Introduction

Fear of falling is prevalent amongst older adults1 and individuals with movement disorders2,3. Older adults with a self-reported fear of falling demonstrate differences in static4 and dynamic5,6 balance control compared to individuals matched for age and level of physical function. These observations suggest that emotional factors have the potential to directly influence the control of balance, potentially contributing to the increased risk of falling documented amongst individuals living with a fear of falling7,8. As a result, there is a clear need to understand the nature of fear-related balance changes and the potential for training to effectively impact these behaviours.

To examine how fear of falling influences balance control, different postural threat manipulations have been used. The underlying premise is that emotional and cognitive changes similar to those experienced on a daily basis by individuals living with a fear of falling can be induced by changing the perceived consequences or likelihood of falling9,10,11. Commonly used threat manipulations have involved either raising the height of the surface on which individuals stand (height-induced threat) or having individuals stand at ground level with or without the expectation of a sudden, unpredictable balance perturbation (threat of perturbation)12. With both threat manipulations, individuals demonstrate hallmarks of increased sympathetic activation (i.e., increased tonic electrodermal activity) and self-report greater anxiety and changes in cognitive appraisals that would be expected in individuals experiencing a fear of falling (i.e., reductions in balance-specific efficacy and perceived ability to cope with the threat)11,13,14. These changes in psychological and autonomic state are accompanied by changes in standing balance control that appear to be moderated by the context of the threat. With height-induced threat, individuals typically lean away from the platform edge and demonstrate higher frequency and smaller amplitude postural adjustments12. By contrast, with the threat of an unpredictable forward or backward perturbation, individuals lean forward and demonstrate higher frequency and larger amplitude postural adjustments15,16.

Recent work has shown that after being repeatedly exposed to height-induced threat17 or the threat of perturbation18, individuals’ emotional response to the threat was significantly attenuated, yet most changes in standing balance control were preserved. With repeated exposure to height, the only threat-induced changes in standing balance to show any attenuation included very high frequency centre of pressure (COP) power (> 1.8 Hz) and coactivation of plantar and dorsiflexor muscles17. By contrast, the posterior lean and reductions in COP amplitude and low-frequency COP power (< 0.05 Hz) did not habituate. Similar changes in COP dynamics were observed following repeated exposure to the threat of perturbation, with habituation limited to relatively high frequency COP oscillations (0.5–1.8 Hz) and mean power frequency (MPF) of COP18. It is unclear why this pattern of adaptation, or lack thereof, was observed. One possible explanation is that only some changes in standing balance control are related to the psychological and autonomic state changes induced by the threat. Unlike threat-induced changes in the average frequency content of COP oscillations, changes in leaning and COP amplitude have shown inconsistent correlations with self-report and physiological indicators of individuals’ emotional response to threat13,14,17,19,20 and exhibit different directions of change depending upon the type of threat experienced (height or perturbation threat)12. This could suggest that these changes in standing balance are employed in a context-dependent manner largely independent of individuals’ psychological and autonomic response to the threat.

Alternatively, there may be a non-linear relationship between some threat-induced changes in standing balance control and the magnitude of individuals’ emotional response to threat, such that complete, or near complete, attenuation of psychological and autonomic state changes induced by the threat are needed before specific standing balance adaptations begin to manifest. The aforementioned studies used relatively brief exposure periods, and self-reported and physiological measures of individuals’ emotional response to threat in these samples seldom approached values comparable to non-threatening conditions17,18. Thus, it remains possible that some threat-induced changes in standing balance control may begin to show adaptation only following a longer period of exposure that more thoroughly attenuates the cognitive-emotional responses to the threat.

Therefore, the current study used an extended exposure protocol to determine if threat-induced changes in standing balance persist following complete or near-complete adaptation of individuals’ psychological and autonomic response to a height-induced threat. To address this question, two separate sets of analyses were completed. The first set of analyses examined how threat-induced changes in standing balance adapt over the course of a long bout of repeated threat exposure across the sample as a whole. The second set of analyses focused exclusively on a subgroup of participants who demonstrated complete attenuation of their psychological and autonomic response to threat. It was expected that the majority of participants would show near complete adaptation of their psychological and autonomic response to threat and these changes would be accompanied by reductions in high-frequency COP power and plantar/dorsiflexor coactivation17,21. On the basis that threat-induced changes in leaning and COP amplitude are context-dependent and adopted largely independent of individuals’ psychological and autonomic response to threat, these were expected to show minimal change over the course of repeated threat exposure, even amongst individuals who showed complete adaptation of their psychological and autonomic response to threat.

A secondary aim of this study was to examine the extent to which standing balance adaptations acquired following repeated threat exposure are retained. Repeated exposure to activities perceived as threatening is an important component of cognitive-behavioural interventions designed to reduce fear of falling22,23. However, no studies to date have examined if any standing balance adaptations acquired during repeated threat exposure are retained when re-exposed to a similar threat. To address this question, participants from this study were re-exposed to the same height-induced postural threat 2–4 weeks after they completed the extended threat exposure protocol. Based on previous studies that have repeatedly exposed individuals to height-related situations, it was hypothesized that individuals’ psychological and autonomic response to threat would be attenuated (relative to their initial exposure to threat) following a short retention period24,25. This reduced emotional response to threat was expected to be accompanied by smaller threat-induced increases in high-frequency COP power and plantar/dorsiflexor coactivation. Other threat-induced changes in standing balance which were not expected to show adaptation with repeated threat exposure (i.e., leaning and COP amplitude and low frequency power) were not expected to be any different between visits.

