Abstract
In cartilaginous fishes, variability in the size of the brain and its major regions is often associated with primary habitat and/or specific behavior patterns, which may allow for predictions on the relative importance of different sensory modalities. The Greenland (Somniosus microcephalus) and Pacific sleeper (S. pacificus) sharks are the only non-lamnid shark species found in the Arctic and are among the longest living vertebrates ever described. Despite a presumed visual impairment caused by the regular presence of parasitic ocular lesions, coupled with the fact that locomotory muscle power is often depressed at cold temperatures, these sharks remain capable of capturing active prey, including pinnipeds. Using magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), brain organization of S. microcephalus and S. pacificus was assessed in the context of up to 117 other cartilaginous fish species, using phylogenetic comparative techniques. Notably, the region of the brain responsible for motor control (cerebellum) is small and lacking foliation, a characteristic not yet described for any other large-bodied (>3 m) shark. Further, the development of the optic tectum is relatively reduced, while olfactory brain regions are among the largest of any shark species described to date, suggestive of an olfactory-mediated rather than a visually-mediated lifestyle.
Introduction
Cartilaginous fishes (Class Chondrichthyes) are the oldest lineage of jawed vertebrates and occupy a wide diversity of ecological niches, from shallow coral reef habitats within the epipelagic zone, to the deep abyssal habitats within the benthopelagic zone. Neurobiological variability of the peripheral and central nervous system across a wide range of species suggests these systems are under intense selective pressure to function within narrow environmental constraints1,2,3,4. In particular, the deep-sea represents the largest and one of the most extreme habitats on earth5,6, characterized by extremes in temperature, light, pressure, and biomass7. The environmental conditions of the deep-sea are known to drive unique adaptations in morphology and physiology, but little is understood about its effects on the nervous system8,9,10.
In recent years, a comparative approach to understanding brain evolution has provided great insights into correlations between complex brains and behavior. Jerison11 pioneered the hypothesis that an increase in brain mass conferred a cognitive advantage and proposed the ‘Principle of Proper Mass’, which asserts that the size of a given brain region will reflect, to some degree, specialized function of that brain region. Although Jerison’s predictions remain to be empirically tested to a great degree across any vertebrate group12, and more recent work has suggested neuron density may be more informative than brain sizee.g.13,14, comparative analysis of relative brain size (encephalization) and the relative development of various brain nuclei (brain organization) remain the most widely accepted neuroanatomical proxies for functional capability and sensory specialization. Indeed, correlations between brain organization and ecological and behavioral traits have been documented in nearly every vertebrate group, including mammals15,16,17,18, birds19,20,21,22, and bony fishes8,9,23,24. Although brain development is evolutionarily constrained25,26, variability in brain organization in cartilaginous fishes has been similarly associated with primary habitat, life history traits, and/or specific behavior patterns, even in divergent species that share commonalities in ecology27,28,29,30. At the peripheral level, significant differences in the size, surface area, and density of the peripheral sense organs of cartilaginous fishes have also been correlated with a range of ecological parameters31,32,33,34,35. This variability suggests there may be differences in sensitivity, acuity, and/or detection thresholds36,37, although the links between various peripheral characteristics and improved function remains uncertaine.g.38,39.
The Greenland (Somniosus microcephalus)40 and Pacific sleeper (S. pacificus)41 sharks are two closely related members of Somniosidae (Class: Chondrichthyes, Order: Squaliformes) and are the only non-lamnid predatory shark species to occur in the Arctic42,43,44,45. Given the extreme latitudes across which these animals are found, many aspects of their behavior, life history, and basic biology are poorly understood in comparison to other species46,47. Described as benthopelagic, Somniosus spp. often occur under the ice and at depths between 1200 and 2000 m48,49 and are among the largest sharks in the world. Reported maximum lengths for S. microcephalus range up to 7.56 m41,43, although this might be an overestimate, given that most reported sizes range from 2.88 m to 5.04 me.g.50,51,52; S. pacificus reaches total lengths of up to 4 m52. Although information on longevity is scarce, these sharks are also the longest-lived vertebrate currently described, with radiocarbon dating of eye lens nuclei suggesting that S. microcephalus lives as long as 392 ± 120 years53.
Among the most distinctive characteristics of S. microcephalus and S. pacificus is the presence of ocular lesions, generated by the ectoparasitic copepod Ommatokoita elongata attached to the shark’s cornea54,55,56, indicative of some form of visual impairment or even blindness. Despite what is presumed to be a diminished visual capacity and that locomotory muscle power is often depressed at cold temperatures57, such that the sleeper sharks are extremely slow swimmers58,59, S. microcephalus and S. pacificus remain capable of capturing active prey, feeding on a wide range of mid-water and benthic invertebrates and fishes52,60. Marine mammal tissue (including pinnipeds) is also frequently reported as a key dietary component in both of these species52,61,62,63. Although whale tissue is believed to be consumed as carrion62,64,65,66, characteristic bite wounds on living and moribund seals have been attributed to either S. microcephalus or S. pacificus, suggesting that both species prey actively on live pinnipeds52,62,67. It is assumed that the sharks may capture seals as they enter the water at ice holes46 or target sleeping seals68; however, it is currently unknown how they localize their prey.
Similar patterns of brain organization within taxa that share ecological or behavioral characteristics suggests that central nervous system morphology may provide insights into sensory specialization. Neural development in other deep-sea sharks reflects their unique environment, with a reduction in the relative size of visually-associated brain regions (i.e., optic tectum)69, and an enlargement of regions associated with olfaction (olfactory bulbs)29 and the octavolateralis senses (dorsal and medial octavolateralis nuclei)10,70, suggestive of a greater reliance on non-visual senses. However, few studies have quantified encephalization or the relative development of major brain areas within the Somniosidae10, despite their unique longevity, wide depth distribution, and unresolved predation strategies. Previous studies have suggested that S. microcephalus has a small brain71, with a well-developed olfactory system72,73. However, no study to date has examined the relative organization of all major brain regions in these sharks and contextualized it within a comparative framework.
Given the comparative neural dataset that now exists for cartilaginous fishes3,30,74,75, the aim of this study is to assess brain size and brain organization (Table 1) of S. microcephalus and S. pacificus within the context of a broad range of other species (n = 117), using phylogenetic comparative techniques. Due to the rarity of these samples, magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) was employed, which facilitates the acquisition of high-resolution 3D data of soft tissue structures. This study marks the first effort to gain an evolutionary perspective into the brain of Somniosus spp. in comparison to species that represent broad taxonomic and ecological diversity. Although not a functional analysis, assessment of the brain may provide insights into the relative importance of different sensory modalities in these unique shark species.
Results
Gross morphology
In both Somniosus microcephalus and S. pacificus, the brain (Table 1) occupies only a small proportion (<30%) of the endocranial cavity, although this is most pronounced in S. microcephalus. The brain of both of these shark species is long and dorsoventrally flattened (Fig. 1A–D), as in other squaliforms. A pair of long olfactory tracts (or peduncles) extend laterally from the telencephalon and terminate in the olfactory bulbs (OBs), which are attached peripherally to large olfactory rosettes. The OBs are divided into medial and lateral hemi-bulbs (Fig. 1A,B). The telencephalon (Fig. 1A–D), which consists of paired evaginated cerebral hemispheres, is similar to other squaliforms30,75. It is small in size, with a reduced dorsoposterior region, particularly the central nucleus of the dorsal pallium, in comparison to carcharhinid species, such as the silky shark (Carcharhinus falciformes, Carcharhiniformes) (Fig. 1IE). MRI data (Fig. 2) also reveals large telencephalic ventricles in the sleeper sharks, characteristic of squalomorph sharks76.
Brain images of a representative specimen of the (A,C) Greenland shark (Somniosus microcephalus) and the (B,D) Pacific sleeper shark (S. pacificus) in (A,B) dorsal and (C,D) lateral views. (A,B) show photomicrographs of one of two paired olfactory bulbs (OB) and the peduncle (olfactory tract), detached from the telencephalon (Tel) at the level anterior to the lateral pallium (dotted arrow), and the remaining brain (telencephalon (Tel), diencephalon (Di), optic tectum (OT), tegmentum (Tg), cerebellum (Cer), and medulla oblongata (Md)). For comparison, photomicrographs of the brain of the (E) silky shark Carcharhinus falciformis (Photo: T. Lisney) and (F) whale shark, Rhincodon typus (Photo: K. Yopak) in lateral view are presented. Olfactory bulbs not shown in (C–F). Scale bars correspond to 1 cm.
MR images of the brain of the (A,C,E) Greenland shark (Somniosus microcephalus) and the (B,D,F) Pacific sleeper shark (S. pacificus), including a (A,B) sagittal slice of the brain of both species and (E–F) digital segmentation of the major structures of the brain in lateral (C,D) and dorsal (E,F) views. Key: green = telencephalon (Tel), yellow = diencephalon (Di), grey = optic tectum (OT), cyan = tegmentum (Tg), blue = cerebellum (Cer), red = medulla oblongata (Md).
