Abstract
The precise neural underpinnings of face pareidolia in patients with Parkinson’s disease (PD) remain unclear. We aimed to clarify face recognition network abnormalities associated with face pareidolia in such patients. Eighty-three patients with PD and 40 healthy controls were recruited in this study. Patients with PD were classified into pareidolia and nonpareidolia groups. Volumetric analyses revealed no significant differences between the pareidolia (n = 39) and nonpareidolia (n = 44) patient groups. We further observed decreased functional connectivity among regions of interest in the bilateral frontotemporal lobes in patients with pareidolia. Seed-based analysis using bilateral temporal fusiform cortices as seeds revealed significantly decreased connectivity with the bilateral inferior medial prefrontal cortices in the pareidolia group. Post hoc regression analysis further demonstrated that the severity of face pareidolia was negatively correlated with functional connectivity between the bilateral temporal fusiform and medial prefrontal cortices. Our findings suggest that top-down modulation of the face recognition network is impaired in patients with PD experiencing face pareidolia.
Introduction
Parkinson’s disease (PD) is a progressive neurodegenerative disorder with an estimated prevalence of approximately 1% among adults over the age of 60 years1. In addition to the well-documented motor signs associated with PD, patients often exhibit nonmotor symptoms—either autonomic, cognitive, or neuropsychiatric2—even in the early stages of the disease3.
Characterized by the visual misperception of an ambiguous stimulus as a face, face pareidolia has a prevalence of as high as 30% among patients with PD: a higher rate than that observed among healthy controls4. Several studies have reported that patients with idiopathic rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder (iRBD) experience pareidolia more frequently than healthy individuals, and that there may be an association between pareidolia and cognitive decline5. In a study involving patients with dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB), face pareidolia was associated with the severity of visual hallucinations and visuospatial deficits6. These studies suggest a possible association between face pareidolia and Lewy body pathology; however, these studies were conducted under heterogeneous clinical conditions, and the precise mechanism of face pareidolia remains unclear.
The neural system for face recognition has been well documented and consists of several brain regions in the occipitotemporal cortex: the fusiform gyrus (fusiform face area: FFA), the inferior occipital cortex (occipital face area), and the posterior superior temporal sulcus. Among these areas, the FFA is considered crucial for face perception and identification7,8,9,10. In addition to these networks, which are mainly involved in “bottom-up” sensory integration for face perception, recent studies have suggested the importance of “top-down” modulations from the frontal region for face perception11,12,13. Previous studies that investigated the neural mechanisms of face pareidolia have revealed that the abovementioned face recognition network is involved in the pareidolic reaction in healthy participants. Using the illusory face perception paradigm with pure-noise visual stimuli, Liu et al. described a distributed network including the frontal, parietal, temporal, and occipital cortices in those with face pareidolia14. Other studies with similar paradigms have also suggested an association between FFA activation and face pareidolia;15,16 however, substantial interindividual variability has also been suggested17. These findings imply a possible association between dysfunction of the face recognition network and pareidolia in PD.
Recently, noninvasive imaging techniques such as resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rsfMRI) have enabled us to investigate the status of brain networks in patients with PD. Previous studies have suggested the early alteration of wide-spread brain networks in PD18,19, as well as its association with motor and nonmotor symptoms20,21,22. Given that brain network alterations can occur in the early stages of PD prior to the development of cognitive impairment, alterations in the face recognition network may be associated with face pareidolia in patients with PD.
It is possible that both “top-down” modulation and “bottom-up” sensory integration streams to the FFA play a vital role in the development of face pareidolia in patients with PD. Informed by our previous electroencephalogram findings that suggest the involvement of “top-down” attentional modulation from the frontal cortices23, we hypothesized that dysfunction of “top-down” modulation associated with the FFA is involved in face pareidolia. Using rsfMRI, the present study aimed to uncover the network abnormalities associated with pareidolia in patients with early-stage PD without dementia or hallucinations.
Results
Clinical characteristics
Among the 97 selected patients with PD, 14 were excluded from further analysis due to poor quality of the imaging data (e.g., excessive head movement). No participants in the healthy control (HC) group exhibited excessive head motion. Ultimately, 83 patients with PD (34 men and 48 women; age: 67.2 ± 9.8 years; Hoehn and Yahr (HY) stage:24 2.6 ± 0.9); and 40 HCs (18 men and 22 women; age: 66.0 ± 3.7 years) were included in the analysis. Based on the Noise Pareidolia Test (NPT) screening, 39 patients with PD (47.6%) were classified into the PD with pareidolia (PD-p + ) group, while 44 patients (52.4%) were classified into the PD without pareidolia (PD-p-) group. The only characteristic that differed significantly between the PD groups was the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score25 (27.0 ± 2.2 vs. 28.8 ± 1.7, p < 0.01) (Table 1).
