Introduction

Math anxiety (MA) is characterised by negative emotional response such as fear and tension when facing math-related situations, which cannot be reduced to either general anxiety or test anxiety1. It disrupts mathematical performance irrespective of gender2, and can emerge in the primary school years3,4. Depending on the extent of MA, the negative impact of MA could have far-reaching consequences beyond academic achievements5. Given that a significant variation in the level of MA is contributed by genetic factors6, understanding the neurocognitive basis of individual differences in MA may shed light on its causal pathway.

To date, there are a limited number of neuroimaging studies, which are mainly based on adults and comparisons of neural response between groups of high and low levels of MA. In adults, high- compared with low-level MA has been shown to be associated with increased activity in bilateral posterior insula, brain areas linked with threat and pain processing, when anticipating mathematical tasks7. High- compared to low-level MA has also been associated with stronger deactivation within the default mode network during tasks that require additional inhibitory functions, possibly reflecting depletion of working memory resources4. Further, increased activity in frontoparietal regions in high-level MA adults when anticipating mathematical tasks has been shown to correspond with reduced performance deficits, suggesting the role of cognitive control in MA8. In children9, high-level MA has been associated with hyperactivity in the right amygdala when solving mathematical problems, and increased connectivity between this and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, areas implicated in the processing and regulation of negative emotions. Compared with low-level MA, high-level MA has been linked with decreased activity in brain areas involved in working memory and attention, including the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and reduced activity in posterior parietal areas, critical to numerical processing. Furthermore, brain stimulation over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex has shown the ability to improve arithmetic performance and reduced cortisol level in those with high-level MA10. To our knowledge, there are no prior published studies assessing the link between brain structure and MA in children or adults.

A recent review11 highlighted that there are specific aspects of numerical and executive function that might explain varying degrees of MA: lower working memory capacity, reduced attentional control, lower inhibitory control and a deficit in low-level numerical representations. Furthermore, increasing levels of MA may negatively affect mathematical achievement, via disruption of core executive functions and/or deficit in low-level numerical representations. Using data collected from a larger study, we sought to test the strength of association made by those predictions in that review, to better understand the neural and cognitive factors that are associated with the degree of anxiety towards mathematics. We aimed to identify the structural brain correlates of the level of MA in a typical school population, and to determine how brain structure mediates the relationship between those cognitive functions that are most strongly predictive of the level of MA. Much of the prior literature has consisted of between-group comparisons and, whilst these have provided important insights on the plausible neural mechanisms of MA, the neural and cognitive architecture that contributes to individual differences in MA, and its association with mathematical achievement, remains unclear.

The present study comprises brain structure and MA data from 79 healthy, Spanish children. The Behavioural Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF)12,13 was used to profile each child’s behavior in specific domains of executive function. The BRIEF indices that were of primary interest for the present study were: inhibitory control and impulsivity (INHBIT), ability to switch and alternate attention (SHIFT) and on-line, representational memory (WORKING MEMORY). The BRIEF is regularly used in clinical and education settings, where higher scores indicate greater difficulty, and therefore lower capacity, in each domain. Numerical representation skills were assessed using a number line task, consisting of positioning numbers on an analogue scale (PN)14, mathematical achievement was determined using the Woodcock Johnson III Achievement (WJ)15 and the level of MA was established via the Math Anxiety Scale (Math-AS)16. Voxel-based morphometry (VBM) was used to identify brain-structure correlates of MA.

Method

Participants

Participants were recruited through two state primary schools in Murcia, Spain, as part of a wider study17,18,19. The primary sample comprised 137 Spanish children, aged 7–12 years (2nd – 6th grade). Written, informed consent was obtained from parents prior to acquiring any data, and verbal consent reobtained immediately prior to data acquisition. Parents were advised that they would be informed by the hospital Radiologist if any clinically relevant abnormalities were detected. T1-weighted structural MRI data were acquired from an initial sample comprising 110 children whose parents gave previous informed consent. Two were not included in the current study as they were reported to be bilingual, which may affect the measurement of math anxiety and numerical achievement. A further 7 were excluded due to learning disabilities. Note that children with learning disabilities were diagnosed before our study and were receiving special education from their schools. We administered tests on all children to ensure that no one felt excluded. We only analysed data of children without learning disabilities. Following exclusion due to unsatisfactory image quality resulting from movement- or other imaging artefacts (n = 21) or neuro-incidental findings (n = 1), the final sample comprised 79 children (age M = 115.20, SD = 14.13; males = 50.6%; right-handed = 88.6%). The study was approved by the University of Murcia Ethics Committee, and it was conducted in accordance with the approved guidelines and the Declaration of Helsinki.

