A trade-off between cognitive and physical performance, with relative preservation of brain function

Abstract

Debate surrounds the issue of how the large, metabolically expensive brains of Homo sapiens can be energetically afforded. At the evolutionary level, decreased investment in muscularity, adiposity and the digestive tract allow for a larger brain. Developmentally, high neo-natal adiposity and preferential distribution of resources to the brain provide an energetic buffer during times of environmental stress. Through an experimental design, we investigated the hypothesis of a trade-off involving brain and muscle at the acute level in humans. Mental performance was measured by a free-recall test, and physical performance by power output on an indoor rowing ergometer. Sixty-two male student rowers performed the two tests in isolation, and then again simultaneously. Paired samples t-tests revealed that both power output and mental performance reduced when tested together compared to in isolation (t(61) = 9.699, p < 0.001 and t(61) = 8.975, p < 0.001). Furthermore, the decrease in physical performance was greater than the decrease in mental performance (t(61) = −2.069, p = 0.043). This is the first investigation to demonstrate an acute level trade-off between these two functions, and provides support for the selfish brain hypothesis due to the relative preservation of cognitive function over physical power output. The underlying mechanism is unclear, and requires further work.

Introduction

Evolutionary and developmental implications of enhanced encephalization

The development of an enlarged and elaborated brain is considered a defining characteristic of human evolution1. The evolution of the Homo clade has been accompanied by significant encephalization2,3. This facilitated the development of more complex social strategies4,5, more effective food acquisition6 and the ability to solve ecological problems through innovative means7. Each of these characteristics may have increased survival and reproductive success, giving a greater life expectancy at the age of first reproduction8.

While the benefits of encephalization are numerous, the brain imposes significant metabolic costs on both the individual9,10,11. High levels of energetic expenditure are necessitated by the brain’s responsibility for regulating the body’s energy supply and controlling the function of many peripheral organs12. These functions require intense neuronal activity, giving the brain the highest metabolic demand relative to size of all organs13.

The question of how larger brains can be metabolically afforded has remained a prominent problem in human evolution11,14,15,16,17. Life history theory states that as energy availability is finite, an organism has a limited energy budget. Energy allocated to one function cannot be used for another. Energy savings in other organs or tissues could allow for energetic diversion to the brain, without the need to increase overall metabolic expenditure11,18. Such a trade-off has been proposed with both digestive tract development17 and adiposity19.

Meeting the brain’s metabolic requirements

The immediate metabolic costs of the brain depend on its activation state. While the metabolic rate is low during sleep20 increased energy consumption has been observed in response to a mental task21, and following somatosensory, olfactory, visual and auditory stimulation22,23,24,25,26,27. The adult brain almost exclusively derives its energy from the metabolism of glucose28. This, coupled with its high energetic demand, ensure that the brain metabolises the most glucose of any organ29,30. The brain, however, is unable to store significant amounts of energy and hence buffer its high yet variable metabolic demand31. As such, the body is required to supply glucose to the brain quickly and effectively. The ‘Selfish Brain Hypothesis’12 posits that the brain prioritises its own glucose needs over those of the peripheral organs, such as skeletal muscle.

Skeletal muscle and encephalization

Skeletal muscle mass is an expensive tissue to maintain, accounting for approximately 20% of human male BMR32,33, and may be compromised to partially offset the brain’s high energy costs11,34. An adaptation to reduce muscle mass would thereby reduce metabolic demand, allowing for a reallocation of energy towards the central nervous system35. The glucose demands of skeletal muscle also increase significantly with activation36,37,38,39,40. In such circumstances, skeletal muscle thereby becomes a powerful competitor of the brain for glucose and oxygen41.

High intensity exercise increases the metabolic demand of skeletal muscles and the brain39,40,42,43,44,45, in proportion to degree of activation. At high levels of activation both are reliant upon glucose metabolism, and require a high rate of oxygen and glucose supply. Should both be challenged simultaneously, competition for these valuable yet limited resources may therefore develop, with one or both organs receiving an insufficient supply for optimal performance.

