Abstract
Interpersonal difficulties are often observed in major depressive disorder (MDD), while the underlying psychological and biological mechanisms have not yet been elucidated. In the present case–control study, a PC-based trust game was conducted for 38 drug-free MDD patients and 38 healthy controls (HC). In the trust game, participants invested money in a partner (trusting behaviors), and also rated each partner’s attractiveness (preference for others). In addition, blood biomarkers including metabolites were measured. Both MDD and HC males exhibited more trusting behaviors compared to females. MDD males’ preference for ordinary-attractive partners (lay-person photographs) was lower than HC males, whereas their preference for high-attractive females (fashion-model photographs) was similar levels to HC males. This tendency in MDD males could reflect a “focused (narrowed) preference for females”. As for blood biomarker analysis, the levels of 37 metabolites including acetylcholine, AMP, GMP, nicotinic acid and tryptophan were significantly different between two groups. Interestingly, among male participants, acetylcholine and nicotinic acid were negatively correlated with the level of focused preference for photographed females. In sum, we have revealed some behavioral, psychological and biological traits of trusting behaviors and preference for others especially in MDD males. Larger studies should be conducted to validate our preliminary findings.
Introduction
Major depressive disorder (MDD) is one of the most common psychiatric conditions observed worldwide1,2,3. MDD patients show not only depressive feelings but also loss of interests, difficulties in social interaction and cooperative behavior4. A previous study showed that low interpersonal trust was associated with both new-onset and long-term depression5. Therefore, exploring interpersonal aspects including trust is warranted to elucidate the pathology of MDD. Decision-making tasks have been highlighted in psychiatry as a method to understand the deeper behavioral traits beyond evaluating patient self-reported symptoms6,7,8,9,10. Trust game is a novel approach to investigate trusting behaviors, and is a kind of economic game developed in the field of social psychology and economics11. Trust game has been widely used to examine psychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia, borderline personality disorder, and depression12,13,14,15,16. Previous reports have shown some correlations between depressive symptoms and trusting behaviors using trust game17,18,19,20. Furthermore, recent studies, using trust game, have paid much attention to the biological basis of trust, indicating that oxytocin and testosterone influence trusting behaviors21,22,23,24,25.
On the other hand, blood metabolites have been suggested as a candidate objective biomarker of depression26,27,28,29,30,31,32. We have developed an original PC-based trust game, which evaluates not only trustworthiness to others but also preference for others using photographed partners’ faces with different levels of attractiveness19,33,34,35,36. Using the PC-based trust game, our previous research targeting university students has revealed that not only trusting behaviors (monetary scores invested to the partners) but also preference for partners (rating scores of the partners’ attractiveness) were associated with blood biomarkers such as serum high density lipoprotein cholesterol (HDL-C), uric acid (UA), high sensitivity C-reactive protein (hsCRP) and plasma fibrin degeneration products (FDP)36.
Loss of interest/pleasure is a crucial symptom of depression4,37,38. However, this symptom is usually assessed in accordance with subjective information provided by patients and objective evaluation tools are not yet established. Our previous study using the PC-based trust game with photographed partners’ faces has shown that trusting behaviors are influenced by attractiveness of the partners’ faces (Watabe et al. PLOS ONE 2015). However, it remains unclear how the attractiveness of the partners influences trusting behavior in patients with depressed individuals. We hypothesize that loss of interest/pleasure may alter attractiveness-driven behaviors in MDD patients. We expect that our PC-based trust game would be an appropriate tool to grasp such aspects by evaluating trusting behaviors and preference for others, both of which are relevant to pro-social and motivational aspects. Therefore, the aim of the present case–control study is to investigate MDD patients’ trusting behaviors and preference for others using our PC-based trust game, and is to explore the biological basis by evaluating blood biomarkers.
Methods
This case–control study was approved by the ethics committee of Kyushu University (30-452) and was carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.
Participants
Drug-free MDD patients and healthy subjects (age and gender matched controls) were enrolled in the present study as participants. All participants were Japanese (Asian) and provided their written informed consent prior to the study. Patients were recruited at Kyushu University Hospital and its related affiliations (mainly outpatient clinics). Healthy controls were recruited via flyers on Kyushu University Hospital and university campus. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR (SCID)39 was conducted by trained psychiatrists for the diagnosis of MDD. 38 drug-free MDD patients (22 males and 16 females) were selected for the present study. As exclusion criteria for MDD patients, we confirmed that all the patients had no history of the following diseases; neurodegenerative diseases, psychotic disorders, mental retardation, substance abuse, and physical diseases such as cardiovascular diseases, liver and kidney diseases, infectious diseases, malignant tumors, and head trauma. 38 healthy subjects were selected as healthy controls (HC) by interviews based on the SCID regarding any previous or ongoing psychiatric disorders, physical diseases, and medications, which were set as exclusion criteria.
Procedure
All participants completed a series of interviews and self-rated questionnaires, played a PC-based trust game, and provided a non-fasting venous blood sample.
Interview
SCID was conducted for all the participants. Severity of depression was assessed with the 17-item Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HAMD-17) by trained psychiatrists or clinical psychologists38,40.
Self-rated questionnaires
A Self‐report questionnaire evaluating the severity of depression was implemented using the Japanese version of Beck Depression Inventory-Second Edition (BDI-II)41. In addition, Yamagishi and Yamagishi’s trust scale (YYS) was conducted to measure respondents’ estimation of others’ trustworthiness42,43. Furthermore, 140-item Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI-140) was conducted to measure seven basic personality dimensions (four temperaments; Novelty Seeking, Harm Avoidance, Reward Dependence, Persistence, and three characters; Self-Directedness, Cooperativeness, Self-Transcendence)44,45.