Materials

Measures that were modeled in the current study include math anxiety, mathematical abilities and working memory.

Math Anxiety

MA was assessed using the Math-AS (known also as the EAM, Escala de Ansiedade a Matemática16). It consists of 25 items that describe situations that are commonly experienced by elementary and high school students during their math lessons. This scale measures the variations in the degrees of math anxiety, from absence to extreme math anxiety. This task was translated from Portuguese into European Spanish by a speaker fluent in both languages (FHS) to enable cross-cultural adaptation. Children indicated the intensity of their response to each item on a five-point Likert scale by crossing out one of the following: (1) None (2) Low (3) Moderate (4) High and (5) Very High. The score was the sum of all points from the 25 questions. This measure has been shown to have accurate validity and reliability when used with children20. Note that we could not control for general anxiety in our analyses however, as we did not have normative data for children below 9 years old using the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Children (STAI-CH)21. To be confident that we measured MA and not general anxiety trait, we ran a correlation analysis on the standardised scores of the 50 out of 79 children. We found a lack of correlation between general anxiety (STAI-CH) and MA, rp = 0.07, p = 0.63.

Numerical Cognition

Math Achievement

Children’s math abilities were assessed using the Spanish version of the Woodcock-Johnson III (WJ-III) Achievement (ACH) battery15, which has been validated for the use of children aged 6–13 years in Spain22. It comprises 4 subtests: calculation, math fluency, quantitative concepts and applied problems (see Supplementary Information: Method). The raw scores of each subtest were transformed into W scores23 following the Rasch’s measurement model24,25. We used the composite score of all 4 subtests in our analyses.

Numerical Representation

Numerical representation was assessed using a number line task, consisting of positioning numbers on an analogue scale (PN)14. The participants were required to map numbers on a vertical line that was marked with “0” at the bottom and “100” at the top. In half of the trials, the line was further marked with 4 horizontal lines at different locations to assist children with number mapping. Children were required to indicate the position of an Arabic numeral, orally or visually presented by the experimenter, by pointing to a specific location on the line. There were 12 trials in this task.

Executive Function

We used parents’ rating of children’s online memory (WORKING MEMORY), attention (SHIFT) and inhibitory control (INHIBIT) using the Spanish version of the Behavioural Rating Inventory of Executive Function, BRIEFTM12,13, which assesses the executive functioning of children between 5–18 years old. It contains 86 items in eight non-overlapping clinical scales and two validity scales.

Procedure

Cognitive and Achievement Testing

Children’s performance on a range of behavioural, cognitive and achievement tasks was assessed prior to collecting the structural MRI scans. Testing was conducted during the Autumn term by six trained assistants with children in groups of two.

Analysis of Demographic, Cognitive and Achievement Data

Several of the measures resulted in positively skewed data. Parametric statistics were combined with permutation testing as this approach, in contrast with non-parametric analyses, has been shown to maximally reduce type I and type II errors26. Confidence intervals (CIs) were estimated using the bias-corrected and accelerated (BCa) percentile bootstrap method (10,000 samples).

The cognitive and achievement data were analysed using SPSS, version 22. The mediation analysis was conducted using the Process macro for SPSS (v2.16.3), available from http://www.processmacro.org/index.html following a published analysis pipeline27. Ten-thousand bootstrap resamples were used to generate bias-corrected, 95% confidence intervals.

Neuroimaging Acquisition

The participants were fitted with ear plugs and soft foam padding used to minimize head movement during the scan. Participants were asked to remain as still as possible for the duration of the scan, and a parent sat beside their child throughout. A T1-weighted image was acquired for each participant using a 1.5 T GE HDX scanner with an 8-channel, phased array, transmit-receive head coil. A 3D FSPGR BRAVO sequence was used to achieve whole brain coverage, composed of 142 axially oriented slices with a reconstructed voxel size of 1 × 1 × 1 mm3, where TR = 12.4 ms, TE = 5.2 ms, flip angle = 12°.