Methods

Participants

Thirty-seven healthy young adults (mean age ± SD = 23.4 ± 4.4 years; 22 females) naïve to the postural threat manipulation participated in this study. Participants were free of musculoskeletal and neurological disorders that could impair their balance. No participants self-reported having an extreme fear of heights. All procedures were performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations, and were reviewed and cleared by the University of British Columbia’s Clinical Research Ethics Board. Participants provided written informed consent.

Prior to completing the experimental procedures, participants provided basic demographic information and completed a battery of personality questionnaires. This included the trait version of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory26 to assess trait anxiety, the Movement-Specific Reinvestment Scale27 to assess individuals’ propensity to reinvest attention in their movement, and the recreational subscale of the Domain Specific Risk-Taking Scale28 to assess physical risk-taking. Participant demographic and personality trait information are provided in Table 1.

Table 1 Demographic and personality information for participants and adaptor subgroup.

Procedures

Participants completed a series of 90-s quiet standing trials at two different postural threat conditions on two visits to the lab separated by approximately 2–4 weeks (mean retention period ± SD = 20.3 ± 4.0 days). Throughout each trial, participants stood barefoot on a force plate (BP400600, AMTI, USA) positioned at the edge of a hydraulic lift (1.52 m × 2.13 m; Pentalift, Canada) with a stance width equal to the length of their foot and their toes were aligned to the anterior edge of the force plate. The borders of participants’ feet were traced onto the force plate to ensure consistent foot placement across all trials within each session. At the LOW condition, the platform was positioned at its lowest height (0.8 m above ground) and a table (0.6 m × 1.6 m) was positioned in front of the platform, creating 60 cm of continuous support surface in front of the participant29. For the HIGH condition, the table was removed, and the platform was elevated 3.2 m above the ground.

During their first visit (initial session), participants completed 2 quiet standing trials at the LOW condition before (LOWpre) and after (LOWpost) a block of 20 trials at the HIGH condition. This blocked presentation order was selected to maximize within-session habituation of the psychological and autonomic response to threat25,30. Throughout all trials, participants were instructed to stand quietly with their arms at their sides and fixate on an eye-level visual target positioned 3.8 m in front of them. To minimize potential fatigue effects associated with the extensive number of standing trials, at least 2-min of seated rest were completed between all trials. Prior to the experimental trials, participants completed a 90-s practice trial at the LOW condition to minimize potential first trial effects9.

During their second visit (retention session), participants completed an abbreviated version of the initial session. In particular, they completed two standing trials at the LOW condition (LOWpre) followed by 5 trials at the HIGH condition. Similar to the initial session, the standing trials were 90-s in length and at least 2-min of seated rest were completed between consecutive trials. Participants also completed a 90-s practice trial prior to the LOWpre trials. No participants reported engaging in any height-related activities during the time between the two visits. Figure 1 provides a schematic illustration of experimental procedures across both visits.

Figure 1
figure 1

Flow chart outlining the experimental protocol. Participants completed a series of 90-s trials of quiet standing at LOW (0.8 m above ground, away from edge) and HIGH (3.2 m above ground, at edge) threat conditions on two visits to the lab separated by 2–4 weeks. Each square represents a single 90-s trial; open squares represent trials completed at the LOW condition, while filled black squares represent trials completed at the HIGH condition. During both visits, participants first completed a single 90-s practice trial (grey square) at the LOW condition. Trials in which questionnaires were administered to assess cognitive and emotional state are indicated with an asterisk.

Throughout all trials during both visits, participants wore a safety harness that was securely fastened by climbing rope to an overhead support beam. The tension of the rope was adjusted such that the harness did not provide support that would assist in the balance task, but would support the participant’s body weight above the platform surface in the event of a fall. A trained spotter was also seated behind the participants to prevent a fall if the participant appeared unsteady. No falls occurred during this experiment and the spotter did not have to intervene to provide support at any point.

Data collection

Ground reaction forces and moments were sampled from the force plate at 100 Hz. Electromyography (EMG) was recorded bilaterally from the soleus (SOL) and tibialis anterior (TA) using Ag/AgCl surface electrodes positioned in a bipolar arrangement (interelectrode distance of ~ 1.25 cm). EMG data were sampled at 3000 Hz (Telemyo 2400R G2, Noraxon, USA), bandpass filtered online (10–1000 Hz), and then A-D sampled at 2000 Hz (Power1401, CED, UK). Skin conductance was recorded from Ag/AgCl surface electrodes placed on the thenar and hypothenar eminences of the non-dominant hand and sampled at 100 Hz (Model 2502SA, CED, UK).