PC-based trust game
As introduced in our previous reports19,36, we have developed an original PC-based trust game, which was conducted using a laptop computer to evaluate both participants’ trusting behaviors and preference for others. Participants (trustors) were instructed on the game rules. Then, the participants were required to make decisions regarding how much of 1300 JPY (about 12 USD) to give to each of 40 partners (trustees) and also to rate the partners’ attractiveness based on facial photographs presented on a computer screen (the score ranges: 0–9), which is thought to reflect raters’ subjective preference for others (Preference Score). The amount of money given to the partner by the participant (Monetary Score) is tripled, and the partner then decided whether to split the money equally with the participant or take the entire amount of the money. The participant’s decision as to how much money to give to the partner is thought to reflect their level of trust in the partner.
In this experiment, partners were virtual players on a computer screen. The participants had no information about their partners except for the facial photographs including the head and shoulders with a neutral facial expression. Photos of the partners were selected from professional fashion models (i.e., “high-attractive partners”) or lay individuals (i.e., “ordinary-attractive partners”). Regarding the photographs of lay individuals, photographs were originally taken for our game development with permission. We randomly selected 20 pictures (10 males and 10 females) to use in the present experiment. We also added 20 pictures of fashion models (10 males and 10 females) taken from a DVD with the same composition under permission by the publisher. This diversity of physical attractiveness was expected to have an effect on decisions in the trust game. All photographs are of Japanese. The participant was not aware of the partner’s decision. After the experiments, each participant was actually paid the amount of money corresponding to the result of a randomly selected game from all games as a reward.
Blood biomarkers
All peripheral venous blood samples were collected between 10:00 and 15:00. The plasma was immediately extracted and then frozen and stored at − 80 °C until required for analysis. Blood biomarkers measured included routine blood biochemical markers and blood metabolites (Metabolomics).
Routine blood biochemical markers including serum total-cholesterol (Total-C), high density lipoprotein-cholesterol (HDL-C), low density lipoprotein-cholesterol (LDL-C), fibrinogen (Fib), fibrin/fibrinogen degradation products (FDP), total-bilirubin (T-bil), direct-bilirubin (D-bil), indirect-bilirubin (I-bil), uric acid (UA), and high-sensitivity C-reactive protein (hsCRP) were measured by automatic biochemical analyzer (SRL, Inc., Tokyo, Japan). Plasma metabolites were measured by liquid chromatography–mass spectrometry (LC–MS) using LCMS-8060 (Shimadzu Corp., Kyoto, Japan), as described previously26.
Statistics
We first performed correlation analysis to assess monotonous relationships between monetary score, preference score, and self-rated questionnaires (YYS and each sub-scale of TCI-140). Pearson's product-moment correlation coefficient was calculated for normal distribution data; Spearman's rank correlation coefficient was calculated for non-normal distribution data, respectively.
Next, we performed ANOVA. Independent variables were participants’ gender (male, female) and psychiatric condition (MDD, HC). Dependent variables were monetary score and preference score for partners presented in the PC-based trust game. ANOVA was performed at four types of photographed partners (ordinary-attractive males, ordinary-attractive females, high-attractive males, and high-attractive females), respectively.
Then, we measured routine blood biochemical markers and plasma metabolites, and subsequent group comparison (MDD vs HC) was performed using Mann–Whitney U test.
Results
Demographics and clinical variables are summarized in Table 1. There was no significant difference in age between MDD patients and HC participants. MDD patients’ trust toward others assessed with a self-rated questionnaire (YYS) was significantly lower than that of HC participants. As for TCI-140 sub-scales, Harm Avoidance was significantly higher and Self-Directedness was lower in MDD than HC, which was consistent with previous reports46,47,48.
Monetary score (trusting behaviors)
To evaluate trusting behaviors, a PC-based trust game including four types of photographed partners was conducted for MDD patients and HC participants. Correlation of monetary score with YYS and TCI-140 sub-scales was shown in Supplementary Table S1. ANOVA of monetary score for four types of photographed partners was shown in Table 2.
Correlation of monetary score (Supplementary Table S1)
YYS score was positively correlated with monetary score in HC, whereas the correlation was not significant in MDD patients. As for TCI-140 sub-scales, Reward Dependence has shown significantly positive correlation with monetary score in HC males.
ANOVA of monetary score for four types of photographed partners (Table 2)
As a whole, both MDD and HC males showed greater trusting behaviors toward partners compared to females. Moreover, MDD patients tended to invest less money in ordinary-attractive photographed female partners and high-attractive male partners.
Preference score
In the trust game, preference score was measured by requiring participants to rate the partners’ attractiveness within a 10-point rating (0–9). Correlation of preference score with YYS and TCI-140 sub-scales was shown in Supplementary Table S1. ANOVA of preference score for four types of photographed partners was shown in Table 3.
Correlation of preference score (Supplementary Table S2)
YYS score was positively correlated with preference score in HC males, HC females and MDD females; whereas the correlation was weaker in MDD males. As for TCI-140 sub-scales, Reward Dependence and Cooperativeness were positively correlated with preference score in HC males.
ANOVA of preference score for four types of photographed partners (Table 3)
For ordinary-attractive partners, MDD males’ preference score was lower than that of HC males. On the other hand, surprisingly, MDD males’ preference score for high-attractive female partners was similar levels to HC males.