Gray matter volume analysis
No significant differences in gray matter volume were observed between the PD-p+ and PD-p− groups. Compared with the HC group, the PD-p+ group exhibited reduced gray matter volume in the left temporal pole, left central operculum, and brainstem, while the PD-p− group exhibited reduced gray matter volume in the left central operculum and brainstem.
Functional connectivity analysis
In the region of interest (ROI)-based analysis, both the PD-p+ and PD-p− groups exhibited substantial differences in functional connectivity throughout the brain when compared to the HC group. The comparison between the two PD groups revealed that the PD-p+ group exhibited decreased functional connectivity in the networks of multiple regions in the frontal and temporal lobes, relative to that observed in the PD-p− group. The most significant differences in functional connectivity were observed in the network between the medial frontal cortices (MedFC) and the left temporal fusiform cortex (TFusC) (Fig. 1).
The decrease in connectivity was statistically significant between PD-p + and PD-p- groups (PD-p + < PD-p−: controlled for age, MMSE). The T values of the negative difference in functional connectivity are shown as a colored bar. Patients in the pareidolia group exhibited decreased functional connectivity between the medial prefrontal cortices and the bilateral temporal lobe (including the left temporal fusiform cortices, bilateral parahippocampal cortices, and right inferior temporal gyri) when compared with patients in the nonpareidolia group. Abbreviations: PD, Parkinson’s disease; MMSE Mini-Mental State Examination, r right, l left, FP frontal pole, MedFC frontal medial cortex, a/pPaHC parahippocampal gyrus anterior/posterior division, pITG inferior temporal gyrus, posterior division, a/pMTG middle temporal gyrus, anterior/posterior division, a/pTFusC temporal fusiform cortex anterior/posterior division, TOFusC temporal occipital fusiform cortex, TP temporal pole, aSTG superior temporal gyrus anterior.
Consistent with the ROI-to-ROI (ROI-based) analysis, in the seed-based analysis using the bilateral TFusC as a seed, the bilateral inferior MedFC exhibited significantly decreased connectivity in the PD-p+ group, when compared with that in the PD-p− group (Fig. 2). Subsequently, we performed a group comparison of Z-scores for functional connectivity values (Fisher’s transformation of the correlation coefficient) between the TFusC and the bilateral inferior MedFC. The mean Z-scores [95% confidence interval (CI)] of each group were as follows, PD-p + : 0.009 [−0.025 to 0.043], PD-p-: 0.080 [0.037 to 0.123], and HC group: 0.110 [0.067 to 0.153]. Groupwise comparison using analysis of variance (ANOVA) revealed a significant difference among the three groups (F(2120) = 6.42; p = 0.002). Post hoc analysis showed that the PD-p + group exhibited significantly decreased connectivity compared with those of the PD-p− and HC groups, while there was no difference in connectivity between the PD-p− and HC groups (PD-p + vs. PD-p- group: FDR-p < 0.05; 95% CI [−0.138 to −0.004], effect size, d = −0.57; PD-p + vs. HC group: FDR-p < 0.05; 95%CI [−0.169 to −0.031], effect size, d = −0.84: PD-p- vs. HC group: FDR-p = 0.84; 95%CI [−0.096 to 0.037], effect size, d = −0.21) (Fig. 3a). Additional post hoc analysis showed no significant effect of symptom lateralization on functional connectivity between the TFusC and the MedFC (t(80.435) = 1.59, 95% CI [−0.012 to 0.108], p = 0.11). Functional connectivity Z-scores were also significantly negatively correlated with NPT scores in the PD-p+ group (r = −0.40, p = 0.01) (Fig. 3b).
Spatial maps for seed-based connectivity analyses with the bilateral temporal fusiform cortices as the seed regions (PD-p + < PD-p−: controlled for age, MMSE). The inferior medial prefrontal cortices exhibited significantly decreased connectivity with the bilateral temporal fusiform cortices. FDR-p false discovery rate-corrected p value.
