Introduction: polarization of beliefs, homophily in communication, information credibility

Polarization of beliefs is about the existence of opposing beliefs within large sections of the society (Bail et al., 2018; DiMaggio et al., 1996; Baldassarri and Gelman, 2008; Prior, 2013). In situations, like climate change action, where unanimous belief can drive the required collective steps, polarization can be a hurdle and may lead to socially undesired actions (Iyengar and Westwood, 2015; Iyengar et al., 2012; McCright and Dunlap, 2010; Center, 2014). Public understanding of climate change has remained polarized (Moore et al., 2019; McCright et al., 2014; Hamilton, 2011). Polarization of beliefs can be affected not only by the nature of users’ tendencies like homophily in communication about the reality of climate change, but also by certain nature of the information itself. Credibility of information is one of such properties of information. We discuss these determinants of polarization below.

Homophily in communication can affect the polarization of beliefs. Homophily in communication is a key tendency of users in information propagation media like Twitter and other platforms: people tend to communicate with others who hold similar beliefs (Barberá, 2015; Conover et al., 2011). Although homophily can facilitate the flow of information (Jackson and López-Pintado, 2013), it can lead to polarization (Bessi et al., 2016) as well and decrease the general ability of the society to learn the truth (Taylor et al., 2018; Golub and Jackson, 2012). Communicating only with people who share similar ideology or opinion restricts beliefs and prevents learning the truth (Madsen et al., 2018; Acemoglu et al., 2011, 2010). For example, homophily among liberals and conservatives in political blogs links (Adamic and Glance, 2005), i.e., the tendency of liberal and conservative blogs to link primarily within their separate communities, leads to echo chambers (Jasny et al., 2015; Garrett, 2009; Flaxman et al., 2016), and hence beliefs can be polarized due to such exposure to selective information (Bakshy et al., 2015). Similarly, homophily in opinion exchanges in social media (e.g., via publicly visible replies and mentions in Twitter) can reinforce beliefs within various sections of the network due to selective exposure. Previously echo chambers have been observed for climate change discussions on social media (Williams et al., 2015; McCright and Dunlap, 2010). Such patterns when repeat themselves in various parts of the network can lead to polarization of beliefs.

Credibility of information is also an important factor to create polarization, especially in online media where usually there exists information from several sources (Greer, 2003) that propagate controversial beliefs. Credibility of information is the precision of information, it signifies how certain the information is and helps to assign a certain level of trust to the information (Kiousis, 2001; Hovland and Weiss, 1951). The credibility of information that propagates in a social network is a critical factor that can shape the beliefs: if incoming information from a person’s social network carries no credibility, then it is less likely to be incorporated in to the belief of the person (Pornpitakpan, 2004; Heesacker et al., 1983). Hence, negative consequences that may arise due to the spread of fake information in social networks depend on information credibility, thereby making it a factor that can induce polarization. The importance of precision of misinforming signals has also been highlighted recently by Allcott and Gentzkow (2017). Credibility is a dimension of information which is independent of veracity of the information. For instance, a particular (unintended) fake story can be more credible if the information source is highly reputable (and hence the fake story inherits the credibility of the source) compared to the case when the same story comes from a less credible information source. It is the former case that has the potential to change beliefs in the society and change the existing levels of polarization concerning the truth of its story. Credibility of information in social media, generally speaking, may be ascribed due to several factors including reputation of information source, number of verified facts cited along with the information, mentioning opinion leaders and others (Markham, 2006; Westerman et al., 2014). Lastly, credibility of a communicated information is independent from the preciseness with which the information source believes the information. For example, a certain propaganda house which believes strongly about a story need not be able to spread such beliefs in the society through various communications because such communications may lack the degree of credibility required to generate substantial change in beliefs in society.

Twitter has become a modern platform for news dissemination and opinion exchanges, and is widely adopted by many users worldwide. We believe discussions and information propagation that happens on such a platform has potential to shape beliefs at a large scale. Important topics like climate change are also discussed on such social media platforms. With fake information on many topics becoming prevalent on widely adopted social media platform like Twitter, it is probably not surprising that there are several tweets both in favor and against the statement that climate change is a real concern. Fake information regarding such an important issue as climate change can pose a collective hurdle for the society at large if such information becomes highly credible among users of the platform. In this study, we infer the credibility of anti-climate change opinions on Twitter using (i) an empirical analysis that investigates the effect of homophily in communication patterns on the polarization of beliefs, and (ii) the predictions of a model of polarization of beliefs that jointly accounts for the roles of information credibility and homophily in communication. For the empirical analysis, we use tweets about climate change topic during 2007–2017. We rely on opinion exchanges among climate change believers and skeptics made via mentioning (and replying) others in tweets as the communication pattern. (Replies in Twitter tag the user names at the beginning of tweets, while user mentions can be made anywhere within the text of tweets). This is because retweets do not contain new opinions and are mere repetitions or broadcast of opinions expressed in original tweets, whereas mentions contain explicit referencing of other users and hence convey exchange of opinions targeted towards users being mentioned.