Focused preference index
This trait of MDD males might be caused by focused (or narrowed) preference. Thus, we defined the ratio of the mean preference score for ordinary-attractive partners to that of high-attractive partners as “focused preference index.” This index is higher when the mean preference score of ordinary-attractive partner is low and that of high-attractive partner is high. Scores of focused preference index were shown in Supplementary Table S3. Mann–Whitney U test was performed to investigate the focused preference for photographed partners (shown in Fig. 1 and Supplementary Table S3). Interestingly, MDD males’ focused preference for photographed females was higher than that of HC males (Fig. 1A). On the other hand, focused preference for photographed males was not significantly different between MDD and HC males (Fig. 1B). These results indicate that MDD males’ preference is focused (narrowed), especially for photographed females.
Group comparison of focused preference index for photographed partners. Dot plots show differences in ratio of preference for male and female partners (i.e. ration of focused preference index) between HC and MDD males (A, B), and HC and MDD females (C, D). Mann–Whitney's U test was performed for group comparison, and MDD males’ focused preference was higher than HC males (A). Horizontal bars represent the median values of each group. Male HC (mHC): N = 22; male MDD (mMDD): N = 22; female HC (fHC): N = 16; female MDD (fMDD): N = 16. HC healthy controls, MDD major depressive disorder.
Group comparison of biomarkers between MDD and HC
Plasma metabolome analysis was performed for all subjects, and 71 metabolites were successfully identified (shown in Supplementary Table S4). We also measured 11 routine blood biochemical markers related to lipids, blood coagulation, and inflammation (Total-C, HDL-C, LDL-C, Fib, FDP, T-Bil, D-Bil, I-Bil, UA, and hsCRP) based on our previous studies26,36 (shown in Supplementary Table S5). A comparison of the above blood biomarkers between MDD patients and HC participants showed significant differences among 33 metabolites including acetylcholine, adenosine monophosphate (AMP) and guanosine monophosphate (GMP), nicotinic acid and tryptophan (Table 4 and Supplementary Table S4), and 4 routine blood biochemical markers especially bilirubin (Supplementary Table S5).
Correlation of plasma metabolites with monetary score and preference score
Correlation coefficient of plasma metabolites with monetary score and preference score was shown in Supplementary Tables S6 and S7, respectively. Uric acid showed positive correlation with monetary score in both MDD and HC males, and also showed positive correlation with MDD males’ preference score. These results were consistent with our previous study36.
Correlation of biomarkers with focused preference index
As shown above, we introduced a “focused preference index” to explore deeper traits of preference for others. Thereafter, to investigate whether these blood biomarkers are associated with the focused preference index for the opposite sex, correlation analysis was performed. Interestingly, acetylcholine (Fig. 2A–C) and nicotinic acid (Fig. 2D–F) showed a negative correlation with males’ focused preference index for female photographed partners. On the other hand, these correlations among females for male photographed partners were not observed (data were not shown).
Correlation of biomarker with focused preference index in males. Scatter plot showing negative correlation of focused preference index between plasma acetylcholine (A–C) and nicotinic acid (D–F) in MDD and HC males. Linear regression lines with dot plots showing relationship between focused preference index and (A) acetylcholine among all males (N = 44), (B) acetylcholine among MDD males (N = 22), (C) acetylcholine among HC males, (D) nicotinic acid among all males (N = 44), (E) nicotinic acid among MDD males (N = 22), and (F) nicotinic acid among HC males (N = 22). MDD major depressive disorder, HC healthy controls.
Discussion
In the present case–control study using PC-based trust game, we have investigated MDD patients’ traits of trusting behaviors and preference for others, and have successfully identified some blood biomarkers relevant to these traits.
Gender differences in trusting behaviors
In the PC-based trust game, male participants invested more money in partners than female participants. These results are consistent with previous studies49,50,51, suggesting that males tend to show trusting behaviors compared to females. Such male trusting behavior might be partly explained by gender differences and socially accepted norms of behavior which place higher emphasis on male competitiveness and female interpersonal skills and empathy52,53. On the other hand, menstrual cycle may have effects in female participants. It is widely known that sex hormones influence emotions and a variety of psychiatric conditions especially depressive symptoms54, suggesting that sex hormones also influence social decision-making in females based on each participant’s menstrual cycle. In the present study, we did not ask about the menstrual cycle. Future investigations should be conducted with this point in mind.
Effects of partners’ appearance and focused preference
Previous studies have investigated the effect of partners’ appearance, which are mainly classified into trustworthy appearance13,55 and attractive appearance56,57. The latter is thought to be relevant to preference for others, since previous studies targeting autism spectrum disorders and borderline personality disorder reported that attractive appearance has impact on not only trusting behaviors but also preference for others regarding dependability56,57. As for the association between depressive symptoms and the attractiveness of others, research using the trust game is limited except for our previous research19.
In the present study, we have introduced the focused preference index, which can estimate the “picky” or “choosy” preference tendency. MDD males’ focused preference index for photographed female partners was particularly high (Fig. 1 and Supplementary Table S3). Overall, both MDD patients and HC participants showed more preference for high-attractive partners compared to ordinary-attractive partners. Interestingly, MDD males showed preference for high-attractive female partners about 3.5 times higher to ordinary-attractive females, indicating that depressed males tend to exhibit highly focused (narrowed) preference. This narrowed tendency of depressed males’ preference is likely related to cognitive flexibility. Cognitive flexibility refers to the ability to adapt to unpredictable and complicated surroundings, and plays a crucial role in social cognitive functioning58,59. Cognitive flexibility and its related social cognitive functioning such as empathy, theory of mind and facial affect perception are reported to be lowered in depressed patients60,61,62. Lowered cognitive flexibility might impair the ability to approve of others, leading to narrowed (i.e. focused) preference in depressed males. Interpersonal dysfunction including social withdrawal and alienation of others is commonly observed in depressed patients37,63, and focused preference possibly promotes such social dysfunction.