The concept of an antagonistic relationship between capacity to perform mental and physical work is not a new one46. As described by the idea of central fatigue, prior mental exertion may impair subsequent physical performance47.

Despite the intuitive appeal of a trade-off between two competing functions, negative covariance in such traits are not frequently observed when phenotypic comparisons are made between individuals within a population48,49. This study seeks to experimentally investigate the possibility of a trade-off involving the brain at the acute, rather than at the evolutionary or developmental, level. It is hypothesised that, when both systems are challenged simultaneously, performance will be inferior to performance when each are challenged in isolation. It is further hypothesised that the relative decrease in muscle power output will exceed the relative decrease in cognitive function.

Results

A description of the samples is given in Table 1. Significant positive correlations were observed between rowing power output in both Protocols A and C (r = 0.484, p < 0.001), as well as recall performance in both Protocols B and C (r = 0.758, p < 0.001). This suggests that participants who row fast or recall many words do so irrespective of condition. Table 2 reports the correlation matrix (Pearsons product-moment correlation coefficients), with a significant negative correlation was observed between rowing power output in Protocol A and Δpower from Protocol A to C (r = −0.343, p = 0.006). No correlation was also observed between recall performance in Protocol B and Δrecall from Protocol B to C (r = −0.203, p = 0.113). This suggests that highly performing participants in Protocols A and B exhibit the greatest decline in performance when both tasks are performed together in Protocol C.

Table 1 Descriptive statistics for power output and free recall.
Table 2 Correlation matrix.

Paired samples t-tests revealed that power output (W) was significantly lower in Protocol C than in Protocol A (Protocol A M = 389.93, SD = 34.819; Protocol C M = 340.20, SD = 43.321; t(61) = 9.699, p < 0.001). Similarly, recall (correct words) was significantly lower in Protocol C than in Protocol B (Protocol B M = 29.11, SD = 3.339; Protocol C M = 26.27, SD = 3.738; t(61) = 8.975, p < 0.001). The percentage change in recall between Protocols B and C was significantly less than the percentage change in power output between Protocol A and C (Δrecall M = −9.6740, SD = 8.62756; Δpower output M = −12.5535, SD = 9.81460, t(61) = −2.069, p = 0.043), see Fig. 1.

Figure 1
figure1

Bar graph showing the relative decrease in cognitive decrease and power output. Power output decreased significantly more than cognitive function.

Discussion

We proposed that the simultaneous challenge of both cognitive and physical faculties would result in impaired performance in each task, compared to performance achieved when in isolation. This hypothesis has been supported. The observation that both tasks cannot be performed optimally at the same time suggests that a trade-off between mental and physical function does indeed exist. The secondary hypothesis was supported as concurrent challenge differentially affected each task; the decrease in brain function was significantly less than that of power output.

Preferential allocation of glucose to the brain may be an evolved trait; a well-fuelled brain may offer better survival odds than well-fuelled muscles when facing an environmental challenge31. In such a situation, the body is able to buffer a muscle-fuel deficit by increasing supply of free fatty acids to fuel skeletal muscles50,51. High intensity physical activity considerably increases the metabolic needs of both the brain and skeletal muscle28,38,39,40,42,43,44,52. Competition for a limited supply of blood glucose and oxygen is a potential mechanism accounting for the fast-acting trade-off in brain and muscle function demonstrated here. The occurrence of glycogen supercompensation in the brain, as well as in skeletal muscles following exhaustive exercise, provides further support for this explanation53.

Although the brain is normally dependent upon glucose for energy, it may also utilise the lactate produced by skeletal muscles during exercise. The brain takes up lactate in proportion to its arterial concentration, which increases with exercise intensity. This increased lactate utilisation contributes to the meeting of high cerebral energy demands, which result from increased neuronal activity during high intensity exercise54. The preferential uptake of lactate as the predominant oxidative substrate of neurones55 has the effect of sparing glucose56. However, our results suggest that substrate competition between the brain and skeletal muscles is significant despite this.