Methods

Data

We use tweets from the online social network site Twitter. Tweets are the messages posted by users on the platform. Tweets can contain hashtags, mentions to other users, external links in addition to text. A tweet posted by a user can be retweeted, replied and liked by other users. The tweets from 2007 to 2017 were collected based on a search filter that each tweet contains at least one of the following words: ‘climate change’, ‘#climatechange’, ‘global warming’, ‘#globalwarming’. The search was performed via Tweeter’s public advanced search page. This collection of tweets does not violate any ethical standards and consists of only publicly available data. The data is complete in the sense that it contains all original tweets that fulfill the search criteria. Retweets of original tweets are not included in the data. Each tweet however contains the retweet statistic, i.e., how many times it has been retweeted. There are a total of 14,353,859 unique tweets (without counting retweets values) and 3,595,205 unique users in the dataset.

Measuring sentiment and opinion

The sentiment of each tweet is computed using VADER (Hutto and Gilbert, 2014) model which is designed to conduct sentiment classification specifically on short texts like tweets. For each tweet, a score is obtained on the scale −1 (most negative) to +1 (most positive). A message with a positive sentiment is usually in favor of the (intended) object in the message. However, the same message may be communicated in a way that contains negative sentiment. Therefore sentiment and opinion contained in a message are two different characteristics of the message. This distinction between sentiment and stance has been clarified by previous studies on stance detection of tweets (Sobhani et al., 2016; Mohammad et al., 2017). From various data used in these previous studies, we use a subset that is used to annotate whether the tweet is in favor or against the statement “climate change is a real concern”. This subset contains manual annotation of whether a tweet is in favor or against the statement. We use this data as the training data to predict the opinion expressed in the texts of tweets in our sample.

The tweets were coded as numerical features using the TF-IDF (term frequency-inverse document frequency) representation and the prediction on the dataset for this study was done using the support vector classifier, which had the highest predictive power among other classifiers (logistic regression, decision tree). Each user’s tweets were first classified into one of the following categories: in favor of the statement, against the statement, no opinion. (Hence, in the first stage, opinion is assigned to each tweet). Next, each user was classified into one of the above categories based on the category that contains the maximum number of tweets from the user. Since a user on Twitter usually has a belief about climate change (in general), the classification algorithm has managed to predict each users’s tweet into a single category only. Only 42 users were classified as having no opinion, and were dropped from the dataset. Most users had tweets in one category only: either in favor, or against the statement. This validates the performance of the classifier at the user level even if the prediction accuracy (at the tweets level) is about 72%.

Measure of homophily

The notion of homophily in communication is the presence of higher interaction among people who hold the same opinion about climate change. For instance, the group of users who believe climate change is not real have a homophily in communication pattern if such users communicate more among themselves than with users who believe climate change is real.

Communication between users can take the forms of retweeting and mentioning (includes replying) on the Twitter platform. Although retweeting, in general, suggests that users share the same ideology, mentioning can occur between users with differing ideologies (tweet wars). To what extent do mentioning patterns in tweets reveal communication among users having similar ideologies? Previous studies (Lietz et al., 2014; Aragón et al., 2013) reveal that homophily of the aggregate network measured using retweeting and mentioning tend to be same, although mentioning is more volatile at the group (sub network with a particular ideology) levels. Hence, based on such studies, we assume that homophily in mentioning patterns reveals the homophily in retweeting (and following) patterns, for which we do not have the data. Communication therefore refers to users interacting via mentioning each other in tweets.