Self-reports of emotional and cognitive state were probed at pre-defined trials throughout the experiment. During the initial session, this included all trials at the LOW condition and trials 1–5, 8, 11, 14, 17, and 20 at the HIGH condition. This order was selected since the greatest reduction in the emotional response to threat is likely to occur within the first five trials at the HIGH threat condition17. Self-reports were probed at all trials during the retention session (Fig. 1). Before each of these trials, participants reported how confident they were that they could maintain their balance and avoid a fall during the upcoming balance task. After these trials, they completed single-item questions that assessed cognitive and somatic anxiety and fear of falling. Responses to each of these questions were completed on visual analog scales (VAS) ranging from 0 to 100. Higher scores reflected greater balance confidence, cognitive and somatic anxiety, and fear of falling. Scores on the measures of cognitive and somatic anxiety were averaged to create a state anxiety score17,18. Focus of attention was also assessed after these trials using a 5-item questionnaire that asked participants to rate how much they thought about or paid attention to different information. Single questions were used to probe attention toward (1) movement processes (i.e., conscious control or monitoring of movement); (2) threat-related stimuli (i.e., feelings of anxiety or worry); (3) self-regulatory strategies (i.e., coping strategies to help remain calm, confident, and/or focused); (4) task objectives (i.e., constraints of the task); and (5) task-irrelevant information (i.e., thoughts unrelated to the balance task). Responses were rated on 9-point Likert scales, with higher scores indicating greater attention to each loci of attention17,18. The items and terminology incorporated into this questionnaire were derived from qualitative research investigating changes in attention associated with height-induced threat31.

Data analysis

Centre of pressure (COP) outcomes

Ground reaction forces and moments were low-pass filtered offline (10 Hz cutoff, 2nd order dual-pass Butterworth filter). From these data, only anterior–posterior COP was calculated since the effect of height-induced threat is greatest in this plane when facing the platform edge12. From the COP data, the mean COP position (MPOS-COP) was calculated to provide a measure of how far individuals leaned away from the platform edge. The mean was then subtracted from the COP signal to remove the bias and a linear detrend was applied. From these data, root mean square (RMS-COP) and mean power frequency (MPF-COP) were calculated to provide estimates of the amplitude and average frequency content of COP oscillations, respectively. When individuals stand under conditions of height-induced threat, MPF-COP typically increases, and recent work has shown this is due to reductions in the amplitude of lower frequency COP oscillations (< 0.05 Hz) and increases in the amplitude of higher frequency COP oscillations (> 0.5 Hz)17. Furthermore, following a relatively short period of repeated threat exposure, only oscillations greater than 1.8 Hz show significant attenuation17. Thus, mean COP power was calculated within three frequency bands: 0–0.05 Hz (low), 0.5–1.8 Hz (medium), and 1.8–5.0 Hz (high)17,18.

Tonic muscle activity and coactivation

EMG data from bilateral SOL and TA were debiased, full-wave rectified, and normalized to mean rectified EMG during maximal voluntary contractions. From these data, tonic activation levels were calculated as mean rectified EMG (averaged between both legs). EMG data were then low-pass filtered (3 Hz cutoff, 5th order dual-pass Butterworth filter) creating linear envelopes. SOL-TA coactivation for each leg was then determined by calculating the absolute overlap of SOL and TA linear envelopes32,33. Coactivation estimates for both legs were then averaged for each participant.

Sympathetic arousal

To provide an estimate of sympathetic arousal, non-specific electrodermal response frequency (EDR.freq) was calculated from skin conductance data. Skin conductance data were first low-pass filtered (1 Hz cutoff, 5th order dual-pass Butterworth filter) and then a customized algorithm identified and counted all electrodermal responses with peak amplitudes greater than 0.05 µS34. All identified EDRs were visually inspected and any false positive due to movement artifacts were rejected35.

Identification of “Adaptors”

Participants’ whose emotional response to threat returned to LOW threat values during the first visit were identified as “Adaptors”. To be considered an Adaptor, the following criteria needed to be met. First, participants needed to have a self-reported increase of at least 5 points on the fear of falling VAS during the first HIGH trial relative to the mean of the first two LOW trials. Participants then needed measures of self-reported fear of falling and EDR.freq to return to LOW threat values. A threshold for what would be considered LOW threat values for each participant was calculated as the mean + 1SD for each of these measures based on the first two LOW trials. The HIGH trial at which point both fear of falling and EDR measures returned to a value equal to or less than the LOW threshold was noted and all psychological, autonomic, and standing balance outcomes for this trial and the next HIGH trial were calculated and averaged (HIGHadapted). The only exception to this was if these criteria were met only by the time of the final HIGH trial, in which case there would be no subsequent HIGH trial, so only data from HIGH20 were used (n = 1). The HIGHadapted trials were included in the statistical procedures as opposed to the last two HIGH trials (i.e., HIGH19 and 20) in order to minimize possible fatigue or boredom effects which may manifest with continued exposure after the point of complete adaptation.