On the other hand, MDD males’ high focused preference might facilitate a novel therapeutic approach in MDD. Previous reports have shown that males generally show higher preference and motivation toward attractive opposite-sex faces64,65. Attractive faces are easier for visual processing66, suggesting that attractive appearance enhances the ability of MDD patients with impaired cognitive functioning or lowered motivation to be attracted. In addition, the ability to experience beauty is hindered by anhedonia (namely, loss of interest/pleasure), but not by depressive feelings per se67. Considering such factors, MDD males’ ability to feel opposite-sex attractiveness is likely to be maintained (or not impaired). Furthermore, high focused preference would encourage novel interpersonal therapeutic application for MDD patients. Also, the trust game might be a prospective tool for assessing not only trusting behaviors but also such focused preference. To elucidate this hypothesis, future research is warranted.
Biomarker analysis
A comparison of plasma metabolites and routine blood biochemical markers results showed significant differences among 33 biomarkers and 4 routine blood biochemical markers in MDD patients. These results partly confirmed our previous studies as to the characterization of depression biomarkers in plasma (i.e. tryptophan and alanine are lower in MDD patients)26,28.
Interestingly, nucleic acid-related metabolites such as AMP and GMP were significantly lower and correlated with the preference score in MDD patients. Nucleotides are the building blocks for DNA/RNA synthesis, for which blood cells including lymphocytes are highly required for their proliferation, maturation, and immune function68. Nutritionally, it has been established that dietary nucleotides are beneficial to promote the growth of infants and enhance the immune system69. Mice fed on a nucleotide-free diet has been shown to be vulnerable to infection, suggesting the reduced levels of nucleotides can affect homeostasis of systemic immunity68. The answer of why the nucleotides are lower in plasma with MDD patients remains to be addressed, and we suggest these nucleotides may be the key molecules to unraveling the long-mysterious relationship between depression and immunity.
Focused preference and blood biomarker
In the present study, we have identified that acetylcholine and nicotinic acid showed a negative correlation with males’ focused preference for photographed females. These results suggest that a decrease (or lack) of acetylcholine and nicotinic acid in the blood may be related to the characteristics of MDD patients who exhibit high focused preference.
A body of research points out the association between acetylcholine and cognitive flexibility. Cognitive flexibility is modulated by the prefrontal cortex and the striatum, and acetylcholine transmission facilitate the cognitive flexibility process in rodents70. In addition, deficits in cognitive flexibility in depressed patients are associated with hypoactivity in the medial prefrontal cortex71,72,73. Interestingly, activity in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex is increased when experiencing beauty stimuli including attractive faces74,75,76,77. These reports suggest that prefrontal cortex is associated not only with cognitive flexibility but also with focused preference in depression. Furthermore, the present result possibly supports the idea that acetylcholine modulates these social aspects. Further research using brain imaging and blood biomarkers is needed to elucidate the role of acetylcholine in focused preference.
Nicotinic acid also showed a negative correlation with males’ focused preference for photographed females. Nicotinic acid is a compound known as niacin (vitamin B3)78. Niacin deficiency is known to manifest various psychiatric symptoms including psychotic and depressive symptoms79. Interestingly, niacin is suggested to be a possible augmentation treatment for schizophrenia80. Although the role of nicotinic acid in depression remains unclear, a case report has shown that nicotinic acid treatment was effective in improving depressive symptoms for a patient with bipolar type II disorder81. Further research including randomized controlled trials for nicotinic acid nutrient treatment in MDD may help to elucidate the association between nicotinic acid and MDD.
Focused preference and microglia
By the way, we have reported that oral administration of minocycline, an antibiotic with inhibitory effect of microglial activation, to healthy males alters their trust-related behaviors using a PC-based trust game. We have found that the amount of money administered to photographed females, which increases in the placebo group, does not increase in the minocycline group34. Microglia, immune cells in the brain, play crucial roles in inflammation reactions, and microglial activation has been suggested to play important roles in the pathophysiology of a variety of psychiatric conditions including autism, schizophrenia, depression, and suicide82,83,84,85,86. We have proposed that suppressing microglial activation is a novel therapeutic target for depression87,88,89. Thus, the present results of focused preference for photographed females may be influenced by microglial activation in depressed males.
Both acetylcholine and nicotinic acid are linked to microglia. Microglia are known to express α7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptor (α7nAchR)90, and α7nAchR agonists are known to protect brain inflammatory reactions by acting on microglia91. A recent rodent study has shown that lifelong supplementation of choline (precursor for acetylcholine) ameliorates cognitive deficits by attenuating microglia activation via the α7nAchR92. Considering the above reports, we interpret the present outcome that lower acetylcholine in depressed males activates microglia, resulting in disturbing cognitive ability and inducing narrowed preference for females. On the other hand, in our previous metabolomic study, we have found that plasma 3-hydroxybutyrate (3HB), also known as β-hydroxybutyrate (BHB), is associated with severity of depression28. Recent rodent studies have revealed that 3HB/BHB has antidepressant-like effects93,94. Moreover, 3HB/BHB is known to inhibit microglial activation via the hydroxycarboxylic acid receptor 2 (HCAR2, also known as GPR109A)95. Interestingly, nicotinic acid (niacin) binds to the HCAR296. Summing up the above references and the present finding, we herein propose that nicotinic acid may regulate human social decision-making (especially preference-related behaviors) by acting on the HCAR2 in microglia. Future translational studies are expected to elucidate these novel hypotheses.