Life history theory describes the competitive allocation of limited resources between physiological functions during development57,58,59,60. The optimal physiological distribution is determined by both the individual’s life stage61 and environment62,63, and is achieved through phenotypic plasticity64. Hales & Barker extended life history theory to consider trade-offs between organs and tissues, such as brain and muscle, by proposing that nutritional stress during early development leads to certain tissues being prioritised over others65. This tactic allows the organism to endure conditions of energy deficit, but with the cost of decreased adaptability to varying ecological conditions later in life due to decreased investment in the development in other organs.

During neonatal development, the brain is most vulnerable to irregularities in energy supply. The high adiposity of humans at birth in comparison to other mammalian species66 provides an energetic buffer67, preserving cerebral metabolism despite high early-life energetic requirements. Furthermore, preferential distribution of resources is evident in undernourished foetuses, in whom some organs grow normally while others are underdeveloped68. For humans, the brain’s development is spared69, perhaps at the expense of muscle70. Low birth weight, indicative of foetal undernourishment, is associated with a negative relationship between development of brain and muscles71. The selfish nature of the brain has also been observed in the unique preservation of brain mass in individuals suffering from long-term malnutrition or starvation51, in children born with intrauterine growth restriction72,73 and in glucose-challenging situations such as fasting or hypo/hyper-glycaemia74,75,76.

The evidence presented in this paper, which builds upon the existing body of research, indicates the possibility of an evolutionary trade-off between brain and muscle energetic demands.

Conclusion

This study has demonstrated an acute level trade-off between cognitive function and physical power output during simultaneous challenge. This supports the selfish brain hypothesis due to the relative preservation of cognitive function over physical power output. The underlying mechanism is unclear, and requires further investigation.

Methods

Sixty-two male rowers were recruited from the University of Cambridge, and testing was carried out in Cambridge, UK (mean age = 21.15 years, SD = 1.618 years). All participants were instructed in the risks and benefits of participating in the study and signed a written informed consent statement. The statement and the study was approved by the University of Cambridge Biology Ethics Committee (Application No: HBREC.2013.12), and the study was conducted in accordance with the approved methodology.

Participants completed three protocols (Table 3). Protocol A consisted of a maximum effort row for 3 minutes at free rate on a Concept 2 rowing ergometer (manufactured by Concept 2, Vermont, USA), and average power output (W) was recorded. Protocol B consisted of a mental free recall task, the number of words correctly recalled was recorded. Protocol C consisted of the same 3 minute row as A, but while simultaneously performing the mental task of Protocol B. Both average power output and number of words correctly recalled were recorded.

Table 3 Experimental protocols.

The rowing ergometer was used because it is an energetically demanding activity, and has been used in previous studies investigating extreme physical stress77,78. The mental task involved free recall. A large printed screen showing 75 words was clearly displayed in front of the participants’ chair (Protocol B), or in front of the rowing ergometer (Protocol C), for a duration of 3 minutes. The participants were required to recall and write as many words as possible in any order from memory within 5 minutes (5 minutes immediately following the row in Protocol C)79. The words were selected from the Toronto Noun Pool80. Two 75-word lists were randomly created from the 150 words used by Kahana & Howard81 and were counterbalanced across participants. Half of the participants were given List 1 for Protocol B and List 2 for Protocol C, with the other half being given List 2 for Protocol B and List 1 for Protocol C. This method ensured that each word was seen an equal number of times across participants, and each participant saw each word only once. Such counterbalancing ensured that any artefacts82 were controlled for to reduce the likelihood of such artefacts83.

The Protocols were completed at 1 week intervals. All participants refrained from extra exercise the day before, and the day of, each Protocol. The same machine was used for Protocols A and B, with the drag factor being consistent. The order in which the participants completed the three protocols was also counterbalanced, in order to control for any effects such as the development of memorising strategies.

References

  1. 1.

    Foley, R. & Lee, P. Ecology and energetics of encephalization in hominid evolution. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 334(1270), 223–32 (1991).

    ADS  CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. 2.

    Hawks, J., Hunley, K., Lee, S. H. & Wolpoff, M. Population bottlenecks and Pleistocene human evolution. Mol Biol Evol. 17(1), 2–22 (2000).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. 3.