Statistics

Within-session adaptation

To examine how measures of psychological and autonomic state and standing balance adapt over the course of repeated threat exposure, a series of two-way (Threat: LOW vs. HIGH; Trial: Pre vs. Post) repeated measures ANOVAs were conducted. For the factor Trial, data were averaged across the first two (LOWpre) and last two (LOWpost) LOW trials for the Pre and Post levels of the LOW condition, while the first (HIGH1) and last (HIGH20) HIGH trials were used for the Pre and Post levels of the HIGH condition, respectively. Significant threat × trial interactions were followed-up with Bonferroni corrected paired-samples t-tests that examined the effect of threat across both levels of trial and the effect of trial across both levels of threat (alpha = 0.0125).

A separate set of statistics were conducted for individuals identified as Adaptors in order to determine if threat-induced changes in standing balance control are preserved in individuals whose emotional response to threat returned fully to LOW threat values. Dependent measures for the following trials were included in one-way repeated measures ANOVAs: LOWpre (average of the first two LOW trials), HIGHpre (average of first two HIGH trials), and HIGHadapted. The reason blocks of two trials were averaged was to reduce variability of the standing balance measures, since fewer participants were expected to be included in these analyses. Significant main effects were followed up with Bonferroni corrected paired-samples t-tests comparing HIGHpre and HIGHadapted with LOWpre (alpha = 0.025).

Retention of adaptations

To determine if any adaptations were retained between visits, two-way (Visit: 1 vs. 2; Trial: Δ HIGH1 vs. Δ HIGH5) repeated measures ANOVAs were conducted for measures of psychological and autonomic state and standing balance control for all participants. For the factor Trial, dependent measures at HIGH1 and HIGH5 were expressed as change scores relative to LOWpre data from respective visits (Δ HIGH1 = HIGH1 – LOWpre; Δ HIGH5 = HIGH5 – LOWpre). Significant interactions were followed up with Bonferroni corrected paired-samples t-tests examining the effect of trial across both levels of visit and the effect of visit across both levels of trial (alpha = 0.0125).

Results

Two participants (1 female) reported increases in fear of falling from the first to last HIGH trial during the initial session. Because the primary aim of this study was to determine if threat-induced changes in standing balance persist after attenuation of the emotional response to threat, these participants were excluded from statistical analyses. Due to technical issues, EMG data were missing for one participant and skin conductance data were missing for another. Several dependent measures were positively skewed; this included low-, medium-, and high-frequency COP power, tonic TA activation, and SOL-TA coactivation. To correct for this non-normality, log transformations were applied to these data.

Visit 1 within-session adaptation: psychological and autonomic outcomes

Significant threat × trial interactions were observed for all psychological and autonomic state measures except for attention toward task objectives (p-values ≤ 0.017; Table 2). Follow-up comparisons (summarized in Table 3) revealed that when individuals were initially exposed to the threat, they were less confident and more anxious, fearful, and physiologically aroused. Participants also directed more attention toward their movement, threat-related stimuli, and self-regulatory strategies, and less attention toward task-irrelevant information when initially exposed to the threat. For each of the aforementioned outcomes, the effect of threat was significantly reduced after repeated threat exposure. In all cases, the reduced effect of threat resulted from each psychological and autonomic state measure showing a rapid, non-linear attenuation across the block of HIGH trials (Fig. 2). While the effect of threat was substantially reduced, all outcomes, except attention toward task-irrelevant information, remained significantly different from LOWpost values (Table 3). For attention toward task objectives (which did not show a significant threat × trial interaction), there were significant main effects of threat and trial, such that individuals directed more attention toward task objectives in the HIGH condition independent of trial, but less attention toward this information across both threat conditions over time (Fig. 2; Table 2). Collectively, this pattern of results indicates that repeated exposure to the HIGH condition resulted in substantial within-session habituation of individuals’ psychological and autonomic response to threat.

Table 2 Two-way repeated-measures ANOVAs (threat: LOW vs. HIGH; trial: pre vs. post) examining within-session habituation of self-report, autonomic, and standing balance outcomes at visit 1.
Table 3 Follow-up pairwise comparisons for significant threat × trial interactions for two-way RM-ANOVAs examining within-session habituation at visit 1.
Figure 2
figure 2

Effect of threat and trial on psychological and autonomic outcomes. Data presented reflect group means and standard errors. The first two and last two LOW trials have been averaged (LOWpre and LOWpost, respectively) and are represented as open circles, while data across HIGH trials 1–20 are represented as filled black circles. Note, self-reported measures at the HIGH condition were only collected on trials 1-5, 8, 11, 14, 17, and 20 as illustrated. Att. = attention toward; EDR.freq = non-specific electrodermal response frequency; MP = movement process; SRS = self-regulatory strategies; TI = task-irrelevant information; TO = task objectives; TRS = Threat-related stimuli; VAS = visual analog scale. Hashtags indicate significant threat × trial interactions resulting from a reduced effect of threat as a function of repeated exposure.