Limitations
The present case–control study has some limitations. First, our study has recruited relatively small samples, indicating selection bias. In addition, we did not conduct multiple test correction to avoid the risk of false negatives, as the purpose of this study was the global analysis of a number of blood biomarkers as an exploratory study for a future validation study. Despite the relatively small sample size, we have successfully detected several psychological traits and candidate biomarkers. Since candidate biomarkers such as acetylcholine and nicotinic acid have been identified, follow-up studies with greater sample size and multicenter recruitment should be conducted to validate our preliminary findings. Finally, we did not assess body mass index (BMI) and diet conditions in the present study. Levels of some blood biomarkers might be affected by BMI and diet conditions. Future studies should assess BMI and as to whether blood samples are collected under fasting or non-fasting conditions.
Conclusions
In the present case–control study targeting drug-free MDD patients, we have shown the prospective utility of PC-based trust game for evaluating not only trusting behaviors but also preference for others. We have introduced “focused preference index” reflecting narrowed preference tendency for others, and have revealed that MDD males exhibited high focused (narrowed) preference for females. This MDD males’ focused preference might encourage novel interpersonal therapeutic application. In addition, we have shown that two blood metabolites, acetylcholine and nicotinic acid, were associated with focused preference among MDD males. Focused preference might be observed not only in MDD but also schizophrenia, autism spectrum disorder, and personality disorders, thus our PC-based tool would be useful to understand such aspect in a variety of psychiatric disorders. Larger studies should be conducted to validate our preliminary findings.
Abbreviations
- α7nAchR:
-
α7 Nicotinic acetylcholine receptor
- AMP:
-
Adenosine monophosphate
- BDI-II:
-
Beck Depression Inventory-Second Edition
- BHB:
-
β-Hydroxybutyrate
- BMI:
-
Body mass index
- D-bil:
-
Direct-bilirubin
- FDP:
-
Fibrin/fibrinogen degeneration products
- Fib:
-
Fibrinogen
- GMP:
-
Guanosine monophosphate
- HAMD-17:
-
17-Item Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression
- 3HB:
-
3-Hydroxybutyrate
- HC:
-
Healthy controls
- HCAR2:
-
Hydroxycarboxylic acid receptor 2
- HDL-C:
-
High density lipoprotein cholesterol
- hsCRP:
-
High sensitivity C-reactive protein
- I-bil:
-
Indirect-bilirubin
- LC–MS:
-
Liquid chromatography–mass spectrometry
- LDL-C:
-
Low density lipoprotein-cholesterol
- MDD:
-
Major depressive disorder
- SCID:
-
Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR
- T-bil:
-
Total-bilirubin
- TCI-140:
-
140-Item Temperament and Character Inventory
- Total-C:
-
Total-cholesterol
- UA:
-
Uric acid
- YYS:
-
Yamagishi and Yamagishi’s trust scale
References
Vos, T. Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years lived with disability for 301 acute and chronic diseases and injuries in 188 countries, 1990–2013: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2013. Lancet 386, 743–800. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(15)60692-4 (2015).
Nishi, D., Ishikawa, H. & Kawakami, N. Prevalence of mental disorders and mental health service use in Japan. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 73, 458–465. https://doi.org/10.1111/pcn.12894 (2019).
Citrome, L. et al. Prevalence, treatment patterns, and stay characteristics associated with hospitalizations for major depressive disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 249, 378–384. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2019.01.044 (2019).
Segrin, C. Social skills deficits associated with depression. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 20, 379–403. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0272-7358(98)00104-4 (2000).
Kim, S. S., Chung, Y., Perry, M. J., Kawachi, I. & Subramanian, S. V. Association between interpersonal trust, reciprocity, and depression in South Korea: A prospective analysis. PLoS ONE 7, e30602. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0030602 (2012).
Gradin, V. B. et al. Neural correlates of social exchanges during the Prisoner’s Dilemma game in depression. Psychol. Med. 46, 1289–1300. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291715002834 (2016).
Pulcu, E. et al. Social-economical decision making in current and remitted major depression. Psychol. Med. 45, 1301–1313. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291714002414 (2015).
Fernandez-Theoduloz, G. et al. Social avoidance in depression: A study using a social decision-making task. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 128, 234–244. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000415 (2019).
Wang, Y. et al. Impaired social decision making in patients with major depressive disorder. BMC Psychiatry 14, 18. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244x-14-18 (2014).
Baeza-Velasco, C. et al. Decision-making in major depressive disorder: Subjective complaint, objective performance, and discrepancy between both. J. Affect. Disord. 270, 102–107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2020.03.064 (2020).
Berg, J., Dickhaut, J. & McCabe, K. Trust, reciprocity and social-history. Games Econ. Behav. 10, 122–142. https://doi.org/10.1006/game.1995.1027 (1995).
Hanssen, E., Krabbendam, L., Robberegt, S. & Fett, A. K. Social and non-social reward learning reduced and related to a familial vulnerability in schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Schizophr. Res. 215, 256–262. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2019.10.019 (2020).
Sutherland, C. A. M. et al. Appearance-based trust processing in schizophrenia. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjc.12234 (2019).
Fett, A. K. et al. To trust or not to trust: The dynamics of social interaction in psychosis. Brain J. Neurol. 135, 976–984. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awr359 (2012).
Liebke, L. et al. Difficulties with being socially accepted: An experimental study in borderline personality disorder. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 127, 670–682. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000373 (2018).
Unoka, Z., Seres, I., Aspan, N., Bodi, N. & Keri, S. Trust game reveals restricted interpersonal transactions in patients with borderline personality disorder. J. Pers. Disord. 23, 399–409. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2009.23.4.399 (2009).
Brendan Clark, C., Thorne, C. B., Hardy, S. & Cropsey, K. L. Cooperation and depressive symptoms. J. Affect. Disord. 150, 1184–1187. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2013.05.011 (2013).