The optic tectum is comprised of two distinct bilateral lobes, where each hemisphere appears small in lateral view (Fig. 1C,D). The cerebellum consists of a central unpaired corpus and laterally situated auricles, where the corpus is small and lacks convolution. It is composed of rostral and caudal lobes, with only a very shallow central sulcus, in comparison to highly foliated species, such as the whale shark (Rhincodon typus) (Fig. 1F). The cerebellar auricles are continuous with the acousticolateralis area of the medulla (the dorsal and medial octavolateralis nuclei), which are moderately developed (Fig. 1A–D). Overall, the medulla is wide and open dorsally, a condition typical of most squaliforms and some lamniforms30,75.
Encephalization
The brain scales with negative allometry against body mass across cartilaginous fishes, as assessed using pGLS (α = 0.43, r2 = 0.69, p < 0.001, n = 117) regression (Fig. 3B, Table 2). Phylogenetically-corrected brain size residuals (Rbr) suggest that S. microcephalus and S. pacificus have a relatively low degree of encephalization (Fig. 3B). Phylogenetically corrected Rbr values range from −12.17 (Megachasma pelagios) to 100.43 (Sphyrna mokarran) (Fig. 3B). S. microcephalus shows a reduced brain size (GLM, Rbr = −0.60), which is most pronounced when phylogenetically corrected (pGLS, Rbr = −1.54), while S. pacificus has a relative brain size similar to other deep-sea cartilaginous fishes (pGLS, Rbr = 1.98).
(A) Regressions of brain mass with body mass across 117 species of cartilaginous fishes, showing the position of Somniosus microcephalus and S. pacificus (red circles), in comparison to seven other members of the Somniosidae (black circles), Oxynotidae (dark grey circles), and other species of sharks and holocephalans (light grey circles). Both Generalized least squares (GLS - solid line) and pGLS models (hatched line) are shown, with full model results shown in Table 2. (B) Phylogenetically size-corrected residuals, which range from Rbr = −12.37 to Rbr = 100.31 across 117 species, are presented for S. microcephalus and S. pacificus alongside five somniosids, two oxynotids, and Galeocerdo cuvier, Carcharhinus falciformis, Sphyrna mokarran, Megachasma pelagios, Carcharodon carcharias, Rhincodon typus, Istitius brasiliensis for broad comparison.
Brain organization
The relative development of the major brain regions (olfactory bulbs, telencephalon, diencephalon, optic tectum, tegmentum, cerebellum, and medulla oblongata), expressed as phylogenetically corrected residuals (denoted as Rstructure; see Table 1), was determined for S. microcephalus and S. pacificus as compared to 84 other species (Fig. 4). Olfactory bulbs (OBs) scale with negative allometry (pGLS, α = 0.97, r2 = 0.80, p < 0.001, n = 83, Fig. 4A, Table 2) across this dataset. Within the family Somniosidae, the OBs comprise between 11% and 33% of the brain, with a clear hypertrophy of this structure in both S. microcephalus (ROB = 2.96) and S. pacificus (ROB = 1.74) (Fig. 4B, Table 1) compared with all other species examined. Other close relatives showed only moderate enlargement of this structure, with residuals ranging from ROB = 0.01 (longnose velvet dogfish, Centroselachus crepidater) to ROB = 0.31 (prickly dogfish, Oxynotus bruniensis) (Fig. 3B). S. microcephalus has one of the largest OBs of any species of cartilaginous fish described to date (Fig. 4B). The telencephalon (pGLS, α = 1.04, r2 = 0.96, p < 0.001, n = 84, Fig. 4C, Table 2) and diencephalon (pGLS, α = 0.89, r2 = 0.92, p < 0.001, n = 84, Fig. 4E, Table 2) are highly predictable from overall brain size, with the telencephalon scaling with positive allometry. The telencephalon of S. microcephalus (RT = −3.59) and S. pacificus (RT = −1.99) accounts for only 17% and 16% of the brain, respectively, showing relative reduction of this structure compared to other sharks (average = 36.3%3). Other members of the Somniosidae, such as Centroscymnus owstonii and Centroselachus crepidater, show similar reduction in telencephalon size (Fig. 4F). The diencephalon is relatively enlarged in S. microcephalus (RD = 0.39) and average to reduced in S. pacificus (RD = −0.03).
Phylogenetic generalized least squares (pGLS) regression of the (A) olfactory bulb mass (C) telencephalon mass, (E) diencephalon mass, (G) optic tectum mass, (I) tegmentum mass, (K) cerebellum mass, and (M) medulla mass with total brain mass across 84 species of cartilaginous fishes, indicating the relative size of major brain regions for Somniosus microcephalus and S. pacificus as compared to members of the Somniosidae (black circles), Oxynotidae (dark grey circles), and other species of sharks and holocephalans (light grey circles). Model results available in Table 2. Phylogenetically corrected residuals predicted from total brain mass for the (B) olfactory bulbs (D) telencephalon, (F) diencephalon, (H) optic tectum, (J) tegmentum, (L) cerebellum, and (N) medulla are presented for Somniosus microcephalus, S. pacificus, and nine other species for comparison.
Scaling of the midbrain structures shows negative allometry for the optic tectum (pGLS, α = 0.87, r2 = 0.68, p < 0.001, n = 69, Fig. 4G, Table 2) and tegmentum (pGLS, α = 0.79, r2 = 0.82, p < 0.001, n = 69, Fig. 4I, Table 2). Midbrain development in the Greenland and Pacific sleeper sharks closely matches that of other somniosids, which possess a relatively average to reduced optic tectum [ROT = −0.04 (S. microcephalus) and ROT = −0.07 (S. pacificus), Fig. 4H] and a relatively average to reduced tegmentum [RTg = −0.34 (S. microcephalus) and RTg = −0.22 (S. pacificus), Fig. 4J]. The optic tectum comprises only ~2.5% of the brain in both species, a characteristic which is stereotypical of deep-sea dwelling somniosids (Fig. 4H).
Hindbrain development is notable in these two species of sharks, particularly in relation to the cerebellum. The cerebellum scales with positive allometry against brain size (pGLS, α = 1.06, r2 = 0.96, p < 0.001, n = 84, Fig. 4K, Table 2) and both S. microcephalus (RC = −0.61) and S. pacificus (RC = −0.47) have a relatively reduced cerebellum, both in comparison to closely-related shark species and across cartilaginous fishes (Fig. 4L). Although the cerebellum can occupy over 30% of the brain in other sharks (e.g. Rhincodon, Fig. 4L), this brain region comprises less than 13% of the brain in both S. microcephalus and S. pacificus, and occupies between 17% and 20% of the brain in other somniosids10. According to the foliation index scheme developed by Yopak et al.30, both S. microcephalus and S. pacificus have a foliation index score of 1 (Table 1), which corresponds to a smooth cerebellar surface, without any invaginations or folds, similar to other somniosids. An increase in foliation is correlated with an increase in cerebellum size (pGLS, α = 1.50, r2 = 0.56, p < 0.001, n = 84), brain size (pGLS, α = 1.54, r2 = 0.52, p < 0.001, n = 84), and body size (pGLS, α = 0.56, r2 = 0.24, p < 0.01, n = 84) across this group; however, Somniosus spp. do not conform to this pattern (Fig. 5). Modeling of foliation index score versus cerebellum size, brain size, and body size showed that these factors exerted significant influence on cerebellar foliation. When all candidate models were assessed, the global model (model 4: foliation index score ~ cerebellum mass + brain mass + body mass) was the best predictor of foliation index score, although cerebellum mass alone (model 3: foliation index score ~ cerebellum mass) also has substantial support (ΔAIC = 0.44, Table S3).
The medulla scales with negative allometry against brain size (pGLS, α = 0.85, r2 = 0.91, p < 0.001, n = 84, Fig. 4M). The medulla of S. microcephalus (RM = 1.21) and S. pacificus (RM = 1.00) (Fig. 4N), are average to enlarged, comprising 24% and 30% of the brain, respectively. In general, other somniosids and oxynotids have relatively large medullas in comparison to other cartilaginous fishes, occupying between 17% and 30% of total brain mass.
Discussion
Brain size and brain morphology may provide insights into shark behavior, particularly in species that are often difficult to study behaviorally due to their rarity and/or the extreme habitat in which they live. Of high ecological importance, the Greenland (Somniosus microcephalus) and Pacific sleeper (S. pacificus) sharks are the only non-lamnid shark species found in the Arctic, with a high prevalence of pinnipeds as a key component of their diet. In this study, the brain of these two unique species was examined as a way of predicting the relative importance of different sensory modalities. These sharks have a reduction in overall brain size and a marked reduction in the size of brain regions associated with higher cognitive functions, such as spatial learning and memory (e.g. the telencephalon77). They also possess relative reductions of the regions that receive the majority of afferents arising from the retinal ganglion cells (optic tectum) and one of the largest olfactory bulbs of any species described to date (comprising >30% of the brain), suggestive of a more olfactory-mediated, rather than a visually-mediated lifestyle.
As proposed by Jerison11, selection for larger brains (and/or enlargement of a particular brain region) are predicted to afford a functional advantage. However, little experimental evidence exists to empirically demonstrate this12,78,79. Thus, comparative studies of brain size and brain organization make the key assumption that there are correlations between brain regions and the functions or behaviors those regions modulate. However, correlatory evidence does not necessary reflect a causal relationship. It is important to note that this study is not a functional analysis; rather, it represents an attempt to use patterns of brain organization as a framework for exploring the relative importance of different sensory systems in poorly understood shark species. The extent to which morphological variation in the brain can directly confer differences in functional performance is of critical importance, but requires further study.