Visit 1 within-session adaptation: standing balance outcomes

Significant threat × trial interactions were observed for MPF-COP, medium- (0.5–1.8 Hz) and high- (1.8–5 Hz) frequency COP power, tonic TA activation, and SOL-TA coactivation (p-value range: 0.001 to 0.025; Table 2). Follow-up comparisons (summarized in Table 3) revealed that when individuals were initially exposed to the threat, they demonstrated greater MPF-COP, medium- and high-frequency COP power, tonic TA activation, and SOL-TA coactivation. By the last HIGH trial, the effect of threat was attenuated for each of these outcomes but remained significantly different from LOWpost values. The reduced effect of threat resulted from significant reductions in each standing balance outcome occurring across only the HIGH condition (Table 3). Similar to the psychological and autonomic state measures, these standing balance outcomes demonstrated rapid, non-linear attenuation over the course of repeated threat exposure (Fig. 3).

Figure 3
figure 3

Effect of threat and trial on standing balance outcomes. Data presented reflect group means and standard errors. The first two and last two LOW trials have been averaged (LOWpre and LOWpost, respectively) and are represented as open circles, while data across HIGH trials 1–20 are represented as filled black circles. COP = centre of pressure; HF = high frequency (1.8–5 Hz); LF = low frequency (0–0.05 Hz); MF = medium frequency (0.5–1.8 Hz); MPF = mean power frequency; MPOS = mean position; RMS = root mean square; SOL-TA = soleus-tibialis anterior. Hashtags indicate significant threat × trial interactions resulting from a reduced effect of threat as a function of repeated exposure.

Significant threat × trial interactions were also observed for RMS-COP and tonic SOL activation (p-values ≤ 0.020; Table 1). Unlike the aforementioned standing balance outcomes, follow-up comparisons revealed that these interactions resulted from the effect of threat only becoming significant after the initial exposure to height (Table 3; Figs. 3, 4). When individuals were first exposed to the threat, RMS-COP and tonic SOL activation were lower, but did not significantly differ from LOWpre values (p-values: 0.191 and 0.441, respectively). However, by the last HIGH trial both outcomes were significantly lower compared to LOWpost (p-values < 0.001). For tonic SOL activation, this was the result of significant decreases in SOL activation occurring from the first to last HIGH trial, while for RMS-COP, this was the result of non-significant increases from the first to last LOW trials and non-significant decreases from the first to last HIGH trial (Table 3). This pattern of results indicates that the effect of threat for each of these outcomes does not diminish over the course of repeated threat exposure, but instead becomes more pronounced.

Figure 4
figure 4

Effect of threat and trial on tonic activation of soleus (SOL) and tibialis anterior (TA) muscles. Data presented reflect group means and standard errors. The first two and last two LOW trials have been averaged (LOWpre and LOWpost, respectively) and are represented as open circles, while data across HIGH trials 1–20 are represented as filled black circles. Hashtags indicate significant threat × trial interactions resulting from a reduced effect of threat as a function of repeated exposure.

For MPOS-COP and low-frequency COP power (0–0.05 Hz), there were no significant threat × trial interactions. However, there were significant main effects of threat for each of these outcomes (p-values < 0.001), such that individuals reduced the power of low-frequency COP oscillations and leaned away from the platform edge at the HIGH compared to LOW condition independent of trial. There was also a significant main effect of trial for MPOS-COP (p = 0.008), such that individuals leaned forward as a function of time independent of threat condition (Table 2; Fig. 3). Collectively, this indicates that the effect of threat for these outcomes does not diminish over the course of repeated threat exposure despite substantial reductions in the psychological and autonomic response to threat.

Visit 1 within-session adaptation for the “Adaptor” subgroup: psychological and autonomic outcomes

Fifteen participants (demographics summarized in Table 1) met the criteria to be identified as “Adaptors” (median trial at which point criteria to be fully adapted were met = HIGH8; range: HIGH3–HIGH20).

One-way repeated measures ANOVAs were significant for all psychological and autonomic outcomes (p-value range: < 0.001 to 0.041; Table 4). Follow-up planned comparisons demonstrated a significant effect of threat when individuals were first exposed to the HIGH condition (HIGHinitial vs. LOWpre) for all outcomes except for attention toward task objectives and task-irrelevant information. In particular, when initially exposed to threat, the “Adaptors” were less confident and more anxious, fearful, and physiologically aroused. In addition, these participants directed more attention toward movement processes, threat-related stimuli, and self-regulatory strategies. However, by the trial at which point they met the criteria for complete adaptation, the effect of threat for each of these outcomes was no longer significant (HIGHadapted vs. LOWpre) (p-value range: 0.094 to 1.00; Fig. 5).

Table 4 One-way repeated measures ANOVAs for “Adaptor” subgroup.
Figure 5
figure 5

Effect of threat and trial on psychological, autonomic, and standing balance outcomes in the “Adaptor” subgroup (n = 15). Data presented reflect group means and standard errors. The first two LOW trials for each participant were averaged (LOWpre) and are presented as open circles. The first two HIGH trials for each participant were averaged (HIGHinitial). The trial at which point the participant was identified as an “Adaptor” and the next HIGH trial were averaged (HIGHadapted). Both HIGHinitial and HIGHadapted are presented as filled black circles. Att. MP = attention toward movement processes; COP = centre of pressure; EDR.freq = non-specific electrodermal response frequency; HF = high frequency (1.8–5 Hz); LF = low frequency (0–0.05 Hz); MF = medium frequency (0.5–1.8 Hz); MPF = mean power frequency; MPOS = mean position; RMS = root mean square; SOL-TA = soleus-tibialis anterior; VAS = visual analog scale. Brackets with asterisks indicate significant planned follow-up comparisons (p < 0.025).