To measure homophily in communication among users with same belief or opinion regarding climate change, we follow the measures used in previous empirical studies on homophily (Currarini et al., 2009; Halberstam and Knight, 2016; Colleoni et al., 2014). Let I be the total number of users in a given month who either mentioned other users or received mentions from other users’ tweets. We indicate belief of a user as b {a, f}, where a and f correspond to the cases where the user is against and in favor, respectively, of the climate change statement. a and f represent two types of belief b. Suppose Ib is the total number of type b users, then \(w_b = \frac{{I_b}}{I}\) is the fraction of type b users. Let vib be the number of type b users mentioned by user i. Then \(s_b = \frac{1}{{I_b}}\mathop {\sum}\nolimits_{i \in I_b} {v_{i,b}}\) is the average number of same type users mentioned by type b users, and \(d_b = \frac{1}{{I_b}}\mathop {\sum}\nolimits_{i \in I_b} {v_{i, - b}}\) is the average number of opposite type users mentioned by type b users. Using this, homophily of the group of type b users is defined as \(H_b\; = \;\frac{{s_b}}{{s_b + d_b}}\;\) and the homophily of the society is defined as

$$H = \mathop {\sum}\limits_{b \in \{ a,f\} } {w_b} H_b.$$

Measure of polarization

A tweet carries an opinion (whether in favor or against climate change being real), denoted as op, with a certain sentiment, denoted as s. (We encode op as 1 if the message in the tweet is against the climate change statement and as −1 if the message is in favor of climate change). Irrespective of the opinion, the sentiment can be positive or negative depending on the way the message is communicated. So we use an emotion-adjusted measure of belief called EAB that combines these two aspects: expressed opinion in the message, and emotional content in the message. For a given tweet with attributes op and s, EAB is defined as op|s|, the product of opinion and absolute value of sentiment.

A large number of tweets have neutral or close to neutral sentiment, thereby increasing the mass around 0 and decreasing the intensity of bimodality in the distribution of EAB. (The presence of high volume of neutral tweets is not uncommon, for example see Kušen and Strembeck (Kušen and Strembeck, 2018). In this sense, the polarization indicator used during the mathematical analysis in opinion updating model (see Supplementary Information) cannot be carried to the empirical setting directly. We therefore need another measure of polarization that is sensitive to the properties (e.g., kurtosis and skewness) of a distribution and carries the intuition of bi-modality approach (i.e., the statistic should be sensitive to the degree of bi-modality of a distribution). To calculate the degree of polarization involved in the EAB distribution, we use the measure of ideological divergence provided by Lelkes (2016), which characterizes the level of polarization based on bi-modality of the distribution by being sensitive to kurtosis and skewness (Freeman and Dale, 2013; Pfister et al., 2013). Using this measure, the polarization of EAB is defined as

$$\frac{{s^2 + 1}}{{k + 3\frac{{(n - 1)^2}}{{(n - 2)(n - 3)}}}},$$

where s and k are the skewness and excess kurtosis of the EAB distribution, and n is the sample size. The values of 1 and 0 correspond to cases when the EAB distribution is perfectly bimodal and perfectly unimodal, respectively. A value greater than 0.56 is categorized as bimodal. Although this threshold may not be reached precisely, approaching this value is considered coming closer to polarization (Lelkes, 2016).

The measure of polarization above is calculated at the levels of tweets and is a stricter measure than when calculated at the level of users. This is because a user might be slightly heterogeneous in his tweets but aggregating all tweets would reveal one exact ideology. For example, when a climate change skeptic user writes a tweet, it is most likely to be against the statement that climate change is real. However a few tweets of his might fall in favor of the statement. In this sense, polarization is easier to obtain at an user level than at the content (tweets) level.

It is easy to see that the polarization measure describes a different phenomenon than the homophily measure. While homophily measure is constructed using users involved in a mention activity in a given month (micro level), polarization is measured using all the EAB of all tweets in the month (macro level). This is in line with Esteban and Ray (1994), according to whom any reasonable measure of polarization must be global in nature.

