Article

Big brains stabilize populations and facilitate colonization of variable habitats in birds

  • Nature Ecology & Evolution 117061715 (2017)
  • doi:10.1038/s41559-017-0316-2
  • Download Citation
Received:
Accepted:
Published online:

Abstract

The cognitive buffer hypothesis posits that environmental variability can be a major driver of the evolution of cognition because an enhanced ability to produce flexible behavioural responses facilitates coping with the unexpected. Although comparative evidence supports different aspects of this hypothesis, a direct connection between cognition and the ability to survive a variable and unpredictable environment has yet to be demonstrated. Here, we use complementary demographic and evolutionary analyses to show that among birds, the mechanistic premise of this hypothesis is well supported but the implied direction of causality is not. Specifically, we show that although population dynamics are more stable and less affected by environmental variation in birds with larger relative brain sizes, the evolution of larger brains often pre-dated and facilitated the colonization of variable habitats rather than the other way around. Our findings highlight the importance of investigating the timeline of evolutionary events when interpreting patterns of phylogenetic correlation.

Introduction

Enhanced encephalization, that is, a greater than expected brain mass for a given body size1, has evolved independently in numerous groups of animals despite its stringent energetic demands and potential developmental costs2,3,4. The cognitive buffer hypothesis posits that the repeated evolution of relatively large brains was driven primarily by the adaptive benefits of being able to mount quick, flexible behavioural responses to frequent or unexpected environmental change5,6. In line with this view, comparative studies have shown that more highly encephalized birds have greater potential for behavioural innovation7,8, lower mortality rates9,10 and a greater capacity to thrive in human-altered environments11,12. In addition, highly encephalized birds have been shown to preferentially occupy environments with more variable climates13,14, where biotic and abiotic conditions change considerably within and across years.

Although these findings are consistent with the cognitive buffer hypothesis, questions remain regarding its validity as a general explanation for the evolution of cognition. In particular, it is currently unclear whether the observed link between survival and encephalization is specifically driven by an enhanced ability to cope with environmental change, or driven instead by other adaptive benefits. In addition, a direction of causality in the relationship between encephalization and environmental variation has not yet been established. Specifically, the cognitive buffer hypothesis predicts that relatively large brains evolved in situ as a result of selection for coping with environmental variation5. However, large brains could have also evolved elsewhere and may have subsequently facilitated the colonization of variable habitats, as suggested by recent reports that anthropogenic introductions of highly encephalized vertebrates to new habitats tend to have higher success rates15,16,17. Here, we leverage the power of modern evolutionary analyses, broad-scale comparative datasets and citizen science to clarify these fundamental issues regarding the role of ecological variation in the evolution of cognition. We begin by applying current state-of-the-art demographic analyses to test directly the mechanistic assumption that enhanced encephalization improves survival in variable habitats. We then apply models of correlated trait evolution to formally assess the direction of causality in the observed correlation between the occupancy of variable habitats and high encephalization in birds.

Results

Estimating cognitive ability

In line with prior large-scale comparative studies on the evolution of cognition, we use relative brain size as a proxy for cognitive ability1. This metric acknowledges that absolute brain size increases naturally in larger species, and estimates instead a species’ cognitive ability as the extent to which its brain is larger (or smaller) than expected for its body size. The relative brain sizes used in our analyses were computed as residuals from a phylogenetic generalized least squares (PGLS) regression of ln brain on ln body size (slope = 0.59 ± 0.00 s.d.; intercept = −2.48 ± 0.05 s.d.; λ = 0.87 ± 0.01 s.d.), including the 2,062 bird species for which brain size is currently available (see Methods and Supplementary Data 2). While such a proxy for cognition is clearly indirect, we note that there is a growing body of experimental and correlative evidence linking relative brain size with cognitive ability18,19,20, and more specifically with behavioural innovation21,22.

Does greater cognition improve survival in more variable environments?

One way to evaluate directly whether enhanced cognition increases survival in more variable environments is to test explicitly whether the interaction between encephalization and environmental variability has a significant effect on population dynamics. If behavioural flexibility facilitates coping with unexpected ecological challenges, then we predict that population dynamics in highly encephalized species should be buffered from environmental extremes and should therefore be less affected by increased environmental variability as compared with those of small-brained species.

We tested this prediction in a sample of North American land birds for which brain size is known and time series data are sufficient to properly estimate year-to-year variation in breeding population numbers23 (N = 126 species; Supplementary Data 1). Demographic data for this analysis were obtained from the North American Breeding Bird Survey (BBS)24, a yearly standardized assessment of breeding bird abundances conducted since 1966 at thousands of locations across the continent. Following the current community standards25, we used hierarchical Bayesian models to estimate regional population dynamics for each species in each North American bird conservation region (BCR; Fig. 1a). BCRs are ecologically distinct regions26 and are widely regarded as suitable biogeographic units for the quantification of population dynamics23. The hierarchical models implemented here estimate yearly fluctuations in abundance while accounting for long-term population trends, route-to-route variation in abundance and imperfect detection by observers (Fig. 1; see Methods). By explicitly separating the sources of error in reported bird counts, these models allow us to estimate the extent to which year-to-year fluctuations in true population size are a product of ecologically relevant processes such as the mortality induced by environmental extremes (also known as ‘process error’ or σ γ ; Fig. 1). Species-specific abundance-weighted averages of the process error, σ ̄ γ (see Methods), were subsequently used to test the hypothesis that population stability is less affected by environmental variability in larger-brained species. To better align our metrics with the narrative of this hypothesis, the dependent variable in these downstream analyses was the negative of σ ̄ γ , hereafter ‘population stability’, such that higher stability scores reflect cases with less pronounced year-to-year fluctuation in population size.

Fig. 1: Graphical representation of our method for estimating and comparing population dynamics of North American birds.
Fig. 1

Population dynamics were first estimated independently for every species, here the northern bobwhite (Colinus virginianus), in every North American conservation region (outlined in purple) and subsequently reduced to individual weighted averages per species. a, Data come from counts (shown as inset plots) reported at routes (depicted as dots on the map) within conservation regions. b, Models estimate the log of abundances that follow a general long-term trend (dark red line) and yearly fluctuations around the trend (light red line) that that are drawn from a normal distribution with mean of 0 and standard deviation of σ γ .

We used PGLS regression models estimated across a sample of 1,000 tree topologies from ref. 27 to investigate the potential effects of environmental variability and encephalization on population stability. Environmental predictors for these models included the mean, within-year variance and predictability of temperature, precipitation and net primary productivity (see Methods). Predictability was estimated through Colwell’s P, an index that captures variation among years in the onset, intensity and duration of periodic phenomena28. Given the strong spatial covariance that is typically observed among environmental parameters29, all environmental variables were first extracted globally at a spatial resolution of 0.5 × 0.5° and subsequently reduced to composite variables at the same resolution using principal component analysis (PCA; Table 1; Supplementary Fig. 1a,b; Methods). As environmental correlations are often region-specific30, the PCA for this regional analysis included only map cells located within our North American study region. The first principal component recovered from this analysis showed a clear latitudinal trend, where lower scores occurred primarily in northern, more seasonal climates with colder and less predictable temperatures, and high scores occurred in southwestern sites with hotter temperatures and more variable, unpredictable precipitation patterns (Supplementary Fig. 1a). The second component of the North American environmental PCA captured differences in mean precipitation as well as in mean, variance and predictability of net primary productivity. In this case, higher scores indicated wetter environments with higher, but more seasonal and unpredictable, productivity including those found along the Pacific coast of the northern USA and Canada, boreal forests, and much of the eastern USA. Low scores for PC2 were found in southwestern deserts and in the far north (Supplementary Fig. 1b).