Within the cartilaginous fishes examined to date, S. microcephalus had a smaller than expected brain for its large body size (Rbr = −1.54; Fig. 3). The only other published account of brain size in S. microcephalus documented a specimen of 280 kg body mass that had a brain size of 10.29g71, which is in line with allometric expectations for the S. microcephalus specimens examined in this study. A relatively small brain is a common characteristic for other deep-sea shark species10, including other members of Somniosidae (Fig. 3B). As brain tissue is energetically costly, a relatively small brain may reflect its lower metabolic rate80,81.
Although the degree to which brain size reflects enhanced cognitive capabilities continues to be highly contentious82,83, it is a common suggestion that encephalization informs behavioral complexity to some degree84. Of the few studies to date to explore a direct link between brain size and a cognitive task, guppies (Poecilia reticulata) selected for a 10% increase in brain size outperform smaller-brained individuals in cognitive learning tasks, which suggests a larger brain confers some cognitive advantages12,79, with tradeoffs between these cognitive benefits and energetic costs80. Although not empirically shown, reductions in brain size in cartilaginous fishes have been attributed to a number of factors, including more opportunistic, passive predation strategies85,86, a close association with the substrate30,75, and lower activity levels (reviewed in3). These behaviors have been suggested to lend themselves to sensory and motor requirements that are likely less cognitively demanding, as compared to agile hunters that occupy more spatially complex habitats, such as coral reefs26,27,30. Across other vertebrate groups, species with larger brains similarly tend to exhibit behavioral innovations in the form of increased sociality, complex learning, tool use, and foraging ecology, than do animals with small brainse.g.17,21,78,87,88.
In addition to a low degree of encephalization, the brain of S. microcephalus occupies only a small proportion of available endocranial space. A relatively small brain is a common attribute of many mature large-bodied shark species, such as Carcharodon carcharias, Rhincodon typus, and Cetorhinus maximus30,86,89, with a correspondingly small brain-to-endocranial-volume ratio86,89,90,91. The low levels of encephalization in large-bodied sharks may reflect an evolutionary increase in body growth without a concurrent increase in brain size, as opposed to an absolute reduction in brain size (termed “gigantism”84). However, small brains housed within expansive crania are not a consistent characteristic across all large-bodied cartilaginous fishes. Some of the most encephalized species, such as the great hammerhead, Sphyrna mokarran30 and mobulid rays3,74, can grow up to body sizes of 6 m in total length (Sphyrna)43 or >7 m in disc width (Manta birostris92) and possess brains that are either tightly housed in the chondrocranium (Sphyrna; Yopak, pers. obs) or are situated within an expansive cranial cavity (Manta74).
Encephalization is also correlated with a high degree of maternal investment in cartilaginous fishes, which exhibit the most diverse array of reproductive strategies of any vertebrate group93. These strategies range from egg-laying (lecithotrophy), with no investment beyond the yolk sac, to live-bearing (matrotrophy), where the developing embryo receives additional provisioning from the mother. Matrotrophic cartilaginous fishes have brains that are 20–70% larger than lecithotrophic species28, where the increased provisioning from the mother may provide a developing embryo with a cognitive advantage at birth. Although our knowledge of reproduction in Somniosus spp is limited, they are known to produce a high number of follicles52,94, although litter sizes are relatively low in both species examined here, with approximately 8–10 pups per litter44. S. microcephalus and S. pacificus are believed to be lecithotrophic live-bearers46,52, where embryos are nourished via a yolk-sac and pups are live19. This low reproductive output and lack of additional provisioning from mother to offspring may explain the low degree of encephalization in Somniosus, but more work is required on the reproductive biology of these sharks.
Unlike S. microcephalus, the brain of the S. pacificus examined in this study is relatively large in comparison to other somniosids and oxynotids and average-sized in comparison to all cartilaginous fishes examined (Rbr = 1.86; Fig. 3). Although maturity was not assessed directly in S. pacificus, its body size (1.38 m TL; 25.6 kg) suggests this animal was a juvenile, as documented studies on the reproductive organs assert that S. pacificus matures at 3.65 m TL52. Cartilaginous fishes (as well as jawless and bony fishes, some amphibians and reptiles) experience indeterminate growth95. As such, their brains continue to grow throughout their lifespan, unlike other vertebrates that experience very little adult neurogenesis96. Although the brain grows through adulthood, the steepest period of growth is often during the early juvenile stages97,98, as seen in some jawless99 and bony fishes100,101 and reptiles102. Since the brain from an adult specimen of S. pacificus was not available for this study for comparison, the degree of encephalization documented in this study for S. pacificus may not be representative of the adult condition.
If natural selection is acting on a particular behavior or sensory characteristic of a species, there may be selective pressures similarly acting on the neural substratum that modulates that modality15,16. Previous studies on other members of Somniosidae have shown that they possess relatively small brains, with small telencephalons, small, smooth cerebellums10,30, and relatively large olfactory bulbs29 and medullas10,70. Like other somniosids, both S. microcephalus and S. pacificus possess a relatively small telencephalon and a relatively large medulla, which occupies between 17% (S. microcephalus) and 15% (S. pacificus) of the brain. S. microcephalus and S. pacificus have among the smallest telencephalons of any species described to date, including other deep-sea sharks and chimaerids10.
The telencephalon is implicated in multimodal sensory integration, complex behavioral control75,103,104, and higher cognitive functions, including allocentric place learning, avoidance learning, and long term memory77,105,106,107, as in teleosts108. In cartilaginous fishes, the telencephalon can comprise from 15–20% of the brain (e.g. in Harriotta raleighana and Deania calcea10) up to as much as 67% of the brain in Sphryna mokarran30. Given this variability, it has been proposed that telencephalon size may be indicative of behavioral complexity, whereby enlargement of this region is documented in shark species with increased sociality, strategic hunting, and navigating in spatially complex habitats3,10,27,30,74. The telencephalon is also one of the few brain regions to scale with positive allometry with brain size (Fig. 4E), such that larger brains become disproportionately composed of this structure, as similarly documented in mammals26. Functionally, lesions to various regions of the telencephalon, specifically the pallium, in some shark species have been shown to impair different types of learning105,106,107, which supports the assertion that the size of this structure likely reflects spatial learning and memory capabilities77. A small telencephalon in Somniosus is in line with phylogenetic expectations for this group and is also consistent with relative reduction of this structure in solitary species that do not dwell in spatially complex habitats30.
Olfaction is a critical sense in the aquatic realm and the detection of dissolved odorants is believed to mediate tasks ranging from predator avoidance, prey detection, and chemosensory communication with conspecifics (reviewed in2,109). Histological analysis of the peripheral system supports a well-developed olfactory capability in S. microcephalus, with a relatively high lamellar surface area and a high rate of renewal of olfactory receptor neurons (ORNs) in the olfactory epithelium72,73. The olfactory bulbs (OBs) receive primary projections from the ORNs and are associated with processing olfactory information33,38. Given the important role of olfaction in feeding, whereby many species follow odor plumes to their prey1,2, there may be selection pressures on the olfactory system for some species in this context. Both S. microcephalus and S. pacificus possess relatively large OBs (Fig. 1A,B), comprising 33% and 31% of the brain, respectively. In particular, compared across a range of other cartilaginous fishes, the OBs of S. microcephalus are among the largest of any species described to date (ROB = 2.96), rivalled only by the tiger shark, Galeocerdo cuvier (ROB = 4.89) and white shark, Carcharodon carcharias (ROB = 3.50) (Fig. 4B), both large-bodied, highly migratory predators. The OBs can vary considerably in size and morphology across species29,110 and show a high degree of statistical independence from the rest of the brain26,29, a pattern also documented across other species, including bony fishes, amphibians, birds and mammals26,111,112,113. Correlations between OB variability and ecology has been widely used to confer olfactory capability in vertebrates114,115,116, although uncertainties exist regarding whether peripheral and central organization reflects functional specialization in cartilaginous fishes38. Variation in OB size may not be indicative of functional variability, and instead may reflect a tighter coupling between other, more highly-interconnected, regions of the brain117.
Previous work has shown relatively large OBs in shark species living in conditions where the use of visual information is in some way compromised, such as the deep-sea, or in species (e.g. G. cuvier and C. carcharias) that may follow chemosensory cues over long distances to locate cetacean carcasses29,97,118. Like G. cuvier and C. carcharias, S. microcephalus and S. pacificus also have a high prevalence of marine mammals in their diet50,52,61,62,65. Pinnipeds can create considerable odoriferous material and the resultant odor trails can likely be tracked over considerable distances by large marine predators119,120. In this context, a well-developed olfactory system would be evolutionarily advantageous in the Greenland and Pacific sleeper sharks, who may be relying on chemoreceptive cues to prey on pinnipeds.
In addition to foraging and locating conspecifics, olfaction has been proposed to play a role in linking locations in olfactory space for both short and long-distance navigation121. Accordingly, Jacobs121 proposes OB size should co-vary with navigational demand29, supported by enlarged OBs documented in migratory birds122, bats with enlarged wingspans18, and mammals with large home ranges123. Although there is not a large enough dataset to test this hypothesis directly in sharks29, the largest OBs are, in fact, found in highly migratory sharks, including G. cuvier and C. carcharias (Fig. 4). Similarly, both S. microcephalus and S. pacificus occupy a broad depth niche from the surface down to 2200 m, and can make daily vertical migrations, in addition to long-range latitudinal movements48,49,58,124. Should a function of the olfactory system be to map patterns of odorants in olfactory space, a correspondingly large OB might support this behavior in somniosids.