FC shows Z-scores of the correlation coefficient between the medial frontal cortex and the bilateral fusiform cortices. a (left): Dot-plot shows comparison of FC among the three groups. The mean of each group with 95% confidence intervals are shown as red rhombi with error bar. * FDR-p < 0.05. (ANOVA with FDR adjusted post hoc Tukey–Kramer test). b (right): Correlation between frontotemporal connectivity and pareidolia score. Scatter plot shows Pearson’s correlation between FC and pareidolia scores in patients with pareidolia. FC functional connectivity, ANOVA analysis of variance, FDR false discovery rate.
Regression analysis using functional connectivity values for the pareidolia score revealed a significant negative correlation with functional connectivity in the network including the MedFC, left TFusC, right parahippocampal gyrus, anterior division, and right temporal occipital fusiform cortex (Fig. 4a). In contrast, regression analysis for MMSE scores revealed a significant negative correlation with connectivity in the different networks around the bilateral hippocampus and middle temporal gyrus (Fig. 4b).
a (left): Regression analysis of functional connectivity revealing pareidolia score-dependent decrement. Functional connectivity between the MedFC and pTFusC was negatively correlated with the pareidolia score. b (right): Regression analysis of functional connectivity revealing the MMSE score-dependent decrement. Functional connectivity between the bilateral hippocampus and middle temporal gyrus was negatively correlated with MMSE score. T values of the negative difference in functional connectivity are shown as a colored bar. The statistical significance was set to an FDR-p < 0.05. MMSE Mini-Mental State Examination, r right, l left, MedFC medial frontal cortices, pTFusC temporal fusiform cortex posterior division, TOFusC temporal occipital fusiform cortex, aSTG superior temporal gyrus anterior, FP frontal pole, pSMG supramarginal gyrus posterior division, toMTG middle temporal gyrus temporooccipital part, aPaHC right parahippocampal gyrus anterior division, pPaHC parahippocampal gyrus posterior division.
Discussion
The present study aimed to investigate network abnormalities associated with pareidolia in patients with early-stage PD, prior to the development of dementia or hallucinations. Our findings revealed decreased connectivity between the medial prefrontal cortices and the medial temporal area, including the FFA, in patients with PD experiencing pareidolia. Moreover, the severity of face pareidolia was negatively correlated with functional connectivity between the bilateral temporal fusiform and medial prefrontal cortices.
Although the “bottom-up” information stream to the FFA is emphasized in the classical model of face recognition, recent studies have revealed the importance of “top-down” modulations from frontal regions to the FFA11,12,23. A previous study revealed that when humans determine whether the visual objects are faces26, their attention focuses on either a face or the scenery in front of them, which alters the neural activity of the FFA and parahippocampal place area, respectively27, as well as the preceding neural responses in the medial prefrontal cortices. Zhang et al. also demonstrated an association between activation of the medial prefrontal cortices and pareidolic reactions in pure-noise images during face detection tasks28. Interestingly, we observed significantly decreased connectivity between the medial frontal region and the fusiform cortices in the PD-p+ group but not in the HC or PD-p− group. We also identified a significant negative correlation between this functional connectivity and pareidolic reactions. These findings suggest the importance of regulatory control from the medial prefrontal region to the FFA in the pathophysiology of face pareidolia.
The idea that loss of attentional control is associated with face pareidolia is consistent with clinical observations that face pareidolia and visual hallucinations are associated with global cognitive decline, which results in attention deficits29,30. Previous studies have highlighted the potential involvement of visual attention-related functional networks including the default mode network, ventral attention network, and dorsal attention network in visual misperception31,32. These findings suggest that both the functional connection between the medial frontal region and the fusiform cortices and the involvement of wide-spread global networks are important in the pathophysiology of face pareidolia. However, our findings support the notion that disrupted connectivity between the medial prefrontal cortices and the FFA can specifically lead to pareidolia in patients with PD. Indeed, our study excluded patients with hallucinations, and our post hoc regression analysis revealed that distinct networks were associated with pareidolia and cognitive status.
Previous studies have also emphasized the involvement of classical “bottom-up” visual streaming from the occipital cortex in face pareidolia. A positron emission tomography study reported that occipital hypoperfusion was associated with pareidolia29. Another rsfMRI study reported a visual hallucination-associated reduction in connectivity between the occipital and cortical-striatal regions in patients with PD33. Neuropsychological studies have also revealed impaired visuoperceptual functions, including line orientation in PD populations34, and longitudinal structural MRI data have suggested the involvement of the occipital, parietal, and temporal areas in visuoperceptual dysfunctions in patients with PD35. However, our study found no significant difference in functional connectivity between the occipital regions and FFA, and both groups exhibited comparable visual perceptual ability as assessed using the JLO. Moreover, our voxel-based morphometry findings indicated that gray matter volumes were comparable between the PD-p+ and PD-p− groups. Taken together, these results indicate that both “top-down” and “bottom-up” processing streams may be critical for face pareidolia. Therefore, the pathophysiology of pareidolia in patients with PD may be heterogeneous.