Neuroimaging Analysis using Voxel-Based Morphometry

The MRI data were analyzed using the FMRIB Software Library (FSL, version 6.0.0; http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/). Non-brain tissue was removed from the structural images and an initial weak bias field correction applied using FSL’s anatomy pipeline (http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/fsl_anat). These brain extracted, bias corrected images were then fed into the second and subsequent stages of FSL-VBM28 (http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/FSLVBM), an optimised VBM protocol29. The images were grey matter-segmented and registered to the MNI-152 standard space using non-linear registration30. The resulting images were averaged and flipped along the x-axis to create a left-right symmetric, study-specific grey matter template. All native grey matter images were then non-linearly registered to this study-specific template and “modulated” to correct for local expansion (or contraction) due to the non-linear component of the spatial transformation. The modulated grey matter images were then smoothed with an isotropic Gaussian kernel with a sigma of 3 mm, Full-Width-Half-Maximum (FWHM) ~7 mm. Finally, voxelwise general linear modelling was applied using Randomise31 (http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/Randomise), which permits permutation-based non-parametric testing, correcting for multiple comparisons across space. Here, 10,000 permutations of the data were generated to test against the null. Threshold-free cluster enhancement (TFCE)32 was used to identify cluster-like structures, taking family-wise error rate (FWE) corrected p-values < 0.05. To avoid any labelling bias, probabilistic anatomical descriptors were determined using FSL Atlas Query (http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/Atlasquery). Anatomical labels were output for voxels that had survived multiple-comparison correction. Cluster peak information was extracted using FSL’s Cluster tool. See Supplementary Information: Method for a detailed description of the GLM analyses.

Data availability

The research meta-data supporting this publication are available on the Open Science Framework repository, see DOI 10.17605/OSF.IO/PDFJE. The senior author, LJF, may be contacted regarding the wider dataset.

Results

Math Anxiety and its Association with Demographic, Cognitive and Numerical Factors

Table 1 outlines the sample’s demographic, numerical and cognitive characteristics (see also Supplementary Information: Table S1 for a detailed breakdown by grade). Typically, the level of MA within the sample was low. An independent-samples t-test determined that there was no difference in the mean level of MA between the sexes (t(65.569) = −1.063, p = 0.292, 95% CI −13.21, 3.569). Age was positively associated with the level of MA (r(79) = 0.237, p = 0.035, CI 0.031, 0.433), consistent with previous studies33 (see Supplementary Information: Fig. S1).

Table 1 Sample Characteristics.

To assess the relationship between MA, numerical- and cognitive-function we conducted a series of partial correlations. Each of the three BRIEF indices of interest (INHIBIT, SHIFT, WORKING MEMORY), plus the PN and WJ were correlated with the Math-AS score (controlling for age and biological sex). SHIFT, WORKING MEMORY and WJ were statistically significant after applying a Bonferroni correction (Table 2).

Table 2 Association between Math Anxiety, Numerical and Cognitive Indices.

Math Anxiety and its Association with the Brain

VBM was used to identify the structural correlates of MA. A general linear model (GLM) comprising Math-AS score and the nuisance variables, age, biological sex, recruitment source and handedness was created. Contrasts for positive and negative associations between grey matter volume (GMV) and MA were computed, where corrections were applied for multiple comparisons across the brain, and adjustment to correct for running two contrasts using a Bonferroni correction. A whole brain analysis did not identify any regions that were positively associated with MA. Four clusters demonstrated a negative association between GMV and MA, which encompassed only cortical grey matter (Table 3 and Fig. 1). The largest cluster spanned both hemispheres across occipital and parietal cortices, including a section running anterior-posterior along the left anterior intraparietal sulcus (IPS), in areas hIP1 and hIP3, as defined by the Juelich Histological Atlas. A second, smaller cluster was identified in the right visual cortices, encompassing extrastriate areas and additionally encompassing the left anterior IPS. Lastly, a small, left lateralized cluster was identified in the inferior parietal lobule. The Bonferroni correction had split this latter cluster, resulting in a further cluster in the lateral occipital cortex. As this comprised a single voxel (MNI: −18, −88, 42), this voxel was excluded from subsequent analyses.

Table 3 Probabilistic Labels for Brain Regions where Grey Matter Volume is Negatively Associated with Math Anxiety.
Figure 1
figure 1

Grey matter correlates of Math Anxiety. Surface rendered image reflects a t-statistic cluster map rendered onto a template brain. All coloured areas reflect those grey matter voxels that were significantly negatively associated with MA after multiple comparison corrections as outlined in the method. The top right panel illustrates the results transformed and rendered onto a single participant’s annoymised T1-structural image. All images are presented in neurological convention, where the left of the image reflects the left of the brain. Surface rendering created using Surf Ice42; Individual subject rendering created using Mango43.