The optic tectum in S. microcephalus and S. pacificus is notably reduced, and occupies ~2.5% of the brain; this relative reduction in tectum size is characteristic for deep-sea dwelling somniosids (Fig. 4H). The superficial layers of the optic tectum receive the majority of primary projections arising from the retinal ganglion cells and are associated with visual processing125,126,127, in addition to receiving projections from other sensory modalities128. Despite its role as a multimodal integration center, variability in the size of the optic tectum is suggested to reflect visual specialization in non-mammalian species8,69,88 and often scales with a number of other aspects of the peripheral nervous system in fishes, including eye size, retinal area, the number of retinal ganglion cells and optic nerve axons, and overall retinal area88,129,130. In addition to living in low-light environments, among the most distinctive characteristic of S. microcephalus and S. pacificus is the presence of ocular lesions, generated by the large ectoparasitic copepod Ommatokoita elongata, which attaches to the cornea54,55,56. This parasite has a very high rate of occurrence in the Arctic sleeper sharks; it is documented in the majority of specimens of S. microcephalus caught in East Greenland, Baffin Island58, Cumberland Sound, and Svalbard, Norway46. Histological assessment of the attachment of these parasites (and subsequent larval deposition and/or infection) shows corneal lacerations, damage, and visual impairment54,56. Although whether O. elongata fully blinds its host is uncertain, these animals may only be capable of light/dark discrimination56. Despite the potential visual impairment caused by O. elongata and a relatively small eye131, Somniosus spp. clearly remain capable of capturing active prey, including pinnipeds62,68. Previous work has proposed these sharks depend primarily on chemoreception to find prey from a distance and only use motion detection and visual cues at close range56. Patterns of brain morphology in S. microcephalus and S. pacificus, with relatively large olfactory bulbs and relatively reduced optic tectums, similarly support an olfactory-mediated rather than a visually-mediated lifestyle. However, experimental evidence on both the visual and olfactory system is required to confirm this.
Cerebellar development in S. microcephalus and S. pacificus is noteworthy. Although the function of the cerebellum has been an area of considerable speculation throughout gnathostomes132, it is generally agreed that the cerebellum regulates motor control and motor learning133. Morphologically, the corpus cerebellum varies substantially in size, level of convolution (a.k.a. foliation) and symmetry in cartilaginous fishes27,30,74,134,135,136, and it has been suggested that this may reflect performance differences in cerebellar-dependent functions and behaviors132. Both S. microcephalus and S. pacificus possess a relatively small, smooth corpus, with low levels of cerebellar foliation - a pattern consistent with that previously described for squalomorph sharks30,75. In contrast, high levels of foliation have been documented in active, agile predators, such as C. carcharias30, species with extreme motor specializations, such as Alopias spp., which uses a rapid strike of the upper lobe of the caudal fin30,118, or large-bodied species that make long-distance migrations, such as R. typus and C. maximus86,89. In contrast, low levels of foliation are common in small, benthic species that rest on the seafloor or benthopelagic species that inhabit the deep-sea, suggestive of lower activity levels10,30. Foliation has been proposed to be an identifying feature of highly encephalized brains26 and that species with larger, more complex cerebella might have the ability to perform more multi-faceted motor tasks than their close relatives lacking these convolutions137. Although the exact mechanism for this characteristic is unknown in sharks, cerebellar foliation likely allows for an increased cerebellar surface area, while reducing the length of neuronal connections26, which may provide an energetically efficient means of coordinating larger body sizes or may serve to improve motor agility at the central level.
To our knowledge, Somniosus spp. are the only large-bodied (>3 m TL) shark species examined to date that have a smooth cerebellar corpus. An increase in foliation is correlated with an increase in cerebellum size, brain size, and body size (Table S3) across this group, where S. microcephalus, in particular, showed a marked deviation from expectation (Fig. 5). The lack of cerebellar complexity may reflect a reduction in fine-tuned motor behavior26,30, as Greenland sharks exhibit the slowest swimming speed for their body size compared to other representative cartilaginous fishes59. Tail beat frequency data also suggests a sluggish cruising speed of between 0.22 and 0.34 m/s58,59 and a maximum speed of 0.74 m/s59 for S. microcephalus, which is much slower than those estimated for species such as C. carcharias120,138 and G. cuvier139,140. Importantly, acceleration during burst swimming speeds for S. microcephalus (0.008 m/s2) are much lower than those recorded in pinnipeds59, which makes this species unlikely to achieve the high swimming speeds necessary for active pursuit of seals.
Ferrando et al.72,73 have proposed that S. microcephalus has well-developed olfactory capabilities and might rely heavily on chemoreception during foraging, which would include predating on marine mammals. Although it has been considered whether seals are being fed upon as carrion, characteristic corkscrew wounds found on living or deceased stranded animals suggest these pinnipeds are still being bitten while alive in the water52,61,62,65. Whole, intact small seals that appeared to be healthy at the time of ingestion also support the assertion that the seals were taken live65. Previous studies speculated that S. microcephalus may actively feed at ice holes (e.g. on over wintering Delphinapterus leucas46) and could be attracted to seal ice holes via a suite of olfactory, acoustic and visual cues, possibly using stealth and camouflage to approach and capture seals at the surface58. Other research suggest that Somniosus sp. may ambush sleeping seals59,68. Arctic seals often sleep in water, either underwater or at the surface141,142. It has been hypothesized that this sleeping behavior allows them to avoid predation by polar bears (Ursus maritimus) that feed on seals primarily on the sea ice59,68. However, unlike cetaceans or otariids, who employ unique unihemispheric sleep patterns as a means to remain active while one half of the brain is in a sleeping state143,144, phocid seals exhibit bilaterally symmetrical (bihemispheric) sleep patterns, characteristic of terrestrial mammals141. Although protected from being hunted on the ice by U. maritimus, an immobilized state during sleep, may leave Arctic seals vulnerable to cryptic predators in the water, including Somniosus.
Conclusions
Although this was not a functional analysis, this study asserts that brain size and organization may provide insight into sensory specialization, particularly in data-deficient species that are often difficult to study behaviorally. The Greenland (S. microcephalus) and Pacific sleeper (S. pacificus) sharks are the only large-bodied predatory shark species to occur in the Arctic. In addition to occurring at depths up to 2200 m, these sharks often feed under the ice with a high prevalence of marine mammals, particularly pinnipeds, found in their stomachs, despite a degree of parasite-induced visual impairment. Although similar in many respects to other deep-sea squaliform sharks, patterns of brain morphology in the Greenland and Pacific sleeper sharks show unique characteristics. These sharks have a relatively reduced optic tectum and one of the largest olfactory bulbs of any species described to date, suggestive of an increased reliance on olfaction over vision. In addition, a notable characteristic of the brain of Somniosus is a small, smooth cerebellar corpus, the first to be documented in a large-bodied (>3 m) shark species. Taken together, brain morphology suggests a slow-moving predator with well-developed olfactory capabilities.
Methods
Animals
Brains from three specimens of Somniosus microcephalus (1 male, 2 female) were collected by the Norwegian Polar Institute (Table S1) in accordance with the ethical guidelines of the University of Windsor and the Norwegian Research Council. This project (Animal Utilization Project Proposal #07-11) was approved by the University of Windsor Animal Care committee, whose policies and procedures are designed to comply with those of the Canadian Council on Animal Care. Similarly, the project activities in Norway (within project 184644/S40/mo) of the Norwegian Research Council followed the rules and regulations of the national animal research ethics committee, under permits from the Governor of Svalbard. One specimen of S. pacificus (female) was collected on western side of Prince William Sound by K.J. Goldman of the Alaska Department of Fish and Game (ADF&G). This specimen was acquired as an incidental bycatch mortality during a routine trawl survey by ADF&G, and the brain donated for use in this study (Table S1).
Morphometrics (body mass, TL, FL, PCL) were collected for all specimens, where possible (Table S1). For S. pacificus, body mass was estimated based on published length-weight relationships47. Although S. microcephalus is predicted to mature at c. 3.0 m TL46,52, maturity was assessed in all specimens and none were mature. Maturity for S. pacificus was predicted based on published estimates for length-at-maturity values of c. 3.65 m TL52,145, although these are highly variable. Using these data, animals were assumed to be immature (S. pacificus) or sub-adult (S. microcephalus) (Table S1).
Tissue processing
Brains were exposed within the cranium and tissue was preserved in an aldehyde-based fixative (10% formalin in 0.1 M phosphate buffer). Following a period of fixation, brains were excised from the cranium and, following removal of the meninges, blood vessels, choroid plexa, connective tissue, and the cranial nerves, the brains were blotted and weighed to the nearest 0.01 g. Olfactory bulbs were separated from the peduncles and weighed. Brain mass was not corrected for fixation. Body mass information was recorded (or estimated from length-weight relationships) on fresh, unfixed samples after Yopak et al.30.