Our results showed no significant effect of symptom lateralization on functional network alteration. This might suggest the left dominant involvement of this network on the pathophysiology of face pareidolia. Considering the hemispheric lateralization in the face recognition system, our ROI-based analysis revealed left-dominant decreased connectivity between the MedFC and the TFusC, although previous clinical observations, as well as neuropsychological and neuroimaging studies have emphasized the dominance of the right hemisphere in the face recognition system36. Recent reports have suggested functional differences in face perception between the two hemispheres. Meng et al. reported that the left FFA is involved in detecting “low-level” face resemblance, whereas the right FFA is involved in the more precise judgment of faces37. Additionally, a recent functional imaging study using an illusory face paradigm in healthy participants revealed that the right FFA has high selectivity for faces, but that the left FFA also responds to nonface objects, suggesting low-selectivity for faces but nonspecific object reactivity14,38. Considering that pareidolic phenomena are not specific to faces and may result in the illusory misperception of other types of objects in patients with PD6,29, frontal dysregulation to the left FFA is likely involved in the pareidolic reactions experienced by patients with PD.
There were several limitations to this study. First, although we excluded patients with PD experiencing dementia and the MMSE scores in the PD-p+ and PD-p− groups were within the normal range, there was a significant difference in MMSE score between the groups. In this study, to control for the effect of cognitive deficits, we included the MMSE score into the model as a possible confounding factor. As stated above, our post hoc regression analysis revealed an independent functional network associated with severe pareidolia and cognitive status. However, we cannot exclude the possible influence of subcortical-frontal dysfunctions, which is usually underestimated by MMSE. Second, we only investigated resting-state functional connectivity, which did not allow us to analyze brain activity during the emergence of pareidolia. Pareidolia is a form of visual misperception that occurs in a visual stimulus-dependent manner6,29. Therefore, it is possible that our findings did not reflect neural networks that are directly correlated with the pareidolic reaction. It should be noted that our findings revealed a significant correlation between the frontotemporal network and face pareidolia; however, this correlation does not necessarily imply causality. Studies using activation paradigms with sufficient spatial and temporal resolution are required to investigate specific network dysfunctions that can cause pareidolia. Third, as our study included only patients without dementia or hallucinations, the underlying pathophysiology of pareidolia may be different from that in patients with cognitive deficits or hallucinations. Further studies that include patients with a broader range of disease severity or longitudinal data will help to clarify this point. Fourth, based on our hypothesis, we only analyzed the cortical network in this study. However, not only lateral geniculate to cortical pathways, but the retinotectal pathway may also be involved in face recognition39, and it has been shown that in PD patients, there is loss of dopaminergic retinal cells and impaired contrast sensitivity in PD40. Therefore, it is possible that these subcortical structures may play some roles in face pareidolia in PD. Finally, we conducted this study with patients in the “ON” state for dopaminergic medications. Although there was no significant difference in levodopa equivalent doses between the PD-p+ and PD-p− groups in our study, we could not fully exclude possible drug effects on either pareidolia or functional connectivity.
In conclusion, our findings emphasized the importance of alterations in the frontotemporal network in face pareidolia among patients with PD using rsfMRI. In accordance with the findings of experimental studies conducted with healthy participants14,28,37,38, our findings also suggest the possible involvement of “top-down” modulation in the pathophysiology of face pareidolia in PD. Although further replication studies are needed, our findings provide insight into the pathophysiology of frequent visual misperception in patients with PD. Future studies focusing on the frontotemporal network may help confirm whether these findings indicate a common underlying pathophysiology of face pareidolia in patients with Lewy body pathology, including PD, DLB, or iRBD. Our findings may also help identify the role of the visuoperceptual networks in the development of visual hallucinations in patients with Lewy body pathology, which may in turn lead to the identification of a potential target for neuromodulative interventions like deep brain stimulation.