To understand the function of these structural correlates of MA, a further GLM was constructed comprising the Math-AS scores and nuisance variables as outlined previously, plus those variables that had earlier shown the most robust associations with the level of MA: attentional control (SHIFT), online memory (WORKING MEMORY) and mathematical achievement (WJ). This GLM was applied to those voxels that were previously identified as being negatively associated with MA.

When controlling for attention, working memory and mathematical achievement, a large number of those voxels initially found to be negatively associated with MA were no longer statistically significant, particularly within the IPS. Although this result alone is insufficient to determine a difference, the data suggest that the association between MA and GMV in these voxels may be mediated by one or more of the newly modelled variables. To assess this possibility, the linear directional contrasts for each newly modelled variable were computed. These data suggested that, of the additional variables added, attentional control was negatively associated with a large proportion of those voxels that were no longer associated with MA (see Supplementary Information: Fig. S2). However, this result did not survive correction for multiple-comparisons. Working memory and math achievement did not explain the association with MA (see Supplementary Information for detailed modelling procedure).

To further assess this, the mean GMV was extracted for each of the 3 clusters identified in the earlier VBM analysis. Though large, the spatial arrangement of these clusters may demarcate differentiation of function, as each comprised relatively distinct anatomical regions: lingual gyrus, cuneal cortex and the intraparietal sulcus. SHIFT, WORKING MEMORY and WJ were entered into a series of partial correlations to examine their relationship with GMV within each of the 3 clusters. When controlling for age, biological sex and Math-AS score, attentional control (SHIFT) was shown to be negatively associated with GMV across the IPS, although this result did not survive a Bonferroni correction for 9 tests. All other tests were non-significant at the uncorrected level (see Table 4).

Table 4 Partial Correlation Results for Regional Grey Matter Volume, Numerical and Cognitive Indices.

The Cognitive Architecture of MA and Resultant Outcomes in Numerical Achievement

Suárez-Pellicioni and colleagues11 outline a model, the processing efficiency theory34 in which intrusive thoughts resultant from MA consume working memory. This, in turn, is argued to expend already limited attentional resources in high MA individuals, leading to diminished performance when complex mathematical operations are performed. Such a relationship provides a causal explanation for the association between MA (Math-AS) and mathematical achievement (WJ). We tested this theoretical model using mediation analysis. When controlling for working memory and the nuisance variables age and biological sex, the relationship between MA and math achievement was no longer significant (c′) (see Table 5 and Fig. 2). The overall mediation model found that MA, working memory, age and biological sex explained approximately 55% of the variance in math achievement, (R2 = 0.5527, F(4, 66) = 20.39, p < 0.0001). Consistent with this model, the mediation analysis suggested that higher levels of MA (Math-AS) were associated with slightly elevated difficulty with holding appropriate information in mind (WORKING MEMORY), which in turn resulted in reduced math achievement (WJ).

Table 5 Mediation Path Coefficients and Confidence Intervals for Math Anxiety Predicting Math Achievement.
Figure 2
figure 2

Summary coefficients for mediation model. Note. Path a = unstandardised IV to mediator; path b = unstandardised mediator to DV; path c = unstandardised total effect (IV to DV); path c′ = unstandardised direct effect. Coefficient values rounded to 2 decimal places; full values reported in accompanying table (3). ns = non-significant *p < 0.01; **p < 0.001. Note. Higher working memory values indicate lower working memory.

Discussion

The current study sought to examine the neurocognitive bases of MA in typically developing children. Unlike most prior work, however, the analyses focused on the level of MA, which is a more rigorous approach than making comparisons based on the presence or absence or of it35. This work therefore provides both a description of how MA might manifest itself across a cohort of typical children, as well as a detailed evaluation of theoretically-driven factors that might influence the degree of MA.

Most of the children in the sample demonstrated low-levels of MA. Around 10% of the entire sample reported moderate to high-levels of MA, and prevalence increased linearly with age. Aside from numerical representations, all other cognitive variables that were of theoretical interest were associated with the degree of MA: the more math anxious children demonstrated reduced inhibitory and attentional control, as well as lower working memory capacity and math achievement. Though MA appears to be associated with differences in baseline executive functions in children with MA, working memory was also shown to mediate the relationship between MA and math achievement suggesting that, possibly in addition to baseline differences, capacity issues may be exacerbated ‘online’ when working with mathematics by the physiological response to anxiety, which in turn leads to poorer learning and performance in mathematics.