Table 1: Summary of PCA of environmental variables across North America

When characterizing the typical habitats of each species in our sample, we considered both spatial distribution and geographic variation in abundance. We first calculated mean environmental components for every North American BCR ( P C 1 ̲ B C R i and P C 2 ̲ B C R i ). Then, we estimated species-specific habitat values, hereafter H1 and H2, by computing the weighted averages of P C 1 ̲ B C R and P C 2 ̲ B C R , where weights were proportional to the relative abundance of the species in each BCR. Correlation between H1 and H2 was high (r = −0.56; Supplementary Fig. 1c), so we excluded the latter from our list of predictors to prevent possible multicollinearity and unnecessary variance inflation. The decision to keep H1 rather than H2 was based on the fact that H1 most directly captures the measures of variability that are relevant for testing the mechanism behind the cognitive buffer hypothesis. We note that both high and low values of H1 reflect increasingly variable and unpredictable conditions. Specifically, low H1 scores indicate variable temperatures, whereas high scores indicate variable precipitation. Thus, to explore the general effects of environmental variability on population dynamics, we included H1 as a quadratic term (H12) in our models of population stability. As H1 is centred at zero, this quadratic term captures the potential effects of both variable temperatures and variable precipitation, and is therefore labelled ‘environmental variability’ hereafter.

We also took into account the possibility that population stability is influenced by a variety of life history and ecological traits. First, we accounted for potential relationships between relative population variability and population size31 by including log-transformed mean abundance as a covariate in our models. Additionally, we considered that environmental variability could affect population dynamics through interactions with traits other than brain size. For example, we considered that lifespan could be a predictor of population stability because longer-lived species tend to exhibit higher adult survival32, and we included an interaction with environmental variability (H12) because highly unpredictable conditions may prevent individuals from realizing their maximum lifespan potential. Similarly, we considered the fact that species with higher annual reproductive output may experience more intense year-to-year population oscillations33 and that this effect could potentially be amplified in more variable habitats. Additionally, we explored the possibility that variable conditions have weaker effects on the population dynamics of large-bodied species because those species tend to be more resilient to periods of resource scarcity34. The same may be true for cooperative breeders—which seem to be able to buffer the effects of harsh years through helping at the nest35—for species with generalist habits—which are typically able to exploit a wider variety of environmental conditions36—and for migrants—which typically avoid the harshest conditions of their breeding grounds by temporarily leaving the area29. Further details on how these traits were defined and quantified can be found in the Methods. All of our data on population stability, brain size, ecology and life history are available in Supplementary Data 1.

Our demographic analysis revealed that a number of ecological traits are significantly associated with population variability (adjusted R2 for PGLS model = 0.22; Table 2). We found that while populations of resident species are less stable in increasingly variable environments, migratory species maintain relatively stable populations across all types of environment ( P ̄  << 0.001; Fig. 2a). Similarly, long-lived species were found to exhibit more stable dynamics than short-lived ones in only the most mild, predictable environments ( P ̄  << 0.001; Fig. 2b), indicating that the potential benefits of long life spans may diminish when conditions are uncertain. Consistent with the idea that cognitive ability improves survival in variable environments, we found a significant interaction between encephalization and H12. Specifically, while species with high encephalization were found to maintain relatively stable populations in both stable and variable environments, those with low encephalization showed a significant decline in population stability as environmental variability increased ( P ̄  << 0.001; Fig. 2c). Our findings are qualitatively similar when phylogenetic relationships are estimated from a consensus tree rather than across a sample of tree topologies (Supplementary Table 1).

Table 2: Summary results of our PGLS regression models of population stability across a sample of 1,000 potential tree topologies for the avian phylogeny
Fig. 2: Significant two-way interactions between species traits and environmental variability on population stability.
Fig. 2

ac, Partial residual plots from phylogenetic generalized linear model with fitted lines indicating the effect of environmental variability on population stability for different migratory strategies (a) and at different values of longevity (b) and relative brain size (c) in a sample of 126 species of North American birds. Environmental variability is measured H1, with low values indicating cold, seasonal habitats with unpredictable temperatures, and high values indicating hot habitats with variable and unpredictable precipitation patterns (see text and Table 1). Traits in b and c were analysed as continuous variables but, for visualization purposes, the fitted lines depicted here predict population stability trends for species at the 5th, 25th, 50th, 75th and 95th percentiles of each focal trait while holding all other predictors at their mean value and setting migratory status as resident.

Although these initial results support the basic mechanistic premise of the cognitive buffer hypothesis, the hierarchical models described above do not account for the fact that variation in population size can be driven not only by exogenous (environmental) factors, but also by internal, or density-dependent factors. In the context of hierarchical modelling, density-dependent processes can be investigated by modelling an explicit demographic process that assumes that true population sizes oscillate around a demographic equilibrium value that does not change over time37 (for example, the Gompertz function38). This assumption is nevertheless clearly violated whenever populations undergo long-term changes in mean abundance, as is the case in many North American land birds39 and nearly 80% of the species in our dataset. As models with density dependence are known to perform poorly in such species40, we explored the effects of density dependence exclusively on the subset of species that did not show any evidence of long-term changes in mean abundance in our initial set of demographic analyses. Given the relatively small number of species in this category (N = 27), these confirmatory analyses could not meaningfully explore the entire set of initial predictors and were therefore focused on evaluating only the potential effects of relative brain size, H12 and their interaction. These more narrowly defined analyses indicate that accounting for density dependence does not change our main finding. That is, the interaction between relative brain size and environmental variability is significant in PGLS models based on the consensus tree (relative brain size × H12: β = 0.63, P = 0.04; relative brain size: β = −0.05, P = 0.84; H12: β = −0.35, p = 0.01), and marginally significant across the entire sample of 1,000 tree topologies (relative brain size × H12: β ̄  = 0.61, P ̄  = 0.06, f = 0.41; relative brain size: β ̄  = −0.04, p ̄  = 0.88, f = 0; H12: β ̄  = −0.32, P ̄  = 0.02, f = 1.00). The marginal significance observed in the latter case highlights the greater effect of phylogenetic uncertainty and the generally low statistical power of comparative tests that are based on only a small number of species.

Did larger brains evolve in more variable environments?

Our demographic analyses lend support to the underlying mechanistic premise of the cognitive buffer hypothesis, which is that higher encephalization can improve survival, specifically when environmental conditions are increasingly unstable. However, to evaluate the extent to which this mechanism provides a general explanation for the evolution of cognition in birds, it is critical to explore the direction of causality in the correlation between an enhanced potential for cognition and the occupancy of variable environments. A clear understanding of the sequence of evolutionary events is particularly necessary in this context because the adaptive benefits invoked by the cognitive buffer hypothesis may just as well promote the evolution of cognition in variable habitats, or facilitate instead the secondary colonization of variable habitats by already highly encephalized species41.