Results

Negative effect of homophily on polarization

The monthly time series of polarization of beliefs and homophily in communication during 2007–2017 are shown in Fig. 1. (Measures of homophily and polarization are described in Methods section). It appears that the evolutions of the two curves are not independent, and polarization tends to decrease at times when homophily increases. Augmented Dickey-Fuller test and Phillips-Ouliaris test suggest that polarization and homophily are cointegrated, i.e., they are bound in a long term equilibrium relationship such that they mean-revert whenever there are deviations away from the equilibrium. (Supplementary Information contains statistical details of tests of cointegration). Hence it becomes natural to model polarization and homophily jointly in a vector error correction (VEC) model (Granger and Weiss, 1983; Engle and Granger, 1987). VEC models are a special class of models derived from vector autoregression (VAR) models with an additional term, called error correction term, to account for the cointegration. VEC model allows to study the lagged effects of homophily on polarization and also the lagged effects of polarization on homophily. Supplementary Table 4 contains details of the parameter estimates of VEC model: the lagged homophily covariates negatively affect polarization in the long term with high statistical significance and the lagged effects of polarization on homophily are not significant. These estimates however do not form a conclusion about the direction of causality between homophily and polarization. For assessing causality, we perform Granger causality test (Granger, 1969) with modifications, as suggested by Toda and Yamamoto (1995), to adapt to the non-stationary nature of both time series; the results are shown in Table 1. (Further statistical details regarding these tests are available in Supplementary Information). The conclusion that emerges is that only homophily Granger-causes polarization with a negative effect, and the causality in the other direction is absent, i.e., polarization does not Granger cause homophily. This empirical result that homophily negatively affects polarization is counter-intuitive to the discussions made previously about the nature of homophily and polarization.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Polarization of beliefs, homophily in communication. The figure shows monthly evolutions of polarization of beliefs about climate change (blue) and homophily in communication patterns on Twitter (orange) during 2007–2017

Table 1 Granger-causality tests

Effect of homophily on polarization depends on information credibility

We study a simple model of emergence of polarization in social networks that highlights the joint effect of credibility of propagating information and homophily in communication of information to affect polarization. We model polarization as bi-modality of the population’s distribution of beliefs and it captures the following basic features, as laid down by Esteban and Ray (1994), which are necessary for a distribution to be considered polarized: there must be a high degree of homogeneity within each group (whose agents hold same belief), a high degree of heterogeneity across groups, and a small number of significantly sized groups. If the groups are of insignificant size (e.g., isolated individuals), they do not contribute to polarization. The beliefs can be emotion-adjusted as well, as is done in the empirical analysis (see Methods section), to include the emotional content of the belief; this does not change the nature of predictions of our model.

In our model, we consider a social network with each agent receiving information about a topic (e.g., the reality of climate change) from the same number, k, of speakers (for simplicity). Speakers of an agent are other agents in the network from whom he receives information. An unobserved fundamental θ, with values in the real line, describes the accuracy of the beliefs: an agent with a higher value is more distant from the truth. At a given time t, we assume there are two types of agents—θl, θr—informed and misinformed, and the social network has a homophily coefficient of \(\frac{h}{k}\) with respect to this property: h speakers of an agent of type θi, i {l, r}, are of the same θi type. We model the prior beliefs of informed (θl) and misinformed (θr) agents as distributions of θ over the real line to allow for heterogeneous prior beliefs at time t. We assume that the prior beliefs of informed and misinformed agents derive from distributions \({\cal{N}}(\theta _0,\delta _l^{ - 1})\) and \({\cal{N}}(\theta _0 + \xi ,\delta _r^{ - 1})\), respectively, with a strictly positive value for ξ, in order to ensure that the misinformed type are, on average, farther away from truth compared to the informed type. The belief of the entire population of the social network at time t, a density-weighted average of the above distributions, is considered to be polarized if it has two modes (peaks in the distribution).

We assume each agent communicates a particular realization of his belief using a message (e.g., via a tweet) during the period between t and a future time t + 1, and all agents update their prior beliefs at t + 1 after incorporating beliefs about the fundamental expressed in their speakers’ messages. Mathematically, a message from a speaker is modeled as an independent noisy signal about the speaker’s realized belief, with the degree of the noise (or uncertainty) being conditional on the type of speaker. Since credibility of a communicated message is the precision of belief expressed in the message, in our model, true and fake information about the reality, arising from informed and misinformed speakers, respectively, propagate with different credibilities in the social network. (We denote the credibilities or precisions of true and fake information as βl and βr, respectively). We assume that the messages from informed agents carry a positive credibility. (In our context of the reality about climate change, this is a reasonable assumption). It is noteworthy to mention that for a speaker of a given type θi, i {l, r}, βi is conceptually different from δi: while δi characterizes the probability with which a particular random realization will be selected as the belief for the speaker, βi characterizes how precisely the realized belief is communicated in a message. In a Bayesian update of beliefs, how a communicated message affects the beliefs of listeners of the message depends on the belief expressed in the communicated message, the precision (or credibility) of the message, prior beliefs held by individual listeners and the precision of such prior beliefs of listeners.

