. Additionally, we tried to apply the model for both males and females within the same developmental frameworks, considering the sex differences of the developmental parameters as follows.
Parameter proposals
First, the time parameter was normalized at the male’s primary maturation time T, i.e., T:= 1. Therefore, our time parameters t0, t1, and t2, are the time relative to the male maturation time T. By definition, \(0 \le t_0 \le t_1 \le T = 1 \le t_2\) holds. Additionally, the maximum canine size z was also normalized to 1 for the case in which the monkey maximally develops its canines during the periods between t0 and T = 1. Following Eq. (5), \(\alpha = \frac{1}{{1 - t_0}}\).
Next, we supposed that the primary maturation age of males (t = T = 1) at which males reach sexual maturity at approximately 8 years old35, although male nose enlargement does not typically start until that time36. In Cercopithecus37 and proboscis monkeys38, canine development starts at approximately 4 years old and continues until 8 years old. The onset time of canine development was therefore set at 4 years old in our model, or half the subadult maturation time, i.e., \(t_0 = \frac{1}{2}\). By contrast, females mature earlier than males. We supposed that the female maturation age was 6 years35; i.e., we used \({{T}} = \frac{6}{8}\) in the model for female development. The time of onset of canine growth is likely similar in both sexes, but females likely terminate canine growth earlier, corresponding to their earlier physical maturation. Therefore, we supposed that female canines develop from approximately 4 years old until 6 years old, representing a shorter than observed in males (i.e., \(t_0 = \frac{1}{2}\) for females).
Records for body mass were used for the parameters in the simulations. Proboscis monkey neonatal body mass was reported as 0.45 kg38, and the maximum body mass among our specimens was 25.5 kg for males and 14.5 kg for females. Therefore, we set m(0) = 0.45, Kmale = 25.5, and Kfemale = 14.5. Additionally, we used the body mass record at approximately 4 years old, or the onset time of canine eruption, which was approximately 6.5 kg (N = 3, records in Japan Monkey Centre). Therefore, we used \(m(\frac{1}{2}) = 6.5\) in the simulations for both males and females. Finally, we assumed that body mass development would terminate immediately after the maturation time T was reached; therefore, we used t2 = T + 0.1 = 1.1 for males and t2 = T + 0.1 = 0.85 for females.
Based on the aforementioned parameters, we simulated body mass at the time at which monkeys fully develop. For this purpose, we attempted to simulate/evaluate m(2), or body mass at approximately 16 years old, using the various combinations of cost parameters and canine termination times, i.e., \(c \in (0,1)\) and \(t_1 \in (0.5,T)\).
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Data availability
Data in support of the findings of this study are available from the corresponding authors by reasonable request.
Code availability
Mathematical models and proposed parameters are also described in the code repositories (https://github.com/hkoda/nasalis_canine_bodymass_model).
References
Darwin, C. The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex (John Murray, 1871).
Plavcan, J. M. & van Schaik, C. P. Intrasexual competition and canine dimorphism in anthropoid primates. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 87, 461–477 (1992).
Anderson, M. Sexual Selection (Princeton University Press, 1994).
Grueter, C. C., Isler, K. & Dixson, B. J. Are badges of status adaptive in large complex primate groups? Evol. Hum. Behav. 36, 398–406 (2015).
Rohwer, S. The evolution of reliable and unreliable badges of fighting ability. Am. Zool. 22, 531–546 (1982).
Sheehan, M. J. & Bergman, T. J. Is there an evolutionary trade-off between quality signaling and social recognition? Behav. Ecol. 27, 2–13 (2016).
Bergman, T. J. & Sheehan, M. J. Social knowledge and signals in primates. Am. J. Primatol. 75, 683–694 (2013).
Yeager, C. P. Proboscis monkey (Nasalis larvatus) social organization: intergroup patterns of association. Am. J. Primatol. 23, 73–86 (1991).
Koda, H. et al. Nasalization by Nasalis larvatus: larger noses audiovisually advertise conspecifics in proboscis monkeys. Sci. Adv. 4, eaaq0250 (2018).
Bhasin, S. et al. In Testosterone: Action, Deficiency, Substitution (eds. Nieschlag, E. & Behre, H. M.) (Cambridge University Press, 2012).
Muller, M. N. Testosterone and reproductive effort in male primates. Horm. Behav. 91, 36–51 (2017).
Zingeser, M. R. & Phoenix, C. H. Metric characteristics of the canine dental complex in prenatally androgenized female rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 49, 187–192 (1978).
Matsuda, I., Tuuga, A., Bernard, H. & Furuichi, T. Inter-individual relationships in proboscis monkeys: a preliminary comparison with other non-human primates. Primates 53, 13–23 (2012).
Plavcan, J. M. In Sexual Selection in Primates: New and Comparative Perspectives (eds. Kappeler, P. M. & van Schaik, C. P.) (Cambridge University Press, 2004).
Sugiyama, Y. On the social change of hanuman langurs (Presbytis entellus) in their natural condition. Primates 6, 381–418 (1965).
Crockett, C. M. & Pope, T. Inferring patterns of aggression from red howler monkey injuries. Am. J. Primatol. 15, 289–308 (1988).
Yeager, C. P. Proboscis monkey (Nasalis larvatus) social organization: nature and possible functions of intergroup patterns of association. Am. J. Primatol. 26, 133–137 (1992).