Imaging and segmentation methods
Given the rarity of these samples, brain organization was assessed non-invasively using magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). Following fixation, brains were transferred to 0.1M PB + 0.01% sodium azide for at least 14 days to remove excess fixative before transferring to fresh 0.1M PB + 0.01% sodium azide with the addition of 5 mM of the contrast agent Dotarem (for up to 1 week at 4 °C). MR image data was acquired from contrast-enhanced, fixed brains.
Brains were removed from the contrast agent solution and embedded in an inert imaging media (Fluorinert FC770). Imaging was performed on a Bruker BioSpec 94/30 US/R (9.4 T) small animal scanner, with an AVANCE III HD console, and using BGA-12SHP gradients (max 0.66 T m−1), a 72 mm ID H-1 quadrature volume RF coil, and ParaVision 6.0 software at the Center for Microscopy, Characterization and Analysis at The University of Western Australia. High-resolution (100 μm isotropic), T1-weighted images were acquired using a 3D FLASH sequence with RF spoiling. Images were produced with high contrast between gray and white matter in cartilaginous fish brains86,136, from which structural characteristics were derived (Fig. 2A,B). The pulse sequence parameters used for this study are shown in Table S2.
Three-dimensional data, acquired from high-resolution MRI, were digitally segmented using ITK-SNAP, a cross-platform, open-source application that includes a toolbox for manual delineation and a simple intuitive interface for user-guided automatic segmentation using an active contour (level set) algorithm146. Six major brain structures, the telencephalon, diencephalon, mesencephalon (sub-divided into the optic tectum and tegmentum), cerebellum, and medulla oblongata were individually segmented from the 3D data (Fig. 2C–F). Boundaries for these brain regions were identified using the criteria of Northcutt75, Yopak et al.30, and Yopak and Lisney69. The volume of each brain region as a proportion of the total brain volume was acquired from these digital segmentations and multiplied by total brain mass (less the olfactory bulbs) to acquire the mass of each structure, which was then compared across species.
The degree of folding of the cerebellum of S. microcephalus and S. pacificus was assessed using a visual grading index from Yopak et al.30. This method involved assigning a quantitative score (1–5) to the length, depth, and number of folds in the cerebellum (Table 1). A grade of 1 corresponded to a smooth cerebellar surface with no folding, increasing in complexity to a grade of 5, which corresponded to extreme foliation with deep, branched grooves30. Although not as quantitatively rigorous as other methods136, the visual foliation index score was available on the greatest number of species10,27,74, thereby facilitating a broad comparative analysis.
Brain mass and brain organization data from S. microcephalus and S. pacificus from this study were combined with comparable data on total brain mass (n = 117) and brain region mass (olfactory bulbs (n = 83); telencephalon, diencephalon, cerebellum, and medulla (n = 84)) and foliation index score (n = 84) from additional cartilaginous fish species27,29,30,71,75. A second dataset was compiled for published data on both optic tectum and tegmentum mass across 69 species69. Olfactory bulb data were not available for Rhincodon typus.
Quantification and statistical analysis
The scaling relationship between brain and body size across cartilaginous fish species was examined using log10 transformed data and the allometric relationship was determined using generalized least-squares (GLS) regression. However, GLS regression does not account for the relatedness of species and treats species values as statistically independent data points, yet closely related species share many characters through common descent rather than through independent evolution147. The use of GLS regression without consideration of underlying phylogenetic relationships can result in overestimation of correlations and result in Type I errors148. Therefore, allometric relationships were also estimated using a phylogenetic generalized least-squares (pGLS) approach for both brain mass versus body mass and brain structure mass versus brain mass149,150. We used a maximum likelihood approach to simultaneously estimate the phylogenetic signal in the model parameters and error structure149,151 using Pagel’s λ statistic150. A λ value of one indicates a correlation between species reflecting Brownian motion, while a λ of zero indicates no correlation between species150,152. Results of the pGLS model are presented together with the results from GLS analyses (Table 2) for brain versus body mass to enable comparison with previous research.
The phylogeny for the taxa set was created by pruning a larger 610 species molecular tree153 to the desired taxa set (Supplementary Fig. S1). Using this reduced tree, pGLS models of evolutionary change were constructed, using the caper154 and nlme155 packages in R156. From each model (brain mass ~ body mass) or (brain structure mass ~ brain mass), standardized pGLS residuals, or vertical deviations from the predicted slope, were also calculated using the phytools packages157. These residuals reflect interspecific variation in the relative size of the brain and each major structure, independent of phylogenetic constraints158,159 (Supplementary Figure S2). Goodness-of-fit measures (r2) were calculated using caper, which is estimated by comparing the goodness-of-fit of the candidate model to an intercept only model154.
Factors shaping cerebellar foliation were examined using pGLS in a model selection framework. Four candidate models were constructed: (1) body mass, (2) brain mass, (3) cerebellum mass, (4) body mass, brain mass, and cerebellum mass. We used Akaike Information Criteria (AIC) to identify the models that best explain our data from the suite of candidate models160,161. Model significance was based on a ∆AIC of 2, with the model yielding the lowest AIC value being the most supported.
Data Availability
The datasets generated and analyzed during the current study that have not already been provided in the accompanying tables are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
References
Collin, S. P. The neuroecology of cartilaginous fishes: Sensory strategies for survival. Brain Behav. Evol. 80, 80–96 (2012).
Gardiner, J. M. et al. In Biology of Sharks and Their Relatives, 2nd Edition (eds Carrier, J. C., Musick, J.A. & Heithaus, M. R.) (CRC Press, 2012).
Yopak, K. E. Neuroecology in cartilaginous fishes: The functional implications of brain scaling. J. Fish Biol 80, 1968–2023 (2012).
Finlay, B. L., Hinz, F. & Darlington, R. B. Mapping behavioral evolution onto brain evolution: The strategic roles of conserved organization in individuals and species. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 366, 2111–2123 (2011).
Danovaro, R. et al. Deep-Sea biodiversity in the Mediterranean sea: The known, the unknown, and the unknowable. PLoS ONE 5, e11832 (2010).
Ramirez-Llodra, E. et al. Deep, diverse and definitely different: Unique attributes of the world’s largest ecosystem. Biogeosci. 7, 2851–2899 (2010).
Collin, S. P., Chapuis, L. & Michiels, N. K. In Marine Extremes: Ocean Safety, Marine Health and the Blue Economy (eds Techera, E. J. & Winter, G.) (Routledge/Earthscan, 2019).
Wagner, H. J. Brain areas in abyssal demersal fishes. Brain Behav. Evol. 57, 301–316 (2001).
Wagner, H. J. Sensory brain areas in mesopelagic fishes. Brain Behav. Evol. 57, 117–133 (2001).
Yopak, K. E. & Montgomery, J. C. Brain organization and specialization in deep-sea chondrichthyans. Brain Behav. Evol. 71, 287–304 (2008).
Jerison, H. J. Evolution of the Brain and Intelligence. (Academic Press, 1973).
Kotrschal, A. et al. Artificial selection on relative brain size in the guppy reveals costs and benefits of evolving a larger brain. Curr. Biol. 23, 168–171 (2013).
Herculano-Houzel, S. & Kaas, J. Gorilla and orangutan brains conform to the primate cellular scaling rules: Implications for human evolution. Brain Behav. Evol. 77, 33–44 (2011).
Herculano-Houzel, S. The human brain in numbers: A linearly scaled-up primate brain. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 3, 31 (2009).
Barton, R. A., Purvis, A. & Harvey, P. H. Evolutionary radiation of visual and olfactory brain systems in primates, bats, and insectivores. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 348, 381–392 (1995).
de Winter, W. & Oxnard, C. E. Evolutionary radiations and convergences in the structural organization of mammalian brains. Nature 409, 710–714 (2001).
Hutcheon, J. M., Kirsch, J. W. & Garland, T. Jr. A comparative analysis of brain size in relation to foraging ecology and phylogeny in the Chiroptera. Brain Behav. Evol. 60, 165–180 (2002).
Safi, K. & Dechmann, D. K. N. Adaptation of brain regions to habitat complexity: A comparative analysis in bats (Chiroptera). Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 272, 179–186 (2005).
Corfield, J. R. et al. Diversity in olfactory bulb size in birds reflects allometry, ecology, and phylogeny. Front. Neuroanat. 9, 102 (2015).
Iwaniuk, A. N. & Hurd, P. L. The evolution of cerebrotypes in birds. Brain Behav. Evol. 65, 215–230 (2005).
Lefebvre, L., Nicolakakis, N. & Boire, D. Tools and brains in birds. Behaviour 139, 939–973 (2002).
Sol, D. et al. Evolutionary divergence in brain size between migratory and resident birds. PloS One 5, e9617 (2010).
Shumway, C. A. The evolution of complex brains and behaviors in African cichlid fishes. Curr. Zool. 56, 144–156 (2010).
Pollen, A. A. et al. Environmental complexity and social organization sculpt the brain in Lake Tanganyikan cichlid fish. Brain Behav. Evol. 70, 21–39 (2007).
Finlay, B. L. & Darlington, R. B. Linked regularities in the development and evolution of mammalian brains. Science 268, 1578–1584 (1995).
Yopak, K. E. et al. A conserved pattern of brain scaling from sharks to primates. Proc Nat Acad Sci 107, 12946–12951 (2010).
Lisney, T. J., Yopak, K. E., Montgomery, J. C. & Collin, S. P. Variation in brain organization and cerebellar foliation in chondrichthyans: Batoids. Brain Behav. Evol. 72, 262–282 (2008).
Mull, C., Yopak, K. E. & Dulvy, N. Does more maternal investment mean a larger brain? Evolutionary relationship between reproductive mode and brain size in chondrichthyans. Mar. Fresh. Res. 62, 567–575 (2011).
Yopak, K. E., Lisney, T. J. & Collin, S. P. Not all sharks are “swimming noses”: Variation in olfactory bulb size in cartilaginous fishes. Brain Struc. Func 220, 1127–1143 (2015).
Yopak, K. E., Lisney, T. J., Collin, S. P. & Montgomery, J. C. Variation in brain organization and cerebellar foliation in chondrichthyans: Sharks and holocephalans. Brain Behav. Evol. 69, 280–300 (2007).
Evangelista, C., Mills, M., Siebeck, U. E. & Collin, S. P. A comparison of the external morphology of the membranous inner ear in elasmobranchs. J Morph 271, 483–495 (2010).
Lisney, T. J. & Collin, S. P. Relative eye size in elasmobranchs. Brain Behav. Evol. 69, 266–279 (2007).
Schluessel, V. et al. Morphometric and untrastructural comparison of the olfactory system in elasmobranchs: The significance of structure-function relationships based on phylogeny and ecology. J. Morph. 269, 1365–1386 (2008).
Theiss, S., Hart, N. S. & Collin, S. P. B. Behavior, and Evolution. 73: 91–101. Morphological indicators of olfactory capability in wobbegong sharks (Orectolobidae, Elasmobranchii). Brain Behav. Evol. 73, 91–101 (2009).
Jordan, L. K. Comparative morphology of stingray lateral line canal and electrosensory systems. J. Morph. 269, 1325–1339 (2008).
Jordan, L. K., Kajiura, S. M. & Gordon, M. S. Functional consequences of structural differences in stingray sensory systems. Part I: Mechanosensory lateral line canals. J Exp Biol 212, 3037–3043 (2009).
Jordan, L. K., Kajiura, S. M. & Gordon, M. S. Functional consequences of structural differences in stingray sensory systems. Part II: Electrosensory system. J. Exp. Biol. 212, 3044–3050 (2009).
Meredith, T. L. & Kajiura, S. M. Olfactory morphology and physiology of elasmobranchs. J. Exp. Bio.l 213, 3449–3456 (2010).
Bedore, C., Harris, L. & Kajiura, S. Behavioral responses of batoid elasmobranchs to prey-simulating electric fields are correlated to peripheral sensory morphology and ecology. Zoology 117, 95–103 (2014).
Bloch, M. & Schneider, J. M.E. Blochii, systema ichthyologiae iconibus cx illustratum. Post obitum auctoris opus inchoatum absolvit, correxit, interpolavit Jo. Gottlob Schneider, Saxo. Berolini, Sumtibus Austoris Impressum et Bibliopolio Sanderiano Commissum. 584 (1801).
Bigelow, H. & Schroeder, W. New sharks from the Western North Atlantic. Proc. New Eng. Zool.Club 23, 21–36 (1944).
Benz, G. W. et al. A second species of Arctic shark: Pacific sleeper shark Somniosus pacificus from Point Hope, Alaska. Polar Biol. 27, 250–252 (2004).
Compagno, L. J. V. FAO Species Catalogue. Sharks of the world. An annotated and illustrated catalogue of shark species known to date. I. Hexanchiformes to Lamniformes. Vol. 4 (FAO Fisheries Synopsis, 1984).
Compagno, L. J. V., Dando, M. & Fowler, S. L. Sharks of the World. (Harper Collins, 2005).
Yano, K., Stevens, J. D. & Compagno, L. J. V. A review of the systematics of the sleeper shark genus Somniosus with redescriptions of Somniosus antaracticus and S. (Rhinoscymnus) longus (Elasmobranchii; Squaliformes Somniosidae). Ichthy. Res. 51, 360–373 (2004).
MacNeil, M. A. et al. Biology of the Greenland shark Somniosus microcephalus. J. Fish Biol. 80, 991–1018 (2012).
Orlov, A. & Baitalyuk, A. Spatial distribution and features of biology of Pacific sleeper shark Somniosus pacificus in the North Pacific. J. Ichthyol. 54, 526–546 (2014).
Campana, S. E., Fisk, A. T. & Klimley, A. P. Movements of Arctic and northwest Atlantic Greenland sharks (Somniosus microcephalus) monitored with archival satellite pop-up tags suggest long-range migrations. Deep Sea Research Part II: Topical Studies in Oceanography 115, 109–115 (2015).
Fisk, A. T., Lydersen, C. & Kovacs, K. M. Archival pop-off tag tracking of Greenland sharks (Somniosus microcephalus) in the high Arctic waters of Svalbard, Norway. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 468, 255–265 (2012).
Fisk, A. T., Tittlemier, S. A., Pranschke, J. L. & Norstrom, R. J. Using anthropogenic contaminants and stable isotopes to assess the feeding ecology of Greenland sharks. Ecology 83, 2162–2172 (2002).
Nielsen, J., Hedeholm, R. B., Simon, M. & Steffensen, J. F. Distribution and feeding ecology of the Greenland shark (Somniosus microcephalus) in Greenland waters. Polar Biol. 37, 37–46 (2014).
Yano, K., Stevens, J. & Compagno, L. Distribution, reproduction and feeding of the Greenland shark Somniosus microcephalus, with notes on two other sleeper sharks, Somniosus pacificus and Somniosus antarcticus. J. Fish Biol. 70, 374–390 (2007).
Nielsen, J. et al. Eye lens radiocarbon reveals centuries of longevity in the Greenland shark (Somniosus microcephalus). Science 353, 702–704 (2016).
Benz, G. W., Borucinska, J. D., Lowry, L. F. & Whiteley, H. E. Ocular lesions associated with attachment of the copepod Ommatokoita elongata (Lernaeopodidae, Siphonostomatoida) to corneas of Pacific sleeper sharks Somniosus pacificus captured off Alaska in Prince William Sound. J. Parasitol. 88, 474–481 (2002).
Benz, G. W., Lucas, Z. & Lowry, L. F. New host and ocean records for the copepod Ommatokoita elongata (Siphonostomatoida, Lernaeopodidae), a parasite of the eyes of sleeper sharks. J. Parasitol. 84, 1271–1274 (1998).
Borucinska, J. D., Whiteley, H. E. & Benz, G. W. Ocular lesions associated with attachment of the parasitic copepod Ommattokoita elongata (Grant) to corneas of Greenland shark Somniosus microcephalus (Bloch and Schneider). J. Fish Dis. 21, 415–422 (1998).
Wardle, C. S. In Environmental Physiology of Fishes (ed. Ali, M. A.) 519–531 (Plenum Press, 1980).
Skomal, G. B. & Benz, G. W. Ultrasonic tracking of Greenland sharks, Somniosus microcephalus, under Arctic ice. Marine Biol. 145, 89–498 (2004).
Watanabe, Y. Y., Lydersen, C., Fisk, A. T. & Kovacs, K. M. The slowest fish: Swim speed and tail-beat frequency of Greenland sharks. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 426-427, 5–11 (2012).
Yang, M. & Page, B. N. Diet of Pacific sleeper shark, Somniosus pacificus, in the Gulf of Alaska. Fish. Bull. 97, 406–409 (1999).
Lucas, Z. & Natanson, L. Two shark species involved in predation on seals at Sable Island, Nova Scotia, Canada. Proc. Nova Scotian Institute of Science 45, 64–88 (2010).
Sigler, M. F. et al. Diet of Pacific sleeper sharks, a potential Steller sea lion predator, in the northeast Pacific Ocean. J. Fish Biol. 69, 392–405 (2006).
McMeans, B. C. et al. The role of Greenland sharks (Somniosus microcephalus) in an Arctic ecosystem: assessed via stable isotopes and fatty acids. Marine Biol. 160, 1223–1238 (2013).
Leclerc, L. M. et al. Greenland sharks (Somniosus microcephalus) scavenge offal from minke (Balaenoptera acutorostrata) whaling operations in Svalbard (Norway). Polar Res. 30, 7342, https://doi.org/10.3402/polar.v30o0.7342 (2011).
Leclerc, L. M. E. et al. A missing puzzle piece in Arctic food web puzzle? Stomach contents of Greenland Sharks sampled off Svalbard, Norway. Polar Biol. 35, 1197–1208 (2012).
Smith, C. R. & Baco, A. R. Ecology of whale falls at the deep-sea floor. Oceanography and Marine Biology: an Annual Review 41, 311–354 (2003).
van den Hoff, J. & Morrice, M. G. Sleeper shark (Somniosus antarcticus) and other bite wounds observed on southern elephant seals (Mirounga leonina) at Macquarie Island. Mar. Mamm. Sci. 24, 239–247 (2008).
Lydersen, C., Fisk, A. T. & Kovacs, K. M. A review of Greenland shark (Somniosus microcephalus) studies in the Kongsfjorden area, Svalbard Norway. Polar Biol. 39, 2169–2178 (2016).
Yopak, K. E. & Lisney, T. J. Allometric scaling of the optic tectum in cartilaginous fishes. Brain Behav. Evol. 80, 108–126 (2012).
Kajiura, S. M., Cornett, A. D. & Yopak, K. E. In Sharks and Their Relatives II: Biodiversity, Adaptive Physiology, and Conservation (eds Jeffry C. Carrier, John A. Musick, & Michael, R. Heithaus) (CRC Press, 2010).
Myagkov, N. A. The brain sizes of living Elasmobranchii as their organization level indicator. I. General Analysis. J. Hinforsch. 32, 553–561 (1991).
Ferrando, S. et al. Anatomy of the olfactory bulb in Greenland shark Somniosus microcephalus (Bloch & Schneider, 1801). J. Applied Ichthyol. 33, 263–269 (2017).
Ferrando, S. et al. Gross morphology and histology of the olfactory organ of the Greenland shark Somniosus microcephalus. Polar Biology 39, 1399–1409 (2016).
Ari, C. Encephalization and brain organization of mobulid rays (Myliobatiformes, Elasmobranchii) with ecological perspectives. Open Anat. J. 3, 1–13 (2011).
Northcutt, R. G. In Sensory Biology of Sharks, Skates, and Rays (eds Hodgson, E. S. & Mathewson, R. F.) 117–194 (Office of Naval Research, 1978).
Northcutt, R. G. Elasmobranch central nervous system organization and its possible evolutionary significance. Am. Zool. 17, 411–429 (1977).
Schluessel, V. Who would have thought that ‘Jaws’ also has brains? Cognitive functions in elasmobranchs. Anim. Cogn. 18, 19–37 (2015).
Benson-Amrama, S. et al. Brain size predicts problem-solving ability in mammalian carnivores. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. 113, 2532–2537 (2016).
Kotrschal, A., Corral-Lopez, A., Amcoff, M. & Kolm, N. A larger brain confers a benefit in a spatial mate search learning task in male guppies. Behav. Ecol. 26, 527–532 (2015).
Isler, K. Brain size evolution: How fish pay for being smart. Curr. Biol. 23, R63–R65 (2013).
Martin, R. D. Relative brain size and basal metabolic rate in terrestrial vertebrates. Nature 293, 57–60 (1981).
Healy, S. D. & Rowe, C. A critique of comparative studies of brain size. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 274, 453–464 (2007).
Healy, S. & Rowe, C. Costs and benefits of evolving a larger brain: Doubts over the evidence that large brains lead to better cognition. Anim. Behav. 86, e1–e3 (2013).
Striedter, G. F. Principles of Brain Evolution. Sinauer Associates, Inc. (2005).
Nilsson, G. E., Routley, M. H. & Renshaw, G. M. C. Low mass-specific brain Na+/K+-ATPase activity in elasmobranch compared to teleost fishes: Implications for the large brain size of elasmobranchs. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 267, 1335–1339 (2000).
Yopak, K. E. & Frank, L. R. B. Size and Brain Organization of the Whale Shark, Rhincodon typus, using Magnetic Resonance Imaging. Brain Behav. Evol. 74, 121–142 (2009).
Iwaniuk, A. N., Nelson, J. E. & Pellis, S. M. Do big-brained animals play more? Comparative analyses of play and relative brain size in mammals. J. Comp. Physiol. 115, 29–41 (2001).
Kotrschal, K., van Staaden, M. J. & Huber, R. Fish brains: Evolution and environmental relationships. Rev. Fish Biol. Fish. 8, 373–408 (1998).
Kruska, D. C. T. The brain of the basking shark (Cetorhinus maximus). Brain Behav. Evol. 32, 353–363 (1988).
Demski, L. S. & Northcutt, R. G. In Great White Sharks: The Biology of Carcharodon carcharias (eds Klimley, A. P. & Ainley, D. G.) 121–130 (Academic Press, 1996).
Ito, H., Yoshimoto, M. & Somiya, H. External brain form and cranial nerves of the megamouth shark, Megachasma pelagios. Copeia 1999, 210–213 (1999).
Graham, R. T. et al. Satellite tracking of manta rays highlights challenges to their conservation. PLoS ONE 7, e36834 (2012).
Conrath, C. & Musick, J. In Biology of Sharks and Their Relatives, 2nd Ed (eds Carrier, J. C., Musick, J. A. & Heithaus, M. R.) 291–312 (CRC Press, 2012).
Ebert, D. A. & Compagno, L. J. V. & Natanson. Biological notes on the Pacific sleeper shark, Somniosus pacificus (Chondrichthyes: Squalidae). California Fish and Game 73, 117–123 (1987).
Sebens, K. P. The ecology of indeterminate growth in animals. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Systemat. 18, 371–407 (1987).
Gage, F. Neurogenesis in the adult brain. J. Neurosci. 22, 612–613 (2002).
Lisney, T. J., Bennett, M. B. & Collin, S. P. Volumetric analysis of sensory brain areas indicates ontogenetic shifts in the relative importance of sensory systems in elasmobranchs. Raffles B. Zool. 14, 7–15 (2007).
Lisney, T. J., Yopak, K. E., Camilieri-Asch, V. & Collin, S. P. Ontogenetic shifts in brain organization in the bluespotted stingray Neotrygon kuhlii (Chondrichthyes: Dasyatidae). Brain Behav. Evol. 89, 68–83 (2017).
Salas, C. et al. Allometric variation of sensory brain regions during the ontogeny of the southern hemisphere lamprey, Geotria australis. Proc. Aust. Neurosci (2013).
Iribarne, L. & Castelló, M. E. Postnatal brain development of the pulse type, weakly electric gymnotid fish Gymnotus omarorum. J. Physiol. Paris 108, 47–60 (2014).
Oikawa, A., Takemori, M. & Itazawa, Y. Relative growth of organs and parts of a marine teleost, the Progy, Pagrus major, with special reference to metabolism-size relationships. Jap. J. Ichthyol. 39, 243–249 (1992).
Ngwenya, A. et al. The continuously growing central nervous system of the Nile crocodile (Crocodylus niloticus). Anat. Rec. 296, 1489–1500 (2013).
Graeber, R. C. In Sensory Biology of Sharks, Skates, and Rays (eds Hodgson, E. S. & Mathewson, R. F.) 195–225 (U.S. Government Printing Office, 1978).
Graeber, R. C., Schroeder, D. M., Jane, J. A. & Ebbesson, S. O. E. Visual discrimination following partial telencephalic ablations in nurse sharks (Ginglymostoma cirratum). J. Comp. Neurol. 180, 325–344 (1978).
Fuss, T., Bleckmann, H. & Schluessel, V. The shark Chiloscyllium griseum can orient using turn responses before and after partial telencephalon ablation. J. Comp. Physiol. A 200, 19–35 (2014).
Fuss, T., Bleckmann, H. & Schluessel, V. Place learning prior to and after telencephalon ablation in bamboo and coral cat sharks (Chiloscyllium griseum and Atelomycterus marmoratus). J. Comp. Physiol. 200, 37–52 (2014).
Schwarze, S., Bleckmann, H. & Schluessel, V. Avoidance conditioning in bamboo sharks (Chiloscyllium griseum and C. punctatum): behavioral and neuroanatomical aspects. J. Comp. Physiol. A (2013).
Rodriguez, F. et al. In Fish cognition and behaviour (eds Brown C., Laland K, & Krause J.) 243–277 (Blackwell, 2006).
Collin, S. P., Kempster, R. & Yopak, K. E. In Fish Physiology: Physiology of Elasmobranch Fishes (eds Shadwick, R. E. Farrell, A. P. & Brauner, C. J.) 34A, 19–99 (Elsevier, 2015).
Dryer, L. & Graziadei, P. P. C. A pilot study on morphological compartmentalization and heterogeneity in the elasmobranch olfactory bulb. Anat. Embryol. (Berl) 190, 41–51 (1993).
Charvet, C. J., Darlington, R. B. & Finlay, B. L. Brain scaling across vertebrates: Conservation and variation. Brain Behav. Evol. 78, 186 (2011).
Gonzalez-Voyer, A., Winberg, S. & Kolm, N. Social fishes and single mothers: Brain evolution in African cichlids. Proc. R. Soc Lond. B 276, 161–167 (2008).
Reep, R. L., Finlay, B. L. & Darlington, R. B. The limbic system in mammalian brain evolution. Brain Behav. Evol. 70, 57–70 (2007).
Healy, S. & Guilford, T. Olfactory-bulb size and nocturnality in birds. Evolution 44, 339–346 (1990).
Northcutt, R. G. In Behavior and Neurology of lizards (eds Greenberg, N. & MacLean, P.) 11–64 (National Institutes of Mental Health, 1978).
Smith, T. D. & Bhatnagar, K. P. Microsmatic primates: Reconsidering how and when size matters. Anat. Rec. 279B, 24–31 (2004).
Finlay, B. L., Darlington, R. B. & Nicastro, N. Developmental structure in brain evolution. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24, 263–308 (2001).
Lisney, T. J. & Collin, S. P. Brain morphology in large pelagic fishes: A comparison between sharks and teleosts. J. Fish Biol. 68, 532–554 (2006).
Long, D. J. & Jones, R. E. In Great White Sharks: the Biology of Carcharodon carcharias (eds Klimley, A. P. & Ainley, D. G.) 293–307 (Academic Press, 1996).
Strong, W. R. J., Murphy, R. C., Bruce, B. D. & Nelson, D. R. Movements and associated observations of bait-attracted while sharks, Carcharodon carcharias: A preliminary report. Aus. J. Mar. Fresh. Res. 43, 13–20 (1992).
Jacobs, L. F. From chemotaxis to the cognitive map: The function of olfaction. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. 109(Suppl. 1), 10693–10700 (2012).
Nevitt, G., Losekoot, M. & Weimerskirch, H. Evidence for olfactory search in wandering albatross, Diomedea exulans. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. 105, 4576–4581 (2008).
Gittleman, J. L. Carnivore olfactory bulb size: Allometry, phylogeny, and ecology. J. Zool. 225, 253–272 (1991).
Hulbert, L. B., Sigler, M. F. & Lunsford, C. R. Depth and movement behaviour of the Pacific sleeper shark in the north–east Pacific Ocean. J. Fish. Biol. 69, 406–425 (2006).
Graeber, R. C. & Ebbesson, S. O. E. Visual discrimination learning in normal and tectal-ablated nurse sharks (Ginglymostoma cirratum). Comp. Biochem. Physiol. 42A, 131–139 (1972).
Northcutt, R. G. Visual pathways in elasmobranchs: Organization and phylogenetic implications. J. Exp. Zool. Suppl. 256, 97–107 (1991).
Reperant, K. et al. The anatomical organization of retinal projecitons in the shark Scyliorhinus canicula, with special reference to the evolution of the selachian primary visual system. Brain Res. Rev. 11, 227–248 (1986).
Bodznick, D. Elasmobranch vision: Multimodal integration in the brain. J. Exp. Zool. Suppl. 256, 108–116 (1991).
Huber, R. & Rylander, M. K. Brain morphology and turbitity preference in Notropis and related genera (Cyprinidae, Teleostei). Environ. Biol. Fish. 33, 153–165 (1992).
Collin, S. P., Lloyd, D. J. & Wagner, H. J. Foveate vision in deep-sea teleosts: a comparison of primary visual and olfactory inputs. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 355, 1315–1320 (2000).
Bigelow, H. B. & Schroeder, W. C. In Fishes of the western North Atlantic. Part 1. Lancelets, cyclostomes, sharks 59–546 (Yalwe University, Sears Foundation for Marine Research 1, 1948).
Yopak, K. E., Pakan, J. & Wylie, D. In Evolution of Nervous Systems, 2nd Edition (ed. Kaas, J. H.) 373–385 (Elsevier, 2017).
Montgomery, J. C., Bodznick, D. & Yopak, K. E. The cerebellum and cerebellar-like structures of cartilaginous fishes. Brain Behav. Evol. 80, 152–165 (2012).
Puzdrowski, R. L. & Gruber, S. Morphologic features of the cerebellum of the Atlantic stingray, and their possible evolutionary significance. Int. Zool. 4, 110–122 (2009).
Puzdrowski, R. L. & Leonard, R. B. Variations in cerebellar morphology of the Atlantic stingray, Dasyatis sabina. Neurosci. Lett. 135, 196–200 (1992).
Yopak, K., Galinsky, V., Berquist, R. & Frank, L. Quantitative classification of cerebellar foliation in cartilaginous fishes (Class: Chondrichthyes) using 3D shape analysis and its implications for evolutionary biology. Brain Behav. Evol. 87, 252–264 (2016).
New, J. G. Comparative neurobiology of the elasmobranch cerebellum: Theme and variations on a sensorimotor interface. Environ. Biol. Fish. 60, 93–108 (2001).
Carey, F. G. et al. Temperature and activities of a white shark, Carcharodon carcharias. Copeia 1982, 254–260 (1982).
Lowe, C. G., Wetherbee, B. M., Crow, G. L. & Tester, A. L. Ontogenetic dietary shifts and feeding behavior of the tiger shark, Galeocerdo cuvier, in Hawaiian waters. Environ. Biol. Fish. 47, 203–211 (1996).
Nakamura, I. et al. Yo-yo vertical movements suggest a foraging strategy for tiger sharks Galeocerdo cuvier. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 424, 237–246 (2011).
Lyamin, O. I. Sleep in the harp seal (Pagophilus groenlandicus). Comparison of sleep on land and in water. J. Sleep Res. 2, 170–174 (1993).
Ridgway, S. H., Harrison, R. J. & Joyce, P. L. Sleep and cardiac rhythm in the gray seal. Science 187, 553–555 (1975).
Lyamin, O. I. et al. Electroencephalogram asymmetry and spectral power during sleep in the northern fur seal. J. Sleep Res. 17, 154–165 (2008a).
Lyamin, O. I. et al. Cetacean sleep: an unusual form of mammalian sleep. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32, 1451–1484 (2008b).
Templeman, W. Distribution of sharks in the Canadian Atlantic (with special reference to Newfoundland waters). Bull. Fish. Res. Board of Canada 140, 1–77 (1963).
Yushkevich, P. A. et al. User-guided 3D active contour segmentation of anatomical structures: Significantly improved efficiency and reliability. Neuroimage 31, 1116–11128 (2006).
Harvey, P. H. & Pagel, M. D. The Comparative Method in Evolutionary Biology. (Oxford University Press, 1991).
Freckleton, R. P. The seven deadly sins of comparative analysis. J Evol. Biol. 22, 1367–1375 (2009).
Freckleton, R. P. On the misuse of residuals in ecology: Regression of residuals vs. multiple regression. J. Anim. Ecol. 71, 542–545 (2002).
Pagel, M. Inferring the historical patterns of biological evolution. Nature 401, 877–884 (1999).
Revell, L. Phylogenetic signal and linear regression on species data. Methods in Ecol. Evol. 1, 319–329 (2010).
Housworth, E., Martins, E. & Lynch, M. The phylogenetic mixed model. Am. Nat. 163, 84–96 (2004).
Stein, R. et al. Global priorities for conserving the evolutionary history of sharks, rays, and chimaeras. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 2, 288–298 (2018).
Orme, D. et al. Comparative Analyses of Phylogenetics and Evolution in R, version 0.5. Comprehensive R Archive Network. Available, http://cran.r-project.org/package=caper (2012).
Pinheiro, J. et al. Nlme: linear and nonlinear mixed effects models. V. 3.1-131, https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlme (2017).
R Core Team R. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, U. h. w. R.-p. o. A language and environment for statistical computing. (Vienna, Austria, 2014) (2014).
Revell, L. Phytools: an R package for phylogenetic comparative biology (and other things). Methods Ecol. Evol. 3, 217–223 (2012).
Purvis, A. & Rambaut, A. Comparative Analysis by Independent Contrasts (CAIC): A statistical package for the Apple Macintosh. (User’s Guide. University of Oxford, Oxford, 1995).
Purvis, A. & Rambaut, A. Comparative analysis by independent contrasts (CAIC): An Apple Macintosh application for analyzing comparative data. Comp. Appl. Biosci. 11, 247–251 (1995).
Hilborn, R. & Mange, M. The ecological detective: Confronting models with data. (Princeton University Press, 1997).
Burnham, K. P. & Anderson, D. Model Selection and Multimodel Inference: A Practical Information-Theoretic Approach. 2nd ed., (Springer, 2002).
Acknowledgements
The authors thank all those who helped with the collection and provision of specimens and/or manuscript advice, particularly K. Goldman for donation of S. pacificus and valuable insights into early analysis. The authors acknowledge the facilities and scientific and technical assistance of the National Imaging Facility, a National Collaborative Research Infrastructure Strategy (NCRIS) capability, at the Centre for Microscopy, Characterisation and Analysis, The University of Western Australia. KEY acknowledges startup funds from the University of North Carolina Wilmington for funding during the writing of this manuscript. KEY and SPC also acknowledge the Australian Research Council (Discovery Grant DP120102327) for support. The Norwegian Research Council (grant number 184644/S40) funded work done on S. microcephalus in European waters to KMK and CL. BM and ACF were funded from the Government of Canada Program for the International Polar Year 2007/2008 (to ATF). CGM acknowledges the support of N. Dulvy and the Shark Conservation Fund for funding during the writing of this manuscript.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
Conceptualization, K.E.Y. and S.P.C.; Methodology, K.E.Y., B.C.M., K.W.F., A.F.; Formal Analysis, C.G.M. and K.E.Y.; Investigation: K.E.Y., B.C.M., K.W.F., A.F; Resources, K.E.Y, S.P.C., K.W.F., K.M.K., C.L., A.F.; Data Curation, K.E.Y.; Writing – Original Draft, K.E.Y.; Writing – Review & Editing, K.E.Y., S.P.C., B.M, C.G.M., K.W.F., K.M.K., C.L., A.F.; Visualization, C.G.M. and K.E.Y.; Supervision, S.P.C. and A.F.; Project Administration: K.E.Y., S.P.C., A.F., K.M.K., C.L.; Funding Acquisition: K.E.Y., S.P.C., K.M.K., C.L. and A.F.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing Interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Yopak, K.E., McMeans, B.C., Mull, C.G. et al. Comparative Brain Morphology of the Greenland and Pacific Sleeper Sharks and its Functional Implications. Sci Rep 9, 10022 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-46225-5
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-46225-5
This article is cited by
-
Volumetric analysis and morphological assessment of the ascending olfactory pathway in an elasmobranch and a teleost using diceCT
Brain Structure and Function (2020)
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.