We evaluated the support for these two non-mutually exclusive evolutionary scenarios by using reversible-jump Markov chain Monte Carlo (rjMCMC) to estimate models of correlated trait evolution42 fitted to an exhaustive global sample of non-migratory birds for which brain size is known (N = 1,288 species; Supplementary Data 2). These models allow inference into potential evolutionary timelines by assessing the likelihood that rates of evolutionary transitions between states of a binary trait (for example, moderate to large encephalization) are dependent on the state of a second binary trait (for example, stable versus variable environmental habitats). In the context of the cognitive buffer hypothesis, these models allow us to test whether the transition from small to large brains is indeed more likely in variable than in stable environments (that is, whether variable environments tend to pre-date large brains). Similarly, these models allow us to evaluate the likelihood of alternative, yet non-mutually exclusive, timelines such as the ‘colonization advantage’ scenario, which predicts that the transition from stable to variable environments should be more likely in large- than in small-brained species.

As in our demographic analysis, environmental variables were first extracted for the relevant study region (here, the entire globe) and subsequently reduced to composite variables through PCA (Supplementary Table 2). The first component of this global PCA, hereafter ‘temperature variability’, captured a gradient of increasing exposure to colder, more seasonally variable and less predictable temperatures (Supplementary Fig. 1d). The second component, hereafter ‘xeric variability’, captured a gradient of increasing exposure to drier and less productive environments with more unpredictable precipitation (Supplementary Fig. 1e). Species-specific habitats were characterized in this case by computing the mean values of local temperature and xeric variability across entire breeding distributions (see Methods).

Because transition rate analyses require discrete trait states, we explored a reasonable range of thresholds for classifying species as having either small or large encephalization, and as being exposed to highly variable or fairly stable environments (30th, 50th, 75th and 90th percentile; see Methods). Encephalization categorizations were based on whether a species’ relative brain size was above or below the predefined threshold. Similarly, exposure to environmental variability was considered high for a given species if either or both environmental principal component scores belonged in a percentile above the predefined threshold. Considering information from both principal components when characterizing exposure to environmental variability allowed us to maintain consistency with our demographic analyses (see Table 1) and to explore the general effects of environmental variability rather than the specific effects of temperature or precipitation variation.

Our models of correlated trait evolution do not support the main prediction of the cognitive buffer hypothesis under any combination of thresholds. Specifically, the evolution of larger relative brain sizes was generally found to be equally likely for species occurring in stable environments and in harsher, more variable ones (that is, there was no support for a difference in transition rate from moderate to large encephalization between environment types; Bayes factor (BF) < 3; Fig. 3d,f; Supplementary Table 3). Furthermore, under certain classification criteria, we even find evidence that advanced encephalization could be more likely to evolve in stable than in highly variable habitats (for example, highly variable environments: >50th percentile; large encephalization: >50th percentile; BF = 3.15; Fig. 3a,c; Supplementary Table 3). Collectively, these results indicate that while environmental variability can theoretically select for enhanced cognition, it is in fact unlikely to have driven many of the major transitions towards large brains in birds.

Fig. 3: Testing the sequence of evolutionary events predicted by the cognitive buffer and colonization advantage hypotheses.
Fig. 3

The cognitive buffer hypothesis predicts that larger brains should preferentially evolve in variable environments, whereas the colonization advantage hypothesis predicts that, once evolved, they should subsequently aid in colonizing variable habitats. a,d, Posterior distributions of the difference in transition rate from moderate to large encephalization in stable versus variable climates (depicted by blue arrows in c and f). b,e, Posterior distribution of the difference in transition rate from stable to variable environments in moderate vs large brained species (depicted by green arrows in c and f) estimated from a sample of 1,288 resident terrestrial bird species from around the globe. Distributions in a, b, d and e are derived from the posterior distributions of rjMCMC analyses (see Supplementary Fig. 3). Positive values indicate support for a particular hypothesis (see Methods). Horizontal red lines in these panels mark the maximum proportion of steps in which the rjMCMC chain can visit a rate difference of 0 while still supporting a scenario in which the two rates of interest differ (that is, BF = 3). Thus, we see positive support for difference in transition rates in a, b and e but not in d. c,f, The full transition matrices summarizing estimated rates of transition between stable (grey) and variable (black outlined) environments or between moderate (purple) and big (pink) relative brain size. Arrow widths are proportional to estimated rates, single asterisks indicate positive support for a difference between rates (BF > 3) and double asterisks indicate strong support for a difference between rates (BF > 12).

In stark contrast, we found that the evidence of an improved colonization ability of variable habitats in highly encephalized avian lineages is both general and strong (Fig. 3b,c,e,f; Supplementary Table 3). Such colonization advantage seems to be specifically linked to an improved ability to deal with environmental variability, because we did not find support for a difference in transition rate from variable to stable habitats between species with small and large encephalization values (Supplementary Table 3). Additionally, our results indicate that even moderate enhancements in cognitive ability and/or moderate increases in environmental variability can help accrue such advantages: when thresholds for classification are too conservative (for example, variable environments: >90th percentile; large encephalization: >75th percentile), differences in transition rates from stable to variable environments are no longer detectable between very large- and moderately large-brained species.

Discussion

Our demographic analysis broadly supports the notion that enhanced cognition can lead to more stable population dynamics. Furthermore, the significant interaction between H12 and encephalization is consistent with the idea that these benefits can be generally accrued under different types of environmental variability and unpredictability (see Table 1). We therefore conclude that there is general support for the proposed mechanism underlying the cognitive buffer hypothesis, which is that bigger than expected brains improve survival when environmental change is frequent and unexpected.

Despite this finding, our transition rate analyses strongly indicate that the general timeline of evolutionary events suggested by the cognitive buffer hypothesis is not broadly supported across the avian phylogeny. Specifically, our results unambiguously indicate that evolutionary transitions towards occupancy of more variable habitats did not generally precede the evolution of enhanced encephalization in birds. Ancestral state reconstructions facilitate the visualization of this result (Fig. 4): several of the most highly encephalized clades in the bird phylogeny (for example, parrots, bowerbirds and hornbills) evolved big brains without any apparent exposure to particularly harsh or variable habitats throughout their evolutionary history (Fig. 4b,c,e). Furthermore, even in clades that currently occupy variable habitats (for example, corvids or woodpeckers), it is unclear that exposure to relatively high ecological variability preceded the evolution of larger brains (Fig. 4d,f). Why then do we see today a correlation between variable habitats and encephalization? Our analyses suggest that this correlation results from either the preferential colonization of variable and unpredictable habitats by highly encephalized species, or the preferential persistence of these highly encephalized species in habitats that underwent major environmental change and became more variable. One possible reason for this pattern is that highly encephalized birds have lower risk of extirpation during the early stages of colonization (that is, when abundances are low43) because of their enhanced ability to withstand environmental change. Similar links between cognition and range expansion have been made in studies documenting the success of highly encephalized species in colonizing new habitats16,17,41, and are the basis of our current understanding of the process of human expansion out of Africa8,44.