    Lee, S. & Wolpoff, M. The pattern of evolution in Pleistocene human brain size. Paleobiology. 29(2), 186–96 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. 4.

    Byrne, R. & Corp, N. Neocortex size predicts deception rate in primates. Proc Biol Sci. 271(1549), 1693–9 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. 5.

    Parker, S. & McKinney, M. Origins of intelligence: the evolution of cognitive development in monkeys, apes, and humans. (Johns Hopkins University Press, 1999).

  6. 6.

    Gibson, K. Cognition, brain size and the extraction of embedded food resources. Primate Ontog Cogn Soc Behav. 3, 92–10 (1986).

    Google Scholar 

  7. 7.

    Reader, S. & Laland, K. Social intelligence, innovation, and enhanced brain size in primates. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 99(7), 4436–41 (2002).

    ADS  CAS  Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. 8.

    Barrickman, N., Bastian, M., Isler, K. & van Schaik, C. Life history costs and benefits of encephalization: a comparative test using data from long-term studies of primates in the wild. J Hum Evol. 54(5), 568–90 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. 9.

    Mink, J., Blumenschine, R. & Adams, D. Ratio of central nervous system to body metabolism in vertebrates: its constancy and functional basis. Am J Physiol Integr Comp Physiol. 241(3), R203–12 (1981).

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. 10.

    Bullmore, E. & Sporns, O. The economy of brain network organization. Nat Rev Neurosci. 13(5), 336–49 (2012).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. 11.

    Isler, K. & van Schaik, C. P. Metabolic costs of brain size evolution. Biol Lett. 2(4), 557–60 (2006).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. 12.

    Peters, A. et al. The selfish brain: competition for energy resources. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 28, 143–80 (2004).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. 13.

    Attwell, D. & Laughlin, S. An energy budget for signalling in the grey matter of the brain. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 21(10), 1133–45 (2001).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. 14.

    Aiello, L. & Dunbar, R. Neocortex size, group size, and the evolution of language. Curr Anthropol. 34(2), 183–93 (1993).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. 15.

    Byrne, R. The technical intelligence hypothesis: an additional evolutionary stimulus to intelligence? In Machiavellian intelligence II: extension and evaluations (eds Whiten, A., Byrne, R.) 289–311 (Cambridge University Press, 1997).

  16. 16.

    Isler, K. & van Schaik, C. Costs of encephalization: the energy trade-off hypothesis tested on birds. J Hum Evol. 51(3), 228–43 (2011).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. 17.

    Aiello., L. & Wheeler, P. The expensive-tissue hypothesis the brain and the digestive evolution. Curr Anthropol. 36(2), 199–221 (1995).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. 18.

    McNab, B. & Eisenberg, J. F. Brain size and its relation to the rate of metabolism in mammals. Am Nat. 133(2), 157–67 (1989).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. 19.

    Navarrete, A., van Schaik, C. P. & Isler, K. Energetics and the evolution of human brain size. Nature. 480(7375), 91–3 (2011).

    ADS  CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. 20.

    Madsen, P. & Vorstrup, S. Cerebral blood flow and metabolism during sleep. Cerebrovas brain met. 3(4), 281–96 (1990).

    Google Scholar 

  21. 21.

    Madsen, P. et al. Persisent resetting of the cerebral oxygen/glucose uptake ratio by brain activation: evidence obtained with the Key-Schmidt technique. J Cereb blood flow Metab. 15, 485–91 (1995).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. 22.

    Fox, P. & Raichle, M. Focal physiological uncoupling of cerebral blood flow and oxidative metabolism during somatosensory stimulation in human subjects. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 83, 1140–4 (1986).

    ADS  CAS  Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. 23.

    Fox, P., Raichle, M., Mintun, M. & Dence, C. Nonoxidative glucose consumption during focal physiological neural activity. Science. 241(4864), 462–4 (1988).

    ADS  CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. 24.