We now present the findings of some analyses conducted using the model discussed above. (Mathematical proofs of the results presented below are available in Supplementary Information). We find that in a random communication network with agents being sufficiently uncertain about the prior beliefs they hold, polarization can not arise at time t + 1, after agents have incorporated their speakers’ beliefs. This means that, irrespective of the precisions with which true and fake information diffuse in the social network, they do not play a role in the emergence of polarization without the support of non-random topology of the social network and agents’ prior beliefs. We also find that if marginal increase in homophily at time t does not increase the probability of polarization at t + 1, it can happen only in the case when the fake signals that propagate in the social network do not have a minimal level of credibility and carry zero precision. A visual illustration of this result is shown in Fig. 2. We believe this provides a potential reason for our previous observation that, in the climate change discussions on Twitter, polarization of beliefs does not increase when homophily in communication patterns increases.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Belief updating model. The figure illustrates increase in polarization at time t + 1 due to increase in homophily (based on beliefs, e.g., reality of climate change) at time t. There is zero probability for such an increase if fake information that propagates in the social network has zero credibility

Discussion

The online social network Twitter has remained an important media for rapid spread of opinions. We studied the opinions expressed in Twitter during 2007–2017 regarding the reality of climate change. Below, we briefly discuss our main findings and their limitations, and suggest directions for future research.

The analysis over a long time period provides insight about the direction of potential causality between homophily and polarization which would otherwise not have been possible in cross-sectional observations. In social networks, polarization of beliefs (existence of large groups of people with opposing beliefs) and homophily in communication (communication among people having same beliefs) tend to be highly correlated. One potential mechanism for such a correlation is that increase in homophily can reinforce individual beliefs leading to the creation of echo chambers and hence increase polarization. Another mechanism for such a correlation is that increase in polarization increases segregation of a society into different beliefs thereby acting as a natural source to increase the probability of like-minded communication (homophily). In the case of Twittersphere of climate change conversations, we performed Granger causality tests on the evolutions of homophily and polarization, and found that only homophily Granger-causes polarization and not vice-versa, i.e., increasing polarization does not lead to increase in like-minded communication. It is important to note that Granger causation of homophily on polarization does not fully establish its causality. Granger causality (as explained in Supplementary Information) is a concept about precedence of the cause (homophily) before the effect (polarization). It says that the evolution of homophily significantly helps to predict evolution of polarization in future. Existence of Granger causality therefore does not exclude the possibility of an unobserved variable to drive the evolution of both homophily and polarization. Although the presence of such an unobserved variable is highly unlikely within the framework of our model and the variables used for empirical analysis, we encourage future research to build upon the above results to improve the causal nature of the effect of homophily on polarization—this would require analyzing scenarios where homophily can be argued to be exogenous so that we are better ensured about effects not being driven by unobserved variables. (Such causal relationship may be investigated for topics other than climate change as well).

We also found that the effect of homophily on polarization is negative, i.e., increase in homophily in communication leads to decrease in polarization of beliefs in future. This is counter-intuitive because we would expect an increase in polarization when homophily increases. Increasing homophily leads to situations where people are exposed to a narrow set of beliefs (Acemoglu et al., 2011, 2010) that conforms to their existing beliefs. When such homophily in communication happens in two large sections of the society with differing beliefs, it enhances polarization (Esteban and Ray, 1994).

Polarization of beliefs can be affected not only by homophily in communication among people, but also by the credibility of information that propagates on Twitter. In this study, we investigated whether credibility of information source plays a role to increase polarization. We studied the ‘credibility’ factor because this has received less attention in the literature and is a very intuitive factor. It is intuitive since information from a source which is not credible is naturally least likely to affect or change the belief of an individual. Credibility is a dimension of information independent of the veracity of information. For example, let us consider a certain information (either true or fake) that comes from two different sources at the same time: one source is a Twitter account of a person who is not very well known, and another is the Twitter account of BBC News (as instance) which has a much larger credibility in the society. In this case the news sent by BBC News has a higher probability to influence the beliefs of people at a large scale. More so, if the news happens to be (intentionally or unintentionally) fake, it can possibly affect the beliefs of a certain section of the society in a wrong direction (since the news is fake) thereby increasing polarization in the society because a new section of the society emerges with beliefs much different from what the entire population believed previously.