Visit 1 within-session adaptation for the “Adaptor” subgroup—standing balance outcomes

Significant one-way repeated measures ANOVAs were observed for MPOS-COP, MPF-COP, medium- and high-frequency COP power, tonic TA activation, and SOL-TA coactivation (p-value range: < 0.001 to 0.008; Table 5). For each of these outcomes, follow-up planned comparisons demonstrated a significant effect of threat when individuals were first exposed to the HIGH condition. For medium- and high-frequency COP power, tonic TA activation, and SOL-TA coactivation, the effect of threat was no longer significant once participants demonstrated complete adaptation of their psychological and autonomic response to threat (p-value range: 0.279 to 0.751; Fig. 5). However, the effect of threat remained significant for MPOS-COP (p < 0.001) and MPF-COP (p = 0.017). One-way repeated measures ANOVAs did not reach significance for RMS-COP (p = 0.084), low-frequency COP power (p = 0.183), and tonic SOL activation (p = 0.07).

Table 5 Two-way repeated-measures ANOVAs (visit: 1 vs. 2; trial: Δ HIGH1 vs. Δ HIGH5) examining retention and within-session adaptation of self-report, autonomic, and standing balance outcomes across visits.

Visit 1 and 2 between-session comparisons: psychological and autonomic outcomes

Two participants did not return for visit 2, reducing the sample of participants to 33 for these analyses.

No significant visit × trial interactions were observed for any psychological and autonomic state outcomes. However, significant main effects of trial were observed for all psychological and autonomic outcomes (all p-values < 0.001; Table 5) such that individuals’ emotional response to threat was attenuated from HIGH1 to HIGH5 independent of the visit (Fig. 6; Table 5). Significant main effects of visit were observed for most self-report outcomes. In particular, the changes observed between HIGH and LOW threat were smaller on the retention compared to initial session for confidence, anxiety, fear of falling, and attention toward movement and threat-related stimuli. No main effect of visit was observed for EDR.freq (p = 0.072) and attention toward self-regulatory strategies (p = 0.404), task objectives (p = 0.386), and task-irrelevant information (p = 0.146) (Table 5).

Figure 6
figure 6

Effect of visit and trial on psychological, autonomic, and standing balance outcomes. Data presented reflect group means and standard errors of change scores (Δ HIGH1 = HIGH1 – LOWpre; Δ HIGH5 = HIGH5 – LOWpre). Change scores for visit 1 and 2 are presented as filled black and grey circles, respectively. Att. MP = attention toward movement processes; COP = centre of pressure; EDR.freq = non-specific electrodermal response frequency; HF = high frequency; LF = low frequency; MF = medium frequency; MPF = mean power frequency; MPOS = mean position; RMS = root mean square; SOL = soleus; TA = tibialis anterior; VAS = visual analog scale. Vertical and horizontal brackets with asterisks indicate significant main effect of visit and trial, respectively (p < 0.05). Hashtags represent significant visit × trial interaction (p < 0.05).

Visit 1 and 2 between-session comparisons—standing balance outcomes

Significant visit × trial interactions were observed for MPOS-COP and tonic SOL activation. For MPOS-COP, this interaction resulted from individuals leaning non-significantly further backward from the HIGH1 to HIGH5 during the initial session (p = 0.210) and non-significantly further forward during the retention session (p = 0.021; Fig. 6). For tonic SOL activation, the interaction resulted from SOL activity showing further decreases from HIGH1 to HIGH5 during the initial session (p = 0.001), but not changing across these same trials during the retention session (p = 0.513; Fig. 6). Of the remaining variables, significant main effects of trial were observed for MPF-COP, medium- and high-frequency COP power, tonic TA activation, and SOL-TA co-activation. In all cases, threat-induced changes in these standing balance outcomes were attenuated from HIGH1 to HIGH5 independent of visit (p-value range: < 0.001 to 0.011; Table 5). A significant main effect of visit was only observed for high-frequency COP power, with individuals demonstrating smaller threat-induced increases in high-frequency COP power during the retention session (p = 0.008; Table 5). No significant interactions or main effects were observed for RMS-COP or low-frequency COP power (Table 5).