Matsuda, I., Kubo, T., Tuuga, A. & Higashi, S. A Bayesian analysis of the temporal change of local density of proboscis monkeys: implications for environmental effects on a multilevel society. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 142, 235–245 (2010).
Dunn, J. C. et al. Evolutionary trade-off between vocal tract and testes dimensions in howler monkeys. Curr. Biol. 25, 2839–2844 (2015).
Kodric-Brown, A., Sibly, R. M. & Brown, J. H. The allometry of ornaments and weapons. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 103, 8733–8738 (2006).
Petrie, M. Are all secondary sexual display structures positively allometric and, if so, why? Anim. Behav. 43, 173–175 (1992).
Hylander, W. L. Functional links between canine height and jaw gape in catarrhines with special reference to early hominins. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 150, 247–259 (2013).
Greenfield, L. O. Origin of the human canine: a new solution to an old enigma. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 35, 153–185 (1992).
van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. In Primate Ethology (ed. Morris, D.) (Aldine Transaction, 1967).
Yeager, C. P. Proboscis monkey (Nasalis larvatus) social organization: group structure. Am. J. Primatol. 20, 95–106 (The Royal Society, UK, 1990).
Weatherall, D. et al. The Use of Non-Human Primates in Research (2006).
Unwin, S., Ancrenaz, M., Bailey, W., Setchell, J. M. & Curtis, D. J. In Field and Laboratory Methods in Primatology: A Practical Guide, 2nd edn (eds Setchell, J. M. & Curtis, D. J.) (Cambridge University Press, 2011).
Stark, D. J., Vaughan, I. P., Ramirez Saldivar, D. A., Nathan, S. K. & Goossens, B. Evaluating methods for estimating home ranges using GPS collars: a comparison using proboscis monkeys (Nasalis larvatus). PLoS ONE 12, e0174891 (2017).
Leutenegger, W. & Kelly, J. T. Relationship of sexual dimorphism in canine size and body size to social, behavioral, and ecological correlates in anthropoid primates. Primates 18, 117–136 (1977).
Quinn, G. P. & Keough, M. J. Experimental Design and Data Analysis for Biologists (Cambridge University Press, 2002).
Burnham, K. P. & Anderson, D. R. Model Selection and Multimodel Inference: A Practical Information—Theoretic Approach, 2nd edn (Springer, 2002).
R-Core-Development-Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing (Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2019).
Bonduriansky, R. & Day, T. The evolution of static allometry in sexually selected traits. Evolution 57, 2450 (2003).
Murai, T. Social behaviors of all-male proboscis monkeys when joined by females. Ecol. Res. 19, 451–454 (2004).
Sha, J., Matsuda, I. & Bernard, H. The Natural History of the Proboscis Monkey (Natural History Publications, Borneo, 2011).
Pournelle, G. H. Observations on reproductive behaviour and early postnatal development of the Proboscis monkey Nasalis larvatus orientalisat San Diego Zoo. Int. Zoo. Yearb. 7, 90–92 (1967).
Leigh, S. R., Setchell, J. M. & Buchanan, L. S. Ontogenetic bases of canine dimorphism in anthropoid primates. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 127, 296–311 (2005).
Schultz, A. H. Growth and development of the proboscis monkey. Bull. Mus. Comp. Zool. 89, 277–314 (1942).
Acknowledgements
We thank the Sabah Biodiversity Centre and the Sabah Wildlife Department for granting permission to carry out this research, our research assistants for support in the field, and the Japan Monkey Centre (Dr. N. Kimura) for providing the information used in our mathematical simulations. We appreciate the profound comments and suggestions from the members of “Tsuda CREST”, especially I. Tsuda to support verifying the mathematical simulations. This study was partly performed under the Cooperative Research Program at KUPRI (2020-B-17) and was partially funded by Japan Science and Technology Agency Core Research for Evolutional Science and Technology 17941861 (#JPMJCR17A4), Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology Grant-in-aid for Scientific Research on Innovative Areas #4903 (Evolinguistics), 17H06380, Japan Society for the Promotion of Science KAKENHI (#19KK0191 to IM), and Yayasan Sime Darby (to B.G.). This paper acknowledges the memory of Dr. Diana Angeles Ramirez Saldivar.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
I.M., B.G., C.P.v.S., and H.K. conceptualized the initial idea; I.M., D.J.S., D.A.R.S., and S.K.S.S.N. performed the wild morphology sampling, A.T. and B.G. arranged the sampling in the wild; I.M. and H.K. performed and interpreted the statistical analyses and mathematical simulation; I.M., C.P.v.S., and H.K. drafted the manuscript. All authors contributed to the final version of the manuscript.
Corresponding authors
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Matsuda, I., Stark, D.J., Saldivar, D.A.R. et al. Large male proboscis monkeys have larger noses but smaller canines. Commun Biol 3, 522 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-020-01245-0
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-020-01245-0
This article is cited by
-
Modifying the diets of captive proboscis monkeys in a temperate zoo to reduce weight loss and renal disease
Primates (2023)
-
An Approaching Motor Boat Induces Stress-Related Behaviors in Proboscis Monkeys (Nasalis larvatus) Living in a Riparian Area
International Journal of Primatology (2022)
-
Factors influencing riverine utilization patterns in two sympatric macaques
Scientific Reports (2020)
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.