Shao, R., Zhang, H. J. & Lee, T. M. The neural basis of social risky decision making in females with major depressive disorder. Neuropsychologia 67, 100–110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.12.009 (2015).
Watabe, M. et al. Relationship between trusting behaviors and psychometrics associated with social network and depression among young generation: A pilot study. PLoS ONE 10, e0120183. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0120183 (2015).
Wehebrink, K. S., Koelkebeck, K., Piest, S., de Dreu, C. K. W. & Kret, M. E. Pupil mimicry and trust—Implication for depression. J. Psychiatr. Res. 97, 70–76. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2017.11.007 (2018).
Ide, J. S. et al. Oxytocin attenuates trust as a subset of more general reinforcement learning, with altered reward circuit functional connectivity in males. NeuroImage 174, 35–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.02.035 (2018).
Krueger, F. et al. Oxytocin receptor genetic variation promotes human trust behavior. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6, 4. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00004 (2012).
Zak, P. J., Kurzban, R. & Matzner, W. T. Oxytocin is associated with human trustworthiness. Horm. Behav. 48, 522–527. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.07.009 (2005).
Buskens, V., Raub, W., van Miltenburg, N., Montoya, E. R. & van Honk, J. Testosterone administration moderates effect of social environment on trust in women depending on second-to-fourth digit ratio. Sci. Rep. 6, 27655. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep27655 (2016).
De Neys, W., Hopfensitz, A. & Bonnefon, J. F. Low second-to-fourth digit ratio predicts indiscriminate social suspicion, not improved trustworthiness detection. Biol. Lett. 9, 20130037. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2013.0037 (2013).
Kuwano, N. et al. Tryptophan-kynurenine and lipid related metabolites as blood biomarkers for first-episode drug-naive patients with major depressive disorder: An exploratory pilot case-control study. J. Affect. Disord. 231, 74–82. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2018.01.014 (2018).
Kuwano, N. et al. Neuron-related blood inflammatory markers as an objective evaluation tool for major depressive disorder: An exploratory pilot case-control study. J. Affect. Disord. 240, 88–98. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2018.07.040 (2018).
Setoyama, D. et al. Plasma metabolites predict severity of depression and suicidal ideation in psychiatric patients—A multicenter pilot analysis. PLoS ONE 11, e0165267. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0165267 (2016).
Gadad, B. S. et al. Peripheral biomarkers of major depression and antidepressant treatment response: Current knowledge and future outlooks. J. Affect. Disord. 233, 3–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2017.07.001 (2018).
Serati, M., Redaelli, M., Buoli, M. & Altamura, A. C. Perinatal major depression biomarkers: A systematic review. J. Affect. Disord. 193, 391–404. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2016.01.027 (2016).
Valkanova, V., Ebmeier, K. P. & Allan, C. L. CRP, IL-6 and depression: A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. J. Affect. Disord. 150, 736–744. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2013.06.004 (2013).
Yamagata, H. et al. Altered plasma protein glycosylation in a mouse model of depression and in patients with major depression. J. Affect. Disord. 233, 79–85. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2017.08.057 (2018).
Watabe, M., Kato, T. A., Monji, A., Horikawa, H. & Kanba, S. Does minocycline, an antibiotic with inhibitory effects on microglial activation, sharpen a sense of trust in social interaction?. Psychopharmacology 220, 551–557. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-011-2509-8 (2012).
Watabe, M. et al. Minocycline, a microglial inhibitor, reduces “honey trap” risk in human economic exchange. Sci. Rep. 3, 1685. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep01685 (2013).
Kato, T. A. et al. Minocycline modulates human social decision-making: possible impact of microglia on personality-oriented social behaviors. PLoS ONE 7, e40461. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0040461 (2012).
Hayakawa, K. et al. Blood biomarkers of Hikikomori, a severe social withdrawal syndrome. Sci. Rep. 8, 2884. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-21260-w (2018).
Kupferberg, A., Bicks, L. & Hasler, G. Social functioning in major depressive disorder. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 69, 313–332. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.07.002 (2016).
Hamilton, M. A rating scale for depression. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 23, 56–62. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.23.1.56 (1960).
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M. & Williams, J. B. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version Patient Edition (SCID-I/P New York, New York, 2002).
Furukawa, T. A. et al. Cross-cultural equivalence in depression assessment: Japan-Europe-North American study. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 112, 279–285. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00587.x (2005).
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A. & Brown, G. K. Manual for Beck Depression Inventory-Second Edition (The Psychological Corporation, New York, 1996).
Yamagishi, T. & Yamagishi, M. Trust and commitment in the United-States and Japan. Mot. Emot. 18, 129–166. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf02249397 (1994).
Yamagishi, T., Cook, K. S. & Watabe, M. Uncertainty, trust, and commitment formation in the United States and Japan. Am. J. Sociol. 104, 165–194. https://doi.org/10.1086/210005 (1998).
Cloninger, C. R. The Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI): A Guide to Its Development and Use (Center for Psychobiology of Personality, Washington University, Washington, DC, 1994).
Kijima, N., Tanaka, E., Suzuki, N., Higuchi, H. & Kitamura, T. Reliability and validity of the Japanese version of the Temperament and Character Inventory. Psychol. Rep. 86, 1050–1058 (2000).
Cloninger, C. R., Svrakic, D. M. & Przybeck, T. R. Can personality assessment predict future depression? A twelve-month follow-up of 631 subjects. J. Affect. Disord. 92, 35–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2005.12.034 (2006).
Nyman, E. et al. Impact of temperament on depression and anxiety symptoms and depressive disorder in a population-based birth cohort. J. Affect. Disord. 131, 393–397. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2010.12.008 (2011).