Fig. 4: Ancestral trait reconstruction of relative brain size and environmental niche.
Fig. 4

a, phylogeny of the 1,288 species in our global sample depicting the reconstructed ancestral states of avian encephalization. bg, reconstruction details for six highly encephalized clades: encephalization (left panels; pink = upper 25th percentile; grey = lower 75th percentile) and variable environmental niches (right panels; blue, upper 25th percentile of ‘temperature variability’ (tv); yellow, upper 25th percentile of ‘xeric variability’ (xv); black, upper 25th percentile for both measures; grey, bottom 75th percentile for both measures). Subtrees in bg are details of reconstructions performed on continuous trait data with branches re-coloured based on the 75th percentile threshold used in analyses of correlated trait evolution (see Methods).

Overall, our results suggest that even though environmental variability can be a viable agent of selection in the evolution of cognition (as also concluded by refs 14,45), this particular mechanism is unlikely to have driven many of the most striking cases of encephalization among birds. It is nevertheless possible that other types of ecological variability not included in this study can explain such transitions. For example, although many parrots and hornbills tend to occupy habitats with fairly stable climates, these species must typically cope with high levels of variation in the location and timing of fruiting trees (a similar situation is likely to occur in other species with complex feeding ecologies45). While we acknowledge that a broad interpretation of ‘variability’ can increase the scope and generality of the cognitive buffer hypothesis5, we note that overgeneralization may lead to the inadvertent mischaracterization of very different types of selection (for example, problem solving, long-term memory, or spatial awareness) as different but equivalent forms of a single process. A perhaps more fruitful approach would therefore be to explore the possibility that there is no single primary driver in the evolution of relatively large brains, and that this process is instead driven by the combined effects of both the constraints2,3,4 and the various potential adaptive benefits of increased processing capacity, including the ability to respond more quickly to new challenges46,47, navigate more complex social interactions48,49, process more intricate sensory information50, and cope with greater spatial and/or temporal variability15,22. As data on these different processes become more readily available, we are confident that future comparative studies will be able to disentangle the relative extent to which these different forces have shaped the evolution of cognition on different taxonomic scales. In the meantime, we hope that the realization that variation in brain size was more likely to shape the distribution of bird species across the globe rather than the other way around can help inform more immediate research agendas.

Methods

Quantification of relative brain size

Our estimates of relative brain size were based on body size data from ref. 51 and brain size data either from published accounts (N = 1,949 species; cited in Supplementary Data 2) or measured directly from museum specimens (N = 113 species). Our total brain dataset includes several species that are not used in either our demographic or correlated trait evolution analyses. Specifically, pelagic species (orders Sphenisciformes, Suliformes, Procellariiformes and Phaethontiformes; families Pelecanidae, Laridae, Stercorariidae and Alcidae) were initially included when computing encephalization values but were subsequently excluded from downstream analyses because land surface temperature and precipitation values are unlikely to be indicative of the actual conditions experienced by species that spend most of their time at sea. All brain size measurements from museum specimens were obtained following the procedures outlined in refs 3,52. Briefly, the foraminae of the cranial nerves are sealed with masking tape and lead shot is poured into the foramen magnum. To prevent the formation of lacunae, the skull is lightly tapped throughout this procedure. Once the shot has risen to the foramen magnum, the contents are decanted into modified syringes or graduated cylinders to determine volume. This method is highly repeatable and provides an accurate estimate of brain size in birds52,53. Brain sizes that were originally reported as volumes in the literature were converted to mass by multiplying millilitres by the average density of fresh brain tissue (1.036 g ml−1)52.

To account for phylogenetic uncertainty, the log-log regression of brain size on body size was independently run on 1,000 randomly selected tree topologies with the Hackett backbone in ref. 27 (www.birdtree.org; downloaded 14 July 2016). The encephalization values used in all of our downstream analyses were computed as the median residuals for each species across these 1,000 models.

Characterization of environmental variability

The environmental variables we consider here include the mean, within-year variance and predictability of temperature, precipitation and net primary productivity. Monthly raster maps of temperature and precipitation values were obtained for years 1900–2005 from ecoClimate.org (provided at 1° resolution, resampled to 0.5° resolution; downloaded 25 July 2016)54. Monthly net primary productivity data for years 2000–2016 were obtained from the MODIS dataset downloaded from NASA Earth Observations (provided at 0.5° resolution; http://neo.sci.gsfc.nasa.gov; accessed 18 March 2016). Predictability was measured as Colwell’s P28, an information-theory-based index that captures variation in the onset, intensity and duration of periodic phenomena and ranges from 0 (completely unpredictable) to 1 (completely predictable).

As environmental variables tend to be strongly correlated29, we reduced the original set of environmental predictors (transformed when required55, centred and scaled) through PCA. Separate analyses were conducted to reduce the dimensionality of environmental data in the demographic and correlated trait evolution sections to account for the fact that environmental correlations are often region-specific30. In the demographic analyses, the environmental PCA was based only on North American data, including all cell values north of the US–Mexico border (that is, only the geographic region where breeding bird survey data are available). In the correlated trait evolution analyses, the environmental PCA included all global terrestrial habitats, excluding Antarctica. Both environmental PCAs recovered similar components (see main text, Table 1 and Supplementary Table 2 for details). In the demographic analysis, the average score for each principal component was initially computed for every BCR and these regional averages were subsequently used to characterize species-typical habitats. Specifically, variables H1 and H2 were computed as weighted averages of the corresponding environmental components (PC1 and PC2), where weights were determined by the species’ relative abundance in each conservation region. Species-typical environmental values for the global analysis of correlated trait evolution were estimated directly by averaging all local (0.5 × 0.5° cell) PCA scores across the species’ entire breeding distribution.

Bird population data

Abundance data for our population dynamics analyses were collected between 1966 and 2014 by the North American BBS (available through www.pwrc.usgs.gov/bbs/; downloaded 28 August 2015)24. The BBS is coordinated by the US Geological Survey (USGS) and the Canadian Wildlife Service and conducted annually by trained volunteers during the height of the breeding season. Participants travel along 24.5-mile roadside routes, conducting 3-min point count surveys at 0.5-mile intervals—recording every bird seen or heard within a 0.25-mile radius. Each BBS survey route was assigned to a single BCR based on route starting coordinates23. BCR maps were provided by the USGS Patuxent Wildlife Research Center (www.pwcr.usgs.gov; downloaded 15 September 2015). Only surveys fulfilling BBS quality criteria (that is, runtype = 1) were included in our analyses.