Methods
Participants
This study was conducted as part of a longitudinal prospective cohort and registry study of PD at Osaka University that registered all patients with PD who were admitted to our hospital. From the patients registered between September 2014 and November 2016, we selected those who complied with the following selection criteria: (1) aged between 40 and 85 years, (2) diagnosed with PD in accordance with the United Kingdom Parkinson’s Disease Society Brain Bank criteria41, (3) HY stage from 1 to 3, and (4) no clinical symptoms of dementia. Exclusion criteria were as follows: (1) a history of other neurological, psychiatric, or severe ocular disease; (2) the presence of subjective hallucinations based on interviews with the patient and caregiver; (3) current use of antipsychotic medications; and (4) a score of less than 24 on MMSE. According to these criteria, 97 patients with PD were selected from among 213 patients who were registered during this period. In addition, we selected 40 age- and sex-matched healthy individuals without any history of neurological or psychiatric diseases from another cohort study42, as HCs for rsfMRI connectivity analysis.
Ethics
This study was approved by the Osaka University Hospital ethics review committee and the Osaka University Research Ethics Committee. Written informed consent was obtained in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and a written agreement was obtained from all participants.
General/baseline evaluations
Clinical symptoms of PD were assessed using the Movement Disorder Society‐sponsored revision of the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (MDS‐UPDRS) parts I to III and the HY stage43, while the patients were in the “ON” state. Cognitive function was assessed using the MMSE, Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB)44, the Japanese version of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA)45, and the short form of the Benton Judgement of Line Orientation Test (JLO), form H46,47. Furthermore, we assessed symptom laterality. Dopaminergic medications for PD were converted to levodopa equivalent daily doses48.
Evaluation for pareidolia
To evaluate hallucinatory phenomena in the patient group, we used the noise pareidolia test (NPT), which is designed to evoke and measure pareidolia49. In the NPT group, participants were required to determine whether a face was present in each stimulus. The perception of a face in nonface stimulus was considered a pareidolic reaction, with the total number of pareidolic reactions was recorded as the pareidolia score. When a participant did not perceive a face in a face-containing stimulus, the result was considered a “miss”. Those who experienced at least one pareidolic response were included in the PD-p+ group, while the rest were included in the PD-p− group.
Acquisition and processing of MR images
All images were acquired using a 3-T MR scanner (GE Healthcare, Milwaukee, WI, USA). High-resolution anatomical scans were obtained using a sagittal three-dimensional (3D) fast-spoiled gradient-recalled echo-pulse sequence, and gradient-echo echo-planar imaging was used for rsfMRI. The parameters were the same as those in our former study50.
Quality check for MRI scans
We used a foam pillow to avoid excessive head motion. Participants with any artifacts in their MR scans or with excessive head movement creating more than 1 mm of displacement or 2.5° of rotation in any direction during rsfMRI were excluded from further analysis.
Imaging acquisition and preprocessing
Gray matter volume analysis was performed with a voxel-based morphometry technique using Statistical Parametric Mapping 12 (SPM12; http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/) on Matlab R2018a (MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA), as follows: segmentation into cerebrospinal fluid, gray matter, and white matter based on tissue probability maps; spatial normalization of gray matter segments to a gray matter template in the Montreal Neurological Institute space using diffeomorphic anatomical registration through exponentiated lie algebra techniques; and a 3D Gaussian filter of 8 mm full-width at half-maximum for smoothing of images51,52.
Functional connectivity data were preprocessed using the CONN-fMRI functional connectivity toolbox v17 (http://www.nitrc.org/projects/conn)53. All T1-weighted structural MR images and rsfMRI data were preprocessed as follows: spatial realignment; slice timing correction; normalization using transformation parameters derived from a high-resolution anatomical scan; and smoothing using a 3D Gaussian filter of 8 mm full-width at half-maximum. All resting-state time series were de-noised and band-pass filtered (0.009–0.08 Hz) for blood-oxygen-level-dependent signals.
Statistical analysis and analysis of MR images
The Mann–Whitney U test and Chi-squared test were used for group comparisons of clinical data, while analyses of covariance including possible confounders were used to analyze brain images. The analyses were performed using R version 3.6 (https://www.r-project.org/), and p values < 0.05 (two-sided) were considered statistically significant.
To analyze gray matter volume in the PD-p + , PD-p−, and HC groups, we performed analyses of covariance using age and total brain volume as covariates of no interest to minimize the effects of aging and the brain size of each participant. The threshold for statistical significance was set to an uncorrected p < 0.001 at the initial voxel level and a false discovery rate-corrected p (FDR-p) < 0.05 (two-sided) at the cluster level.
Functional connectivity was assessed using mean resting-state time series extracted from supratentorial regions of interest (ROIs) from the Harvard-Oxford cortical and subcortical structural atlases54,55,56,57 Functional connectivity value was calculated by using Pearson’s coefficient of correlation followed by Fisher’s transformation58. To analyze functional connectivity between PD