Zaninotto, L. et al. A meta-analysis of temperament and character dimensions in patients with mood disorders: Comparison to healthy controls and unaffected siblings. J. Affect. Disord. 194, 84–97. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.12.077 (2016).
Snijders, C. & Keren, G. Games and Human Behavior: Essays in Honor of Amnon Rapoport 355–385 (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers, Mahwah, 1999).
Chaudhuri, A. & Gangadharan, L. Gender differences in trust and reciprocity. (Wellesley College Working Paper, 2002).
Qi, Y., Li, Q. & Du, F. Are rich people perceived as more trustworthy? Perceived socioeconomic status modulates judgments of trustworthiness and trust behavior based on facial appearance. Front. Psychol. 9, 512. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00512 (2018).
Anderson, L. R. & Blanchard, P. N. Sex differences in task and social-emotional behavior. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 3, 109–139 (1982).
Eagly, A. H. & Wood, W. Feminism and the evolution of sex differences and similarities. Sex Roles 64, 758–767. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-011-9949-9 (2011).
Schmidt, P. J., Nieman, L. K., Danaceau, M. A., Adams, L. F. & Rubinow, D. R. Differential behavioral effects of gonadal steroids in women with and in those without premenstrual syndrome. N. Engl. J. Med. 338, 209–216. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJM199801223380401 (1998).
Chang, L. J., Doll, B. B., van’t Wout, M., Frank, M. J. & Sanfey, A. G. Seeing is believing: trustworthiness as a dynamic belief. Cogn. Psychol. 61, 87–105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.03.001 (2010).
Li, P., Zhang, C. & Yi, L. Brief report: Sensitivity of children with autism spectrum disorders to face appearance in selective trust. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 46, 2520–2525. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-016-2761-1 (2016).
Ebert, A. et al. Modulation of interpersonal trust in borderline personality disorder by intranasal oxytocin and childhood trauma. Soc. Neurosci. 8, 305–313. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2013.807301 (2013).
Armbruster, D. J. N., Ueltzhöffer, K., Basten, U. & Fiebach, C. J. Prefrontal cortical mechanisms underlying individual differences in cognitive flexibility and stability. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 2385–2399. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00286 (2012).
Dajani, D. R. & Uddin, L. Q. Demystifying cognitive flexibility: Implications for clinical and developmental neuroscience. Trends Neurosci. 38, 571–578. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2015.07.003 (2015).
Forster, K. et al. The relationship between social cognition and executive function in Major Depressive Disorder in high-functioning adolescents and young adults. Psychiatry Res. 263, 139–146. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2018.02.046 (2018).
Thoma, P., Schmidt, T., Juckel, G., Norra, C. & Suchan, B. Nice or effective? Social problem solving strategies in patients with major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Res. 228, 835–842. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2015.05.015 (2015).
Thoma, P. et al. Cognitive and affective empathy in depression linked to executive control. Psychiatry Res. 189, 373–378. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011.07.030 (2011).
Segrin, C. & Abramson, L. Y. Negative Reactions to Depressive Behaviors: A Communication Theories Analysis. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 103, 655–668. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.103.4.655 (1994).
Valuch, C., Pfluger, L. S., Wallner, B., Laeng, B. & Ansorge, U. Using eye tracking to test for individual differences in attention to attractive faces. Front. Psychol. 6, 42. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00042 (2015).
Levy, B. et al. Gender differences in the motivational processing of facial beauty. Learn. Mot. 39, 136–145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lmot.2007.09.002 (2008).
Hung, S. M., Nieh, C. H. & Hsieh, P. J. Unconscious processing of facial attractiveness: Invisible attractive faces orient visual attention. Sci. Rep. 6, 37117. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep37117 (2016).
Brielmann, A. A. & Pelli, D. G. Intense beauty requires intense pleasure. Front. Psychol. 10, 2420. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02420 (2019).
Gil, A. Modulation of the immune response mediated by dietary nucleotides. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 56(Suppl 3), S1-4. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.ejcn.1601475 (2002).
Pickering, L. K. et al. Modulation of the immune system by human milk and infant formula containing nucleotides. Pediatrics 101, 242–249. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.101.2.242 (1998).
Prado, V. F., Janickova, H., Al-Onaizi, M. A. & Prado, M. A. Cholinergic circuits in cognitive flexibility. Neuroscience 345, 130–141. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.09.013 (2017).
Anand, A. et al. Activity and connectivity of brain mood regulating circuit in depression: A functional magnetic resonance study. Biol. Psychiatr. 57, 1079–1088. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.021 (2005).
Bermpohl, F. et al. Attentional modulation of emotional stimulus processing in patients with major depression–alterations in prefrontal cortical regions. Neurosci. Lett. 463, 108–113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2009.07.061 (2009).
Koenigs, M. & Grafman, J. The functional neuroanatomy of depression: Distinct roles for ventromedial and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Behav. Brain Res. 201, 239–243. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2009.03.004 (2009).
Kawabata, H. & Zeki, S. Neural correlates of beauty. J. Neurophysiol. 91, 1699–1705. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00696.2003 (2004).
Ishizu, T. & Zeki, S. Toward a brain-based theory of beauty. PLoS ONE 6, e21852. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0021852 (2011).
Pegors, T. K., Kable, J. W., Chatterjee, A. & Epstein, R. A. Common and unique representations in pFC for face and place attractiveness. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 27, 959–973. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00777 (2015).
Wang, T. et al. Is moral beauty different from facial beauty? Evidence from an fMRI study. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 10, 814–823. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsu123 (2015).
Rennick, A., Kalakeche, R., Seel, L. & Shepler, B. Nicotinic acid and nicotinamide: A review of their use for hyperphosphatemia in dialysis patients. Pharmacotherapy 33, 683–690. https://doi.org/10.1002/phar.1258 (2013).