Quantification of population dynamics

We characterized the temporal dynamics of bird populations within BCRs across North America using hierarchical Bayesian models following ref. 25. The log of abundance, x j,i,t , for a given species at survey route j within BCR i in year t is modelled as:

log x = S i + β i ×t+ γ i , t + ω i , j +ηI j , t + ɛ i , j , t

where S i is the average abundance within BCR i, β i is the temporal trend in abundance within BCR i and η is the first-year observer effect where I(j,t) is 1 if the survey at year t is an observer’s first record at route j and 0 otherwise. Year effects, γ i,t , and route-observer effects, ω i,j , are modelled as BCR specific random effects, whereas ε i,j,t was modelled as a general random effect of count overdispersion. Given the potential for differences in observer ability, a separate value of ω is given to each unique route-observer combination. To account for imperfect detection during surveys, the observed count on route j within BCR i during year t is assumed to have a Poisson distribution with mean x j,i,t . Abundances are allowed to vary among survey routes within a BCR, but all routes are assumed to follow the same relative temporal trend (β i ) and to undergo the same yearly fluctuations around this trend (γ i,t ). The variance of route-observer effects within a BCR, σ ω i 2 , is drawn from a global hyperdistribution. To conform with the assumption of normality of residuals in general linear models, we use the negative of the standard deviation in annual fluctuations (−1 × sqrt( σ γ i 2 )) as our dependent variable in subsequent analyses of population stability. The sign inversion is simply done to facilitate interpretation of our results, such that higher values reflect more stable populations.

As hierarchical models tend to underestimate the magnitude of annual fluctuations when the number of missing survey years is high56, we estimated trends for a period when survey data is relatively consistent, namely from 1985 onwards. In addition, we improved data quality by including only route-observer combinations with 10 or more years of survey data and estimating only parameters for BCRs with at least 20 years of survey data and a minimum of 14 survey routes39. Model parameters were estimated with MCMC analysis using package ‘rjags’57. Four independent chains were run for each model, each of which included a burn-in of 25,000 steps, an additional chain length of 25,000 steps and a thinning interval of 10. Priors for S i , β i and η were normal distributions with mean of 0 and variance of 106. Prior distributions for variances were inverse gamma distributions with scale and shape equal to 0.001. Our assessment of chain convergence was done through the ‘coda’ package in R58 and included both a visual inspection of the traces of posterior estimates and an estimation of potential scale reduction factors (PSRF) via Gelman and Rubin’s convergence diagnostic59. Only estimates obtained from BCRs in which PSRF values were under 1.1 for all parameters (that is, chains with proper convergence) were included in our subsequent analyses of population stability. We considered positive support for temporal trends when the 95% credible interval of β i did not include zero.

Hierarchical models with density dependence were also fitted to all species that did not exhibit evidence of linear trends in our initial analysis (n = 27). Specifically, we re-estimated population stability for these species using a discrete time, stochastic Gompertz model following ref. 38. These models estimate density-dependent population change at the route level while allowing random environmentally driven fluctuations and accounting for observer error in reported abundances. The log of abundance at time t, log(x t ), is modelled here as a function of log(x t−1):

l o g x t =a+b× l o g x t - 1 + E t

where a is the intrinsic rate of increase and b is the strength of density dependence. Values of b were allowed to range from −1 (strong) to 1 (no density dependence)37. Relative annual fluctuations, E t , have a normal distribution with mean zero and variance σ2 E . Similarly, the log of observed counts in year t is assumed to have a distribution with mean of log(x t ) and a variance of τ2. To conform with the assumption of normality of residuals in general linear models, we used the negative log of the estimated year-to-year variance (that is, −1 × log(σ2 E )), as our dependent variable in subsequent analyses of population stability. As above, the sign inversion here is simply done to facilitate interpretation of our results, such that higher values reflect more stable populations. Data quality checks for hierarchical models with density dependence included estimating only models for routes with at least 20 years of survey data from 1985 onwards and no more than three consecutive years of missing data. Parameters were estimated using MCMC analysis with four independent chains, each ran with a burn-in period of 100,000 steps, an additional chain length of 50,000 steps and a thinning interval of 10 steps. Priors for a were drawn from a non-informative uniform distribution from 0 to 106, for b from a uniform distribution from −1 to 1, and for σ2 E and τ2 from an inverse gamma distribution with scale and shape equal to 0.001. As with our linear trend models, chain convergence diagnostics were performed through visual inspection and the Gelman and Rubin convergence diagnostic59. Data for downstream analyses of population stability only included estimates for routes that reached proper convergence.

For both linear trend and density-dependence hierarchical models, we excluded species that typically pose clear challenges to detection, such as aquatic (families Gaviidae, Podicipedidae, Pelecanidae, Phalacrocoracidae, Anhingidae, Anatidae, Rallidae, Ardeidae, Threskiornithidae and Ciconiidae), nocturnal (families Tytonidae, Strigidae and Caprimulgidae) and primarily aerial species (families Apodidae and Hirundinidae). For all other species, we summarized regional measures of population stability into a single species-specific value by computing density-weighted averages across BCRs (linear trend models) or routes (density-dependence models). Thus, our measures of population variability account for differences in population dynamics across a species’ range60, but place greater importance on the population dynamics that occur in regions or sites where the species is better represented.

Estimating correlates of population stability

Data on longevity and annual reproductive output were obtained from ref. 51 (the latter was calculated as the product of clutch size and clutches per year). Social systems were classified as either cooperative or non-cooperative breeding based on ref. 61. Habitat generalism was measured as the number of different BCRs in which a species was reported throughout the BBS dataset. Migratory status was determined from range maps by BirdLife International (birdlife.org; downloaded 18 March 2016). Specifically, a species was considered resident if there was complete overlap between winter and breeding portions of its range, and considered migratory otherwise.

To test the effects of putative predictor variables on population stability scores we used PGLS regression models estimated with the ‘geiger’62 and ‘nlme’63 packages in R64. All regression models (including the one used to estimate relative brain sizes) were computed using Pagel’s λ transformation. To account for uncertainty in phylogenetic relationships, every regression model reported here was independently run with 1,000 different tree topologies from ref. 27. Model fit was assessed through adjusted R2 (ref. 65). In the main text we report the average estimated coefficient for each parameter and the proportion of trees in which such estimates were significant (that is, the f statistic). Body size, longevity, annual reproductive output and estimated mean abundance were log-transformed prior to analysis. Our fully parameterized models included all main effects as well as interactions between longevity, annual reproductive output, habitat generalism, body size, relative brain size, sociality and migration with H12. Models were subsequently reduced by iteratively removing, one at a time, terms with the highest P value (removing interactions prior to main effects) and assessing whether removal led to a significant improvement of Akaike information criterion (AIC) values (that is, ΔAIC > 2). We also computed variance inflation factors (VIF) for all of our reduced models to confirm low potential for multicollinearity (all VIF values were <2).