    Sharp, F., Kauer, J. & Shepherd, G. Local sites of activity-related glucose metabolism in rat olfactory bulb during olfactory stimulation. Brain Res. 98(3), 596–600 (1975).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. 25.

    Kennedy, C. et al. Metabolic mapping of the primary visual system of the monkey by means of the autoradiographic [14C]deoxyglucose technique. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 73(11), 4230–4 (1976).

    ADS  CAS  Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. 26.

    Ginsberg, M. D., Dietrich, W. D. & Busto, R. Coupled forebrain increases of local cerebral glucose utilization and blood flow during physiologic stimulation of a somatosensory pathway in the rat: demonstration by double-label autoradiography. Neurology. 37(1), 11–9 (1987).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. 27.

    Roland, P., Eriksson, L., Stone-Elander, S. & Widen, L. Does mental activity change the oxidative metabolism of the brain? J Neurosci. 7, 2373–89 (1987).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. 28.

    Bélanger, M., Allaman, I. & Magistretti, P. Brain energy metabolism: focus on astrocyte-neuron metabolic cooperation. Cell Metab. 14(6), 724–38 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. 29.

    Reinmuth, O., Scheinberg, P. & Bourne, B. Total cerebral blood flow and metabolism archives of neurology. Arch Neurol. 12(1), 49–66 (1985).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. 30.

    Peters, A. The selfish brain: Competition for energy resources. Am J Hum Biol. 23(1), 29–34 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. 31.

    Beedie, C. & Lane, A. The role of glucose in self-control: another look at the evidence and an alternative conceptualization. Personal Soc Psychol Rev. 16(2), 143–53 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. 32.

    Elias, M. Organ and tissue contribution to metabolic rate in Energy metabolism: tissue determinents and cellular corollaries (ed McKinney, J., Tucker, H.) 51–79 (Raven Press, 1992).

  33. 33.

    Snodgrass, J. J., Leonard, W. R. & Robertson, M. L. Interspecific variation in body composition and its influence on metabolic variation in primates and other mammals. Am J Phys Anthropol. 28, 255 (1999).

    Google Scholar 

  34. 34.

    Snodgrass, J. J., Leonard, W. R. & Robertson, M. L. The energetics of encephalization in early hominids in The evolution of hominin diets. 15–29 (Springer Netherlands, 2009).

  35. 35.

    Leonard, W. R., Robertson, M. L., Snodgrass, J. J. & Kuzawa, C. W. Metabolic correlates of hominid brain evolution. Comp Biochem Physiol Part A Mol Integr Physiol. 136(1), 5–15 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. 36.

    Brooks, G. & Mercier, J. Balance of carbohydrate and lipid utilization during exercise: the “crossover” concept. J Appl Physiol. 76(6), 2253–61 (1994).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. 37.

    Romijn, J. A., Gastaldelli, A., Horowitz, J. F., Endert, E. & Wolfe, R. R. Regulation in relation of endogenous fat and carbohydrate to exercise intensity and duration metabolism. Am J Physiol. 265, 380–91 (1993).

    Google Scholar 

  38. 38.

    McArdle, W., Katch, F. & Katch, V. Exercise physiology: energy, nutrition and human performance (Lippincott, Williams & Wilkins, 2001).

  39. 39.

    Wahren, J., Felig, P., Ahlborg, G. & Jorfeldt, L. Glucose metabolism during leg exercise in man. J Clin Invest. 50(12), 2715–25 (1971).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. 40.

    Ahlborg, G., Felig, P., Hagenfeldt, L. & Hendler, R. Substrate turnover during prolonged exercise in man; splanching and leg metabolism of glucose, free fatty acids, and amino acids. J Clin Invest. 53, 1080–90 (1974).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. 41.

    Fehm, H., Kern, W. & Peters, A. The selfish brain: competition for energy resources. Prog Brain Res. 153, 129–40 (2006).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. 42.

    Holloszny, J. & Kohrt, W. Regulation of carbohydrate and fat metabolism during and after exercise. Annu Rev Nutr. 16, 121–38 (1996).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. 43.