Rudin, D. O. The major psychoses and neuroses as omega-3 essential fatty acid deficiency syndrome: substrate pellagra. Biol. Psychiatr. 16, 837–850 (1981).
Xu, X. J. & Jiang, G. S. Niacin-respondent subset of schizophrenia: A therapeutic review. Eur. Rev. Med. Pharmacol. Sci. 19, 988–997 (2015).
Jonsson, B. H. Nicotinic acid long-term effectiveness in a patient with bipolar type II disorder: A case of vitamin dependency. Nutrients. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu10020134 (2018).
Suzuki, H. et al. Suicide and microglia: Recent findings and future perspectives based on human studies. Front. Cell Neurosci. 13, 31. https://doi.org/10.3389/fncel.2019.00031 (2019).
Kato, T. A., Watabe, M. & Kanba, S. Neuron-glia interaction as a possible glue to translate the mind-brain gap: A novel multi-dimensional approach toward psychology and psychiatry. Front. Psychiatry 4, 139. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00139 (2013).
Kato, T. A. & Kanba, S. Are microglia minding us? Digging up the unconscious mind–brain relationship from a neuropsychoanalytic approach. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7, 13. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00013 (2013).
Monji, A., Kato, T. & Kanba, S. Cytokines and schizophrenia: Microglia hypothesis of schizophrenia. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 63, 257–265 (2009).
Setiawan, E. et al. Association of translocator protein total distribution volume with duration of untreated major depressive disorder: A cross-sectional study. Lancet Psychiatry 5, 339–347. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30048-8 (2018).
Nozaki, K. et al. Antidepressant effect of the translocator protein antagonist ONO-2952 on mouse behaviors under chronic social defeat stress. Neuropharmacology 162, 107835. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropharm.2019.107835 (2020).
Kato, T. A. et al. Neurotransmitters, psychotropic drugs and microglia: Clinical implications for psychiatry. Curr. Med. Chem. 20, 331–344 (2013).
Horikawa, H. et al. Inhibitory effects of SSRIs on IFN-gamma induced microglial activation through the regulation of intracellular calcium. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 34, 1306–1316. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.07.015 (2010).
Shytle, R. D. et al. Cholinergic modulation of microglial activation by alpha 7 nicotinic receptors. J. Neurochem. 89, 337–343. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1471-4159.2004.02347.x (2004).
Egea, J. et al. Anti-inflammatory role of microglial alpha7 nAChRs and its role in neuroprotection. Biochem. Pharmacol. 97, 463–472. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bcp.2015.07.032 (2015).
Velazquez, R. et al. Lifelong choline supplementation ameliorates Alzheimer’s disease pathology and associated cognitive deficits by attenuating microglia activation. Aging Cell 18, e13037. https://doi.org/10.1111/acel.13037 (2019).
Kajitani, N. et al. Prefrontal cortex infusion of beta-hydroxybutyrate, an endogenous NLRP3 inflammasome inhibitor, produces antidepressant-like effects in a rodent model of depression. Neuropsychopharmacol. Rep. 40, 157–165. https://doi.org/10.1002/npr2.12099 (2020).
Yamanashi, T. et al. Beta-hydroxybutyrate, an endogenic NLRP3 inflammasome inhibitor, attenuates stress-induced behavioral and inflammatory responses. Sci. Rep. 7, 7677. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-08055-1 (2017).
Fu, S. P. et al. Anti-inflammatory effects of BHBA in both in vivo and in vitro Parkinson’s disease models are mediated by GPR109A-dependent mechanisms. J. Neuroinflamm. 12, 9. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12974-014-0230-3 (2015).
Wakade, C., Chong, R., Bradley, E., Thomas, B. & Morgan, J. Upregulation of GPR109A in Parkinson’s disease. PLoS ONE 9, e109818. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0109818 (2014).
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank Dr. Koji Tanaka, Mr. Keita Kurahara, Ms. Megumi Matsuura, Ms. Sakumi Kakimoto, Ms. Aya Yamada, Ms. Yoko Zushi, Ms. Akiko Hanada and Ms. Nagisa Kakimoto in Kyushu University for their research assistance.
Funding
This work was partially supported by Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research on (1) Innovative Areas “Will-Dynamics” of The Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science, and Technology, Japan (JP16H06403 to T.A.K.), (2) The Japan Agency for Medical Research and Development (AMED) (Syogaisya-Taisaku-Sogo-Kenkyu-Kaihatsu-Jigyo to T.A.K. & S.K. (JP17dk0307047, JP19dk0307073 & JP18dk0307075), and Yugo-No to T.A.K. (JP19dm0107095)), (3) KAKENHI—the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JP18K07417 to D.S.; JP26713039, JP15K15431, JP16H03741, JP18H04042 & JP19K21591 to T.A.K.), and (4) SENSHIN Medical Research Foundation (to T.A.K.). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
T.A.K. contributed to the conception and design. T.A.K. was responsible for protocol of the study. H.K., D.S., K.H., N.K., M.S-K., and R.K. T.A.K. contributed to the investigation. H.K., D.S, M.W., M.O., T.A.K. contributed to the data checking, analysis, and interpretation of data. H.K., D.S., and T.A.K. drafted the article, and S.K. and D.K. revised it critically for important intellectual content. All the authors provided final approval of the version to be published.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Kubo, H., Setoyama, D., Watabe, M. et al. Plasma acetylcholine and nicotinic acid are correlated with focused preference for photographed females in depressed males: an economic game study. Sci Rep 11, 2199 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-75115-4
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-75115-4
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.