Estimating evolutionary rates of transition between character states

We investigated the potential timeline of evolution of encephalization and climactic niche in birds using models of correlated trait evolution42, implemented through the discrete function of BayesTraits v2 on a global sample of species (Supplementary Data 2). Pelagic and migratory species were excluded from these analyses, resulting in a total sample of 1,288 resident terrestrial species. BayesTraits estimates the eight possible transition rates between potential character states (see Fig. 3c or f), assuming that simultaneous transitions in both brain size and environment are so unlikely that they can be ignored42. As both brain size and environmental variability are continuous variables, we explored a number of different cutoff values to convert them into binary traits suitable for this kind of analysis. Specifically, we classified species as having large encephalization values when they occurred above the 30th, 50th, 75th and 90th percentile of brain size distribution. While a 30th percentile cutoff for encephalization may seem too permissive at first glance, we note that this was the minimum possible threshold at which all ‘large-brained’ species had a positive brain residual (that is, bigger brain than expected from body size) and the number of observed transitions between different states was sufficient for the proper estimation of transition rates66. We note that the skewed distribution towards more highly encephalized species in our sample is due to the effects of phylogenetic correction in the estimation of relative brain size, as well as to the subsampling of species from our much larger global brain dataset. Exposure to environmental variability was classified as high for species above the 50th, 75th and 90th percentiles in either ‘temperature variability’ or ‘xeric variability’. Because models of correlated trait evolution have the potential to identify spurious correlations when the number of transitions between states is low66, we began by confirming that all of our thresholds yielded a reasonable number of transitions between states using ancestral character state estimation via the R package ‘phytools’67 and averaging the detected number of transitions across 1,000 tree topologies. At the 30th percentile threshold we detected an average of 29 transitions from small to large encephalization and 65 transitions from large to small encephalization. At the 50th percentile threshold we detected an average of 102 transitions from small to large encephalization, 112 transitions from large to small encephalization, 253 transitions from stable to variable environments and 414 transitions from variable to stable environments. At the 75th percentile threshold we detected an average of 64 transitions from small to large encephalization, 36 transitions from large to small encephalization, 265 transitions from stable to variable environments and 195 transitions from variable to stable environments. Finally, at the 90th percentile threshold we detected an average of 46 transitions from small to large encephalization, 15 transitions from large to small encephalization, 237 transitions from stable to variable environments and 127 transitions from variable to stable environments. The 90th percentile threshold was therefore ultimately dropped as a criterion for dichotomizing encephalization because the low number of transitions it yielded would preclude any meaningful estimates of transition rates66.

Rates of evolutionary transition were estimated using rjMCMC analyses. Parameter values were first estimated using maximum likelihood analysis to inform our choice of priors. For all six combinations of cutoff, we calculated mean values of transition rates across our sample of 1,000 trees. Maximum likelihood estimates of each parameter value were of a similar magnitude regardless of cutoffs and ranged from 0.00002 to 0.34. Next, rjMCMC analyses were performed for 200,000,000 iterations with a burn-in of 5,000,000, a thinning interval of 1,000 iterations and an exponential prior whose mean is seeded from a uniform hyperprior ranging between 0 and 0.5. Reversible-jump helps avoid model over-parameterization by exploring alternative models that can differ in parameter number68. Because reversible-jump analyses estimate the posterior probability of all possible model configurations along with individual parameter values, this algorithm offers the additional advantage of enabling tests of very specific hypothesis. Specifically, the posterior distribution of model types obtained through rjMCMC can be used to assess the strength of evidence that two particular transitions are different or not by comparing the relative sampling frequency of models in which the two transition types were constrained to be the same with that of models in which these two rates were allowed to vary independently of each other69. Statistically, these comparisons are made via BFs, which are calculated as:

B F i j =P M i | D P M j | D xP M j P M j

where i is the model set where rates are allowed to vary independently, j is a reduced model set in which the two rates are constrained to be the same, P(M n |D) is the posterior probability of model set n (computed as the proportion of steps in which the chain visited model n) and P(M n ) is the prior probability of model set n68,69. For example, when testing the cognitive buffer hypothesis, P(M i |D) is the frequency of all model configurations within the posterior distribution in which the transition rate from moderate to large encephalization varied between stable and variable environments, whereas j includes all model configurations in the posterior distribution where these rates were constrained to be equal in both environments. Similarly, when testing the colonization advantage scenario, P(M i |D) is the frequency of all model configurations in which the transition rate from stable to variable environments varied between moderate and large encephalization, while j includes all configurations where these rates were constrained to be equal in both brain size classes. P(M n ) values for this formula are computed by exploring all possible model combinations via expanded Stirling numbers69: P(M j ) = 0.9592 and P(M i ) = 0.0408. Overall, resulting BF values from 3 to 12 suggest positive support for model set i and values above 12 suggest that model set i is strongly supported when compared with model set j68. We also report the proportion of steps in our model chains (P) in which the difference between two rates of interest was equal to zero (that is, the transition rate for the character of interest was independent of the state of the second trait). In this case, values of P < 0.014 indicate positive support for a difference between rates (that is, BF > 3)69. Because hypothesis testing directly assesses the proportion of steps in the posterior distribution where transition rates of interest are constrained to be equal, we visualize these results by plotting the distribution of ‘rate differences’ calculated across the posterior distribution. These rate differences were calculated at each step of the chain as either the difference in estimated transition rate from moderate to large brain sizes in variable versus stable environments (when testing the cognitive buffer hypothesis), or the difference in estimated transition rates from stable to variable environments in species with large versus moderate brain sizes (when testing the colonization advantage hypothesis). Plotting the distributions of rate differences (Fig. 3) allows us to assess both the support for a particular hypothesis (the proportion of steps where rate difference = 0) and the directionality of these potential differences. Besides explicitly testing the cognitive buffer and colonization advantage scenarios as indicated above, we also tested for differences in the rates of colonization of stable environments between brain size classes as well as for differences in the rate of evolution of small to moderate brain sizes in stable versus variable habitats.

We ran each rjMCMC analysis three times to insure chain convergence and assess the consistency of our results. These checks were performed with the ‘coda’ package in R58 and included visually inspecting the traces of all of our posterior estimates, assuring effective sample sizes were greater than 1,000, and estimating PSRF using Gelman and Rubin’s convergence diagnostic59. PSRF values were below 1.1 for all parameter estimates indicating proper chain convergence properties. Effective sample sizes over 1,000 were obtained for all runs, except for analyses using the combination of 50th percentile encephalization threshold and 75th percentile environment threshold. To ensure consistent results for this cutoff, we performed three additional runs for 619,000,000 iterations (the upper limit of our current computational resources). While four rate parameters in these models still failed to reach target effective sample sizes of 1,000 during the extended runs, their effective sample sizes were nevertheless fairly high (range: 371–997). Furthermore, the plots of running values across iterations for BFs testing the cognitive buffer and colonization advantage hypotheses in these models indicate that these results are also highly stable (Supplementary Fig. 2). Posterior distributions of parameter estimates from the different chains produced for each threshold were subsequently pooled to calculate both the mean values and standard deviations for each transition rate (Supplementary Fig. 3).

Ancestral trait reconstruction

The ancestral states reported in Fig. 4 were reconstructed for visualization purposes only and estimated with the ‘phytools’66 package in R. Reconstructions of continuous trait data were based on maximum likelihood and a randomly chosen tree within our candidate set. Colour coding in Fig. 4bg is based on results from separate ancestral trait reconstructions for the different environmental variables.

Data availability

All data generated or analysed during this study are either available through cited sources or included in this published article and its Supplementary Information files.

Additional Information

Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  1. 1.

    Bennett, P. M. & Harvey, P. H. Relative brain size and ecology in birds. J. Zool. 207, 151–169 (1985).

  2. 2.