Discussion

The primary aim of this study was to determine if threat-induced changes in standing balance persist following near complete attenuation of individuals’ psychological and autonomic response to a height-induced threat. Consistent with previous work, when individuals were initially exposed to the threat, they leaned significantly away from the platform edge, significantly increased MPF-COP and plantar/dorsiflexor coactivation, and tended to decrease RMS-COP12. Changes in MPF-COP were the result of significant reductions in low-frequency COP power (< 0.05 Hz) and increases in higher-frequency COP power (> 0.5 Hz). With repeated threat exposure, individuals demonstrated a rapid and substantial attenuation of their psychological and autonomic response to the threat. However, only some threat-induced changes in standing balance tended to follow a similar pattern of adaptation. In particular, MPF-COP, higher-frequency COP power (> 0.5 Hz), and plantar/dorsiflexor coactivation were significantly reduced over the course of repeated threat exposure. Other threat-induced changes in standing balance, including the posterior lean and decreased amplitude of COP displacements and low-frequency COP power, did not change over the course of repeated threat exposure. This same pattern of adaptation was still observed in a subgroup of participants whose psychological and autonomic response to threat returned to LOW threat values. These observations suggest that while some threat-induced changes in standing balance vary closely with individuals’ psychological and autonomic response to threat, other balance changes may be employed in a given threat context independent of psychological and autonomic state.

A secondary aim of this study was to determine if standing balance adaptations are retained across repeated visits. Consistent with our hypotheses, individuals self-reported a reduced psychological response to threat upon re-exposure following a 2–4-week retention period. However, individuals’ autonomic response to threat, as measured by the frequency of non-specific electrodermal responses, did not differ significantly across visits. Furthermore, few standing balance outcomes showed retention, with only high-frequency COP power (> 1.8 Hz) demonstrating a reduced effect of threat at the time of the retention session. These results suggest that some standing balance adaptations acquired during a single block of repeated threat exposure can be retained, but retention of additional balance adaptations may require that the psychological, along with the autonomic response to threat, are minimized upon re-exposure.

Standing balance outcomes prone to adaptation following repeated threat exposure

Previous studies have shown that threat-induced changes in MPF-COP, high-frequency COP oscillations, and plantar/dorsiflexor coactivation are highly correlated with individuals’ psychological and autonomic response to threat17,21. Thus, it is not surprising that these changes in standing balance control appeared to change in parallel with measures of psychological and autonomic state over the course of repeated threat exposure and even returned to LOW threat values in the Adaptor subgroup. Collectively, this further supports the notion that these particular changes in standing balance are heavily dependent on individuals’ psychological and autonomic response to threat.

It is unclear if these particular threat-induced changes in standing balance serve a functional role when individuals are confronted with a threat to balance. Plantar/dorsiflexor coactivation increases ankle joint stiffness during standing, but there is little evidence this provides additional stability36,37. Rather, plantar/dorsiflexor coactivation is an energetically costly compensatory strategy38 that may interfere with voluntary and reactive postural control39,40. Some have suggested that increased ankle stiffness may improve postural recovery following perturbations, but only if accompanied by reductions in sensorimotor gain41. Moreover, some perturbations, such as support surface rotations, are more destabilizing as ankle stiffness and stretch sensitivity increase42,43. Since postural threat has been shown to facilitate muscle afferent11,44,45 and vestibular reflex gain46,47, such changes in coactivation may be maladaptive in this context. High-frequency COP oscillations may also serve a limited functional role, since COP movement greater than 2 Hz has a negligible influence on the control of sway during standing due to the body’s large moment of inertia48. If anything, these unnecessary movements may interfere with or mask balance-relevant somatosensory inputs49,50. Thus, it would appear favourable that these particular components of the behavioural response to threat are most prone to rapid habituation with repeated threat exposure.

Context-dependent standing balance changes

Previous studies have shown that when individuals are repeatedly exposed to height or the threat of an impending perturbation, some threat-induced changes in standing balance control are largely invariant17,18. This has been taken to suggest that some components of the behavioural response to threat may be adopted irrespective of the psychological and autonomic state changes induced by the threat. The present study supports this supposition and casts doubt on the possibility that the lack of standing balance adaptations seen in previous studies were the result of insufficient habituation17,18. This is because threat-induced changes in COP mean position and low-frequency power still showed little sign of attenuation, even in the Adaptor subgroup. Furthermore, threat-induced reductions in RMS-COP and tonic SOL activation tended to become greater over the course of repeated threat-exposure.

While these standing balance outcomes do not appear to be strongly influenced by individuals’ psychological and autonomic response to threat, previous studies suggest they are heavily dependent on the nature of the postural threat. Both the threat of height and impending perturbation elicit similar changes in psychological and autonomic state11. However, when standing with the expectation of an unpredictable forward or backward perturbation, individuals lean forward and increase their amplitude of postural sway15,16,51. This is the opposite to what is observed with a height-induced threat, where individuals tend to lean away from the edge of the platform and restrict their postural sway12. In both instances, these standing balance changes appear appropriate given the nature of the threat. Leaning backward and limiting postural sway reduces the likelihood of a fall over the platform edge when standing at height, while leaning forward and increasing postural sway may facilitate compensatory stepping and postural recovery when standing on the ground and responding to a perturbation51,52. Thus, these components of the behavioural response to threat appear to reflect context-appropriate adaptations to minimize the likelihood of a fall or injury in the face of unique postural challenges.