    Nybo, L. & Secher, N. H. Cerebral perturbations provoked by prolonged exercise. Prog Neurobiol. 72(4), 223–61 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. 44.

    Ide, K., Horn, A. & Secher, N. Cerebral metabolic response to submaximal exercise. J Appl Physiol. 87, 1604–8 (1999).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. 45.

    Dalsgaard, M., Ide, K., Cai, Y., Quistorff, B. & Secher, N. The intent to exercise influences the cerebral O2/carbohydrate uptake ratio in humans. J Physiol. 540(2), 681–9 (2002).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. 46.

    Mosso, A. Fatigue. (Drummond M. & Drummond W. B., 1904).

  47. 47.

    Marcora, S. M., Staiano, W. & Manning, V. Mental fatigue impairs physical performance in humans. J Appl Physiol. 106(3), 857–64 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. 48.

    Glazier, D. S. Trade-offs between reproductive and somatic (storage) investments in animals: a comparative test of the Van Noordwijk and De Jong model. Evol Ecol. 13(6), 539–55 (2000).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. 49.

    van Noordwijk, A. & de Jong, G. Acquisition and Allocation of Resources: Their Influence on Variation in Life History Tactics. Am Nat. 128(1), 137–42 (1986).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. 50.

    Houten, S. et al. Impaired amino acid metabolism contributes to fasting-induced hypoglycemia in fatty acid oxidation defects. Hum Mol Genet. 22(25), 5249–61 (2013).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. 51.

    Wang, P. & Mariman, E. Insulin resistance in an energy-centered perspective. Physiol Behav. 94(2), 198–205 (2008).

    ADS  CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. 52.

    Maher, F., Vannucci, S. J. & Simpson, I. A. Glucose transporter proteins in brain. FASEB J. 8(13), 1003–11 (1998).

    Google Scholar 

  53. 53.

    Matsui, T. et al. Brain glycogen supercompensation following exhaustive exercise. J Physiol. 590(Pt 3), 607–16 (2012).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. 54.

    Kemppainen, J. et al. High intensity exercise decreases global brain glucose uptake in humans. J Physiol. 568(Pt 1), 323–32 (2005).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. 55.

    Bouzier-Sore, A. K., Voisin, P., Canioni, P., Magistretti, P. J. & Pellerin, L. Lactate is a preferential oxidative energy substrate over glucose for neurons in culture. J Cereb Blood flow Metab. 23(11), 1298–306 (2003).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. 56.

    Quistorff, B., Secher, N. H. & Van Lieshout, J. J. Lactate fuels the human brain during exercise. FASEB J. 22(10), 3443–9 (2008).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. 57.

    Stearns, S. C. Trade-Offs in life-history evolution. Br Ecol Soc. 3, 259–68 (1989).

    Google Scholar 

  58. 58.

    Zera, A. J. & Harshman, L. G. The physiology of life history trade-offs in animals. Ecology. 32, 95–126 (2001).

    Google Scholar 

  59. 59.

    Leonard, W. R. Laboratory and field methods for measuring human energy expenditure. Am J Hum Biol. 24(3), 372–84 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. 60.

    Stearns, S. C. The Evolution of Life Histories. (Oxford UnIversity Press, 1992).

  61. 61.

    Holliday, M. Body composition and energy needs during growth In: Human growth: a comprehensive treatise (Falkner, F., Tanner, J., ed) 101–17 (New York, NY, 1986).

  62. 62.

    McDade, T. W., Reyes-García, V., Tanner, S., Huanca, T. & Leonard, W. R. Maintenance versus growth: investigating the costs of immune activation among children in lowland Bolivia. Am J Phys Anthropol. 136(4), 478–84 (2008).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. 63.

    Kramer, K. L., Greaves, R. D. & Ellison, P. T. Early reproductive maturity among Pumé foragers: Implications of a pooled energy model to fast life histories. Am J Hum Biol. 21(4), 430–7 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. 64.

    Hill, K. & Hurtado, A. M. Ache life history. (Aldine de Gruyter, 1996).

  65. 65.

    Hales, C. & Barker, D. Type 2 (non-insulin-dependent) diabetes mellitus: the thrifty phenotype hypothesis. Diabetologia. 35, 595–601 (1992).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. 66.

    Kuzawa, C. W. Adipose tissue in human infancy and childhood: an evolutionary perspective. Am J Phys Anthropol. 27, 177–209 (1998).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. 67.

    Wells, J. C. K. The evolution of human fatness and susceptibility to obesity: an ethological approach. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc. 81(2), 183–205 (2006).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. 68.

    Widdowson, E. & McCance, R. A review: new thoughts on growth. Pediatr Res. 9(3), 154–6 (1975).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. 69.

    Hofman, M. Energy metabolism, brain size and longevity in mammals. Q Rev Biol. 58(4), 495–512 (1983).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. 70.

    Barker, D. J. The foetal and infant origins of adult disease. (BMJ Books, 1983).

  71. 71.

    Baker, J. et al. Brains versus brawn: an empirical test of Barker’s brain sparing model. Am J Hum Biol. 22(2), 206–15 (2010).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. 72.

    Latini, G. et al. Foetal growth of kidneys, liver and spleen in intrauterine growth restriction: “programming” causing “metabolic syndrome” in adult age. Acta Paediatr. 93(12), 1635–9 (2004).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. 73.

    Grunewald, P. Chronic Fetal Distress and Placental Insufficiency. Biol Neonate. 5, 215 (1963).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. 74.

    Oltmanns, K. M. et al. Insulin Resistance and the Cardiometabolic Syndrome: Adipose Tissue and Skeletal Muscle Factors Differential energetic response of brain vs. skeletal muscle upon glycemic variations in healthy humans. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 294, 12–6 (2008).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. 75.

    Hilsted, J., Jensen, K., Thomsen, C., Larsen, S. & Henriksen, O. Maintenance of high-energy brain phosphorous compounds during insulin-induced hypoglycemia in men: 31P nuclear magnetic resonance spectroscopy study. Diabetes. 37(6), 760–2 (1988).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. 76.

    Coker, R. & Kjaer, M. Glucoregulation during exercise. Sport Med. 35(7), 575–83 (2005).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. 77.

    Hagerman, F., Connors, M., Gault, J., Hagerman, G. & Polinski, W. Energy expenditure during simulated rowing. J Appl Physiol. 45(1), 87–93 (1978).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. 78.

    Golf, S. & Bender, S. On the Significance of Magnesium in Extreme Physical Stress. Cardiovasc Drugs Ther. 12, 197–202 (1988).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. 79.

    Pesce, C., Crova, C., Cereatti, L., Casella, R. & Bellucci, M. Physical activity and mental performance in preadolescents: Effects of acute exercise on free-recall memory. Ment Health Phys Act. 2(1), 16–22 (2009).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  80. 80.

    van der Helm, E., Gujar, N., Nishida, M. & Walker, M. Sleep-dependent facilitation of episodic memory details. PLoS One. 6(11), e27421 (2011).

    ADS  Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  81. 81.

    Kahana, M. & Howard, M. Spacing and lag effects in free recall of pure lists. Psychon Bull Rev. 12(1), 159–64 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. 82.

    Friendly, M., Franklin, P., Hoffman, D. & Rubin, D. Methods & Designs, The Toronto Word Pool: Norms for imagery, concreteness, orthographic variables, and grammatical usage for 1,080 words. Behav Res Methods Instrum. 14(4), 375–99 (1982).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. 83.

    Amichetti, N., Stanley, R., White, A. & Wingfield, A. Monitoring the capacity of working memory: Executive control and effects of listening effort. Mem Cognit. 41, 839–49 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

D.L., J.C.K.W. and J.T.S. designed the experiments, D.L. performed the experiments, analysed the results and wrote the paper. All authors reviewed the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Daniel Longman.

Ethics declarations

Competing Interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Longman, D., Stock, J.T. & Wells, J.C.K. A trade-off between cognitive and physical performance, with relative preservation of brain function. Sci Rep 7, 13709 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-14186-2

Download citation

Further reading

Comments

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.