    Isler, K. & van Schaik, C. P. Metabolic costs of brain size evolution. Biol. Lett. 2, 557–560 (2006).

  3. 3.

    Iwaniuk, A. N. & Nelson, J. E. Developmental differences are correlated with relative brain size in birds: a comparative analysis. Can. J. Zool. 81, 1913–1928 (2003).

  4. 4.

    Barton, R. A. & Capellini, I. Maternal investment, life histories, and the costs of brain growth in mammals. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, 6169–6174 (2011).

  5. 5.

    Sol, D. in Cognitive Ecology II (eds Dukas, R. & Ratcliffe, J. M.) 111–134 (Univ. Chicago Press, Chicago, 2009).

  6. 6.

    Potts, R. Variability selection in hominid evolution. Evol. Anthropol. 7, 81–96 (1998).

  7. 7.

    Reader, S. M. & Laland, K. N. Social intelligence, innovation, and enhanced brain size in primates. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 99, 4436–4441 (2002).

  8. 8.

    Lefebvre, L. Brains, innovations, tools and cultural transmission in birds, non-human primates, and fossil hominins. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7, 245 (2013).

  9. 9.

    Sol, D., Székely, T., Liker, A. & Lefebvre, L. Big-brained birds survive better in nature. Proc. R. Soc. B 274, 763–769 (2007).

  10. 10.

    Maille, A. & Schradin, C. Survival is linked with reaction time and spatial memory in African striped mice. Biol. Lett. 12, 20160346 (2016).

  11. 11.

    Shultz, S., Bradbury, R. B., Evans, K. L., Gregory, R. D. & Blackburn, T. M. Brain size and resource specialization predict long-term population trends in British birds. Proc. R. Soc. B 272, 2305–2311 (2005).

  12. 12.

    Maklakov, A. A., Immler, S., Gonzalez-Voyer, A., Rönn, J. & Kolm, N. Brains and the city: big-brained passerine birds succeed in urban environments. Biol. Lett. 7, 730–732 (2011).

  13. 13.

    Vincze, O. Light enough to travel or wise enough to stay? Brain size evolution and migratory behavior in birds. Evolution 70, 2123–2133 (2016).

  14. 14.

    Sayol, F. et al. Environmental variation and the evolution of large brains in birds. Nat. Commun. 7, 13971 (2016).

  15. 15.

    Sol, D., Bacher, S., Reader, S. M., Lefebvre, L. & Price, S. E. T. D. Brain size predicts the success of mammal species introduced into novel environments. Am. Nat. 172, S63–S71 (2008).

  16. 16.

    Sol, D. et al. Unraveling the life history of successful invaders. Science 337, 580–583 (2012).

  17. 17.

    Amiel, J. J., Tingley, R. & Shine, R. Smart moves: effects of relative brain size on establishment success of invasive amphibians and reptiles. PLoS ONE 6, e18277 (2011).

  18. 18.

    Lefebvre, L. & Sol, D. Brains, lifestyles and cognition: are there general trends? Brain Behav. Evol. 72, 135–144 (2008).

  19. 19.

    Kotrschal, A., Corral-Lopez, A., Amcoff, M. & Kolm, N. A larger brain confers a benefit in a spatial mate search learning task in male guppies. Behav. Ecol. 26, 527–532 (2015).

  20. 20.

    Kotrschal, A. et al. Artificial selection on relative brain size in the guppy reveals costs and benefits of evolving a larger brain. Curr. Biol. 23, 168–171 (2013).

  21. 21.

    Lefebvre, L., Reader, S. M. & Sol, D. Brains, innovations and evolution in birds and primates. Brain Behav. Evol. 63, 233–246 (2004).

  22. 22.

    Sol, D., Lefebvre, L. & Rodríguez-Teijeiro, J. D. Brain size, innovative propensity and migratory behaviour in temperate Palaearctic birds. Proc. R. Soc. B 272, 1433–1441 (2005).

  23. 23.

    Sauer, J. R., Fallon, J. E. & Johnson, R. Use of North American Breeding Bird Survey data to estimate population change for bird conservation regions. J. Wildlife Manage. 67, 372–389 (2003).

  24. 24.

    Sauer, J. R. et al. The North American Breeding Bird Survey: Results and Analysis 1966–2015 Version 2.07.2017 (USGS Patuxent Wildlife Research Center, 2017); http://www.mbr-pwrc.usgs.gov/bbs/.

  25. 25.

    Smith, A. C., Hudson, M.-A. R., Downes, C. & Francis, C. M. Estimating breeding bird survey trends and annual indices for Canada: how do the new hierarchical Bayesian estimates differ from previous estimates? Can. Field Nat. 128, 119–134 (2014).

  26. 26.

    Clark, J. R. et al. North American Bird Conservation Initiative: Bird Conservation Region Descriptions, a Supplement to the North American Bird Conservation Initiative Bird Conservation Regions Map (US NABCI Committee, Washington DC, 2000).

  27. 27.

    Jetz, W., Thomas, G. H., Joy, J. B., Hartmann, K. & Mooers, A. O. The global diversity of birds in space and time. Nature 491, 444–448 (2012).

  28. 28.

    Colwell, R. K. Predictability, constancy, and contingency of periodic phenomena. Ecology 55, 1148–1153 (1974).

  29. 29.

    Botero, C. A., Dor, R., McCain, C. M. & Safran, R. J. Environmental harshness is positively correlated with intraspecific divergence in mammals and birds. Mol. Ecol. 23, 259–268 (2014).

  30. 30.

    Sheehan, M. J. et al. Different axes of environmental variation explain the presence vs. extent of cooperative nest founding associations in Polistes paper wasps. Ecol. Lett. 18, 1057–1067 (2015).

  31. 31.

    Bjørnstad, O. N. & Grenfell, B. T. Noisy clockwork: time series analysis of population fluctuations in animals. Science 293, 638–643 (2001).

  32. 32.

    Ricklefs, R. E. & Scheuerlein, A. Comparison of aging-related mortality among birds and mammals. Exp. Gerontol. 36, 845–857 (2001).

  33. 33.

    McNab, B. K. Food habits, energetics, and the population biology of mammals. Am. Nat. 116, 106–124 (1980).

  34. 34.

    Lindstedt, S. L. & Boyce, M. S. Seasonality, fasting endurance, and body size in mammals. Am. Nat. 125, 873–878 (1985).

  35. 35.

    Rubenstein, D. R. & Lovette, I. J. Temporal environmental variability drives the evolution of cooperative breeding in birds. Curr. Biol. 17, 1414–1419 (2007).

  36. 36.

    Devictor, V., Julliard, R. & Jiguet, F. Distribution of specialist and generalist species along spatial gradients of habitat disturbance and fragmentation. Oikos 117, 507–514 (2008).

  37. 37.

    Ives, A., Dennis, B., Cottingham, K. & Carpenter, S. Estimating community stability and ecological interactions from time-series data. Ecol. Monogr. 73, 301–330 (2003).

  38. 38.

    Dennis, B., Ponciano, J. M., Lele, S. R., Taper, M. L. & Staples, D. F. Estimating density dependence, process noise, and observation error. Ecol. Monogr. 76, 323–341 (2006).