We modeled these two determinants of polarization discussed above, homophily in communication and credibility of information, jointly in a belief updating model where agents in a networked society receive true and fake information from their neighbors. We modeled the credibility of each type of information using the precision or certainty of the information. The model predicts that marginal increase in homophily always leads to increase in polarization expect for the only case when fake information has no credibility. In the case when fake information is not credible, the model predicts a negative effect of homophily on polarization. (This description is illustrated in Fig. 2). Since we observed a negative effect of homophily on polarization in the empirical analysis of climate change discussions on the Twitter platform, we conclude that tweets expressing anti-climate change beliefs are largely not credible to the broader society.

Our results disentangle the presence of fake information on social media from its potential negative effect on the society. The spread of fake information (either as misinformation or disinformation), as has been prevalent during recent years in online social media (Vosoughi et al., 2018; Del Vicario et al., 2016), has the potential to pose harm to the society by polarizing the beliefs of people (Vosoughi et al., 2018; Del Vicario et al., 2016; Bessi et al., 2016); for instance, it can influence political election outcomes (Shin et al., 2017). However, does it mean that fake information always has a negative effect on the society? Based on our results of this study, we can say that it is not always the case. In the case of reality about climate change, although there are many climate-skeptic messages, we showed that such messages are largely not credible and hence polarization (a negative consequence for society) does not increase when users on the platform communicate among themselves. Hence, in general, we believe that the presence of fake information (on various topics including climate change) in social media and the web is not a conclusive evidence of its negative effect on the society at large. This also reaffirms the fact that the negative effects of echo chambers in social networks, which are known to arise due to high levels of homophily in communication, might be overstated (Warner, 2010; Bail et al., 2018; Flaxman et al., 2016; Dubois and Blank, 2018).

We now discuss some assumptions about agents in our model, and how alternative assumptions about human behavior (in real world) may also explain how increase in homophily can lead to decrease in polarization in future. Agents in the model are rational (Madsen et al., 2018) and purely rely on previously held beliefs and beliefs expressed by neighbors in their immediate social networks to arrive at new beliefs. Such assumption of Bayesian updating of beliefs dictates that when homophily increases (i.e., agents’ exposure to non-conforming beliefs decreases, or in other words, cross-attitudinal interactions decreases), the ability of misinformed agents to move closer to truth decreases. Human behavior, in general, is highly heterogeneous and there may exist different ways in which people update beliefs in real world. Let us consider a different situation where the utility of agents depends not only on the belief of own type but also on the (opposite) belief held by the other type. In particular consider a situation where people update beliefs in the following manner—when people are exposed to cross ideologies, instead of gaining higher utility by incorporating and averaging it with own ideologies, they gain higher utility by the fact that they know something (what they think is supposed to be known) and the opposite party is misguided and carries the wrong belief. In such a case, if homophily increases, cross-attitudinal interactions decrease and the misinformed agents are less convinced about their belief because their exposure to informed type (opposite type) agents decreases. This leads to dilution of ones’s held beliefs and creates a decrease in polarization. Although such a behavioral assumption can lead to an alternative way to explain negative effect of homophily on polarization, we believe that such assumption may not scale to a wider population. (Especially in the case of reality about climate change, we believe that informed people would also want ill-informed sections of the society to know about the reality as they do). In this context, we believe that by imposing a Bayesian updating criteria we have studied the expected outcomes in a baseline model, and is (ideally) representative of the behavior of a larger population. We consider the model we analyzed in this study to be parsimonious enough to be able to explain the negative effect of homophily on polarization by highlighting the role of information credibility. Future research, depending on the nature of investigation, may find our analysis as a useful guide.

Individual rumors are being debunked in due time by fact checking measures (Zhao et al., 2015; Ciampaglia et al., 2015), however, public perception about some issues like human induced (anthropogenic) climate change have remained controversial for a long time (Tvinnereim et al., 2017). Such controversy appears to persist even after several investigations made by the scientific community. Various factors like exposure to different kind of information on the media (Iyengar and Massey, 2019; Williams, 2016), politicization of climate change (McCright and Dunlap, 2011), and exposure to fake information on social media platforms are potential reasons that can contribute to polarization of beliefs about the reality of climate change. In this study, we showed that social media messages on platform like Twitter is not a potential concern because although there are many sources propagating fake beliefs regarding climate change, the collective credibility of such sources is negligible. Future research may contribute to improve our understanding about how public perceives the reality about climate change. It is better that the society is not polarized on beliefs about the reality of climate change, so that timely environmental policies can be implemented with least public resistance. Since credibility of major fake information sources about climate change can polarize beliefs on a large scale, it is important to assess credibility of such sources and their potential negative effects on society. (In this context, polarization is one such negative or undesired effect).