Retention of psychological, autonomic, and standing balance outcomes

When participants were re-exposed to the height-induced threat 2–4 weeks after their initial block of exposure, they demonstrated within-session adaptations similar to what was seen during their initial session. In particular, their self-reported and autonomic response to threat decreased over time, and this was accompanied by similar reductions in MPF-COP, higher-frequency COP power (> 0.5 Hz), and plantar/dorsiflexor coactivation. One exception was mean position of COP, as individuals showed a tendency to lean slightly forward over the course of the abbreviated exposure period during the retention session only.

One of the most striking findings was that few of the standing balance adaptations acquired during the initial visit were retained, with only high-frequency COP power (> 1.8 Hz) showing a smaller effect of threat at follow-up. This was likely a consequence of only modestly retained reductions in the psychological and autonomic response to threat. While individuals did report greater confidence and less fear, anxiety, and attention toward their movement when they were re-exposed to the threat after 2–4 weeks, their autonomic response, as measured by the frequency of non-specific electrodermal responses, was not significantly reduced. Spontaneous recovery of a previously habituated emotional response is commonly observed following exposure therapy53, and the exposure protocol used in the present study was not well-suited to minimize this54,55,56.

The present study used a massed and constant exposure protocol. This form of exposure was selected in order to maximize within-session habituation of the psychological and autonomic response to threat25,30. This was crucial to address the primary goal of this study, which was to determine if threat-induced changes in standing balance persist after near complete attenuation of the psychological and autonomic response to threat. Although effective in this regard, this form of exposure does not promote the encoding, consolidation, and retrieval processes thought to be critical for long-term fear reduction54,55,56. Furthermore, the exposure protocol was entirely unguided. That is, participants were not instructed to avoid using coping strategies, such as distraction or cognitive avoidance, which can facilitate within-session habituation, but limit long-term fear reduction55,57. Exposure protocols which explicitly limit the use of such strategies and vary the conditions of the exposure in terms of timing, intensity, and/or the context of threat presentations are more effective in minimizing individuals’ emotional response to a similar threat on subsequent exposures30,54,55,57,58. However, these types of exposure protocols were considered undesirable for the present study since they tend to result in sustained within-session anxiety and arousal54,55. Nevertheless, given that the standing balance adaptations acquired over the course of repeated threat exposure generally appear beneficial to the control of balance, future work should determine if improved retention can be obtained using different exposure protocols.

Limitations, implications, and future directions

A strength of this study was that the extended threat exposure protocol was sufficient to completely diminish the psychological and autonomic response to threat to LOW values in a subgroup of participants during the initial session. However, since only 15 participants met the criteria to be included in this subgroup, results related to these analyses are underpowered and should be interpreted with caution. Multiple factors may have contributed to between-subjects variability in habituation to the height-induced threat. Personality traits that have been shown to predict individuals’ response to height-induced postural threat59 were recorded and did not differ between Adaptors and Non-adaptors (Table 1). However, it is unclear how other personality traits or previous experiences with height-related activities may have influenced habituation in this context. The results of this study are also only generalizable to healthy young adults. Previous work has shown that young and older adults show a similar pattern of psychological, autonomic, and standing balance adaptations when repeatedly exposed to the threat of perturbation18. However, it is not clear if this is the case for different types of postural threat or with different clinical populations (e.g., Parkinson’s disease, stroke survivors, etc.). We believe it is important to first investigate the underlying mechanisms of emotional influences on balance in healthy individuals unbiased from any underlying fear or anxiety issues known to accompany balance disorders due to age or disease1,2,3. However, given the therapeutic potential of repeated threat exposure for minimizing fear of falling and fall risk in different clinical populations, future work is needed to determine if similar psychological, autonomic, and standing balance adaptations are observed in different clinical populations in response to unique threat scenarios.

This study also demonstrated that the emotional response to threat decreased rapidly with repeated exposure, such that the effect of threat for most psychological and autonomic outcomes was reduced by more than 50% within as few as five 90-s standing trials. Studies using postural threat manipulations need to take this rapid habituation into account, particularly if multiple conditions or trials need to be completed under a HIGH threat scenario. Since changes in the autonomic and behavioural response to threat are poorly retained across visits, studies requiring participants to be highly fearful or aroused across multiple threat conditions could be advised to complete their experiment over separate days to minimize confounds related to habituation.

Conclusions

This study demonstrates that while some threat-induced changes in standing balance are heavily dependent on individuals’ psychological and autonomic response to threat, and are therefore amendable to intervention, others are more resistant to change and appear to be employed in a primarily context-dependent manner. While the changes to standing balance observed following a single block of repeated threat exposure appear functional to the control of balance, they are not readily retained after 2–4 weeks without exposure. This is likely a consequence of inadequate emotional learning inherent to the massed and constant exposure protocol used in the present study. However, given the potential for exposure interventions to minimize potentially maladaptive changes in balance, it is incumbent upon future work to determine how to most effectively structure exposure exercises to maximize long-term fear reductions and associated balance changes. Such interventions may have the potential to reduce fall-risk amongst individuals living and/or working with a fear of falling.