  39. 39.

    Sauer, J. R. & Link, W. A. Analysis of the North American Breeding Bird Survey using hierarchical models. Auk 128, 87–98 (2011).

  40. 40.

    Brook, B. W. & Bradshaw, C. J. A. Strength of evidence for density dependence in abundance time series of 1198 species. Ecology 87, 1445–1451 (2006).

  41. 41.

    Ishida, Y. et al. Genetic connectivity across marginal habitats: the elephants of the Namib Desert. Ecol. Evol. 6, 6189–6201 (2016).

  42. 42.

    Pagel, M. Detecting correlated evolution on phylogenies: a general method for the comparative analysis of discrete characters. Proc. R. Soc. B 255, 37–45 (1994).

  43. 43.

    Green, D. M. The ecology of extinction: population fluctuation and decline in amphibians. Biol. Conserv. 111, 331–343 (2003).

  44. 44.

    Wells, J. C. K. & Stock, J. T. The biology of the colonizing ape. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 134, 191–222 (2007).

  45. 45.

    Roth, T. C., LaDage, L. D., Freas, C. A. & Pravosudov, V. V. Variation in memory and the hippocampus across populations from different climates: a common garden approach. Proc. R. Soc. B 279, 402–410 (2012).

  46. 46.

    Kozlovsky, D. Y., Branch, C. L. & Pravosudov, V. V. Problem-solving ability and response to novelty in mountain chickadees (Poecile gambeli) from different elevations. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 69, 635–643 (2015).

  47. 47.

    Benson-Amram, S., Dantzer, B., Stricker, G., Swanson, E. M. & Holekamp, K. E. Brain size predicts problem-solving ability in mammalian carnivores. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 113, 2532–2537 (2016).

  48. 48.

    Dunbar, R. I. M. & Shultz, S. Evolution in the social brain. Science 317, 1344–1347 (2007).

  49. 49.

    Emery, N. J., Seed, A. M., von Bayern, A. M. P. & Clayton, N. S. Cognitive adaptations of social bonding in birds. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 362, 489–505 (2007).

  50. 50.

    Garamszegi, L. Z., Møller, A. P. & Erritzøe, J. Coevolving avian eye size and brain size in relation to prey capture and nocturnality. Proc. R. Soc. B 269, 961–967 (2002).

  51. 51.

    Myhrvold, N. P. et al. An amniote life-history database to perform comparative analyses with birds, mammals, and reptiles. Ecology 96, 3109–3109 (2015).

  52. 52.

    Iwaniuk, A. N. & Nelson, J. E. Can endocranial volume be used as an estimate of brain size in birds? Can. J. Zool. 80, 16–23 (2002).

  53. 53.

    Sol, D. et al. Evolutionary divergence in brain size between migratory and resident birds. PLoS ONE 5, e9617 (2010).

  54. 54.

    Lima-Ribeiro, M. S. et al. EcoClimate: a database of climate data from multiple models for past, present, and future for macroecologists and biogeographers. Biodivers. Informatics 10, 1–21 (2015).

  55. 55.

    Osborne, J. Notes on the use of data transformations. Pract. Assess. Res. Eval. 8, 1–7 (2002).

  56. 56.

    Smith, A. C., Hudson, M.-A. R., Downes, C. M. & Francis, C. M. Change points in the population trends of aerial-insectivorous birds in North America: synchronized in time across species and regions. PLoS ONE 10, e0130768 (2015).

  57. 57.

    Plummer, M. rjags: Bayesian Graphical Models Using MCMC (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2013); https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/rjags/index.html.

  58. 58.

    Plummer, M., Best, N., Cowles, K. & Vines, K. CODA: convergence diagnosis and output analysis for MCMC. R News 6, 7–11 (2006).

  59. 59.

    Gelman, A. & Rubin, D. B. Inference from iterative simulation using multiple sequences. Stat. Sci. 7, 457–472 (1992).

  60. 60.

    Gaston, K. J. & McArdle, B. H. The temporal variability of animal abundances: measures, methods and patterns. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 345, 335–358 (1994).

  61. 61.

    Jetz, W. & Rubenstein, D. R. Environmental uncertainty and the global biogeography of cooperative breeding in birds. Curr. Biol. 21, 72–78 (2011).

  62. 62.

    Harmon, L. J., Weir, J. T., Brock, C. D., Glor, R. E. & Challenger, W. GEIGER: investigating evolutionary radiations. Bioinformatics 24, 129–131 (2008).

  63. 63.

    Pinheiro, J. et al. nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models. (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, 2016); https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlme.

  64. 64.

    R Development Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, 2008).

  65. 65.

    Orme, D. et al. The caper Package: Comparative Analysis of Phylogenetics and Evolution in R v0.5.2.. (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, 2013. http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/caper/index.html.

  66. 66.

    Maddison, W. P. & FitzJohn, R. G. The unsolved challenge to phylogenetic correlation tests for categorical characters. Syst. Biol. 64, 127–136 (2015).

  67. 67.

    Revell, L. J. phytools: an R package for phylogenetic comparative biology (and other things). Methods Ecol. Evol. 3, 217–223 (2012).

  68. 68.

    Pagel, M., Meade, A., Crespi, A. E. B. J. & Losos, E. J. B. Bayesian analysis of correlated evolution of discrete characters by reversible‐jump Markov chain Monte Carlo. Am. Nat. 167, 808–825 (2006).

  69. 69.

    Barbeitos, M. S., Romano, S. L. & Lasker, H. R. Repeated loss of coloniality and symbiosis in scleractinian corals. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 107, 11877–11882 (2010).

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank B. Carlson for invaluable feedback on an earlier draft of this manuscript. We are also grateful to the BBS and the countless volunteers that participate annually in this yearly survey. Bayesian analyses were run in the Washington University Center for High Performance Computing (CHPC), which is partially funded by NIH grants 1S10RR022984-01A1 and 1S10OD018091-01. We thank M. Tobias for his helpful advice on HPC.

Author information

Affiliations

  1. Department of Biology, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, 63130, USA

    • Trevor S. Fristoe
    •  & Carlos A. Botero
  2. Canadian Centre for Behavioural Neuroscience, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, Alberta T1K 6T5, Canada

    • Andrew N. Iwaniuk

Authors

  1. Search for Trevor S. Fristoe in:

  2. Search for Andrew N. Iwaniuk in:

  3. Search for Carlos A. Botero in:

Contributions

T.S.F. and C.A.B. designed analyses, compiled data and wrote the manuscript. T.S.F. additionally performed analyses and prepared figures. A.N.I. collected and compiled data, and contributed to writing.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Trevor S. Fristoe.

Electronic supplementary material

  1. Supplementary Information

    Supplementary Tables 1–3, Supplementary Figures 1–3

  2. Supplementary Data 1

    Data for 126 species used in population analyses. Variable descriptions can be found in the methods section of the manuscript.

  3. Supplementary Data 2

    Data for 2,062 species used in estimating relative brain sizes, including 1,288 species included in global evolutionary analyses. Variable descriptions can be found in the methods section of the manuscript.