Contrary to previous studies36,37, we found no association between MA and low-level numerical representations. There has been considerable debate regarding the presence of a low-level deficit in math ability in MA11. It may be that the task used here was not fine-grained enough to highlight any association, although note that the data from the task used did correlate with the measure of math achievement, suggesting that the task was tapping into numerical cognition to some degree. The current pattern of results is also indicative of experiential differences between high and low math anxious individuals, as MA may result in avoidance of exposure to math problems38. Moreover, an experientially-driven reduction in math achievement does not discount a primary, causal deficit in numerical representation; it may be that more extreme levels of MA reflect both causal (representational) and affectual (experiential) mechanisms. Indeed, cross-sectional work strongly suggests the presence of a less precise representation of numerical representation in adults with high MA37.

The present study also identified that reduced grey matter volume in the IPS, lingual gyrus and cuneal cortex was associated with increased MA. The identification of a structural correlate in relatively young children suggests that there may be differences in early brain structure that underpin the development of MA. Thus, whilst prior functional imaging studies demonstrate how network functionality can explain differences in mathematical performance in people with math anxiety8,9, these data provide the first evidence of a possible underlying structural basis. Genetic modelling suggests that around 40% of variance in MA6 can be explained by heredity, thus, a neurodevelopmental precursor is not implausible. Prior work has implicated the IPS in a network of regions showing aberrant activity in young, math anxious children, where these patterns of activation were unrelated to intelligence, general anxiety, reading ability or working memory9. Our data suggest that the IPS region identified in the current study might serve an attentional function, where children with reduced grey matter in this area had lower reported attentional resources, and this reduced attentional capacity was associated with increased MA. This is consistent with the assumption of the processing efficiency theory34 that high MA individuals may already have a limited attentional resource, and MA would further consume working memory, contributing to lower performance on complex mathematical operations. Future studies could investigate whether this reduced grey matter in the IPS is a structural adaptation due to reduced attentional capacity or compensatory strategies associated with MA39, or a potential biomarker on the causal pathway of the development of MA. One model that should be examined in future research is that children with MA, or who go on to develop MA, start off with differences in IPS structure, which translate into a deficit in baseline attentional capacity. According to this view, a limited ability to attend to stimuli, particularly mathematical stimuli – where demands on attentional resources are often high due to the nature of arithmetic problems – could result in general feelings of anxiety, which later become habitually associated with doing math, causing the development of MA.

In addition to providing a rich description of the neurocognitive bases of the level of math anxiety, the current study provides testable hypotheses regarding the emergence and maintenance of MA. Having a more nuanced understanding of the neurocognitive profile of MA, including any impairments that may cause, contribute to, or result from MA, has important implications for the development of targeted, individualized intervention. Longitudinal and cross-sectional work to profile MA against developmental dyscalculia, which may appear qualitatively similar40, is required, however, to assess the validity of these assertions.

Limitations and Suggested Future Directions

The review by Suárez-Pellicioni and colleagues11 outlines three core explanations of MA: (1) Task-related competition for working memory resources (2) A deficit in low-level numerical representation (3) Math anxiety as an inhibition/attentional-control deficit. Using data collected from a wider study, we evaluated each of these, combining paediatric MRI data from typically developing children to advance a neurocognitive model of the level of MA. Whilst our results address some of the core areas of interest in the cognitive literature, the present study does not, however, provide an assessment across all cognitive domains, so cannot be considered an exhaustive evaluation of the neurocognitive architecture of MA. Moreover, our executive function measures were derived from parental report. Although the measure of executive function administered is used extensively in education and clinical settings, a standardized, automated neurocognitive test battery may provide more valid data regarding performance and capacity.

Future work should include measures sampled ‘online’ whilst performing numerical tasks, based on event-related potentials, functional magnetic resonance imaging or autonomic measurement, for example. Whilst such work has been conducted with adults, there has been little progress towards developing a dynamic view of how individual differences in children’s physiological responses to numerical stimuli vary as a function of MA. Importantly, such an approach could identify further potential antecedents of MA; thus, permitting directed early intervention ideally to prevent, or at least reduce, the negative consequences of MA. Similarly, multimodal approaches could achieve deeper evidence regarding the functional properties of MA and, if approached longitudinally, its emergence could be evaluated. Still, though longitudinal approaches permit causal inference, they attract significant technical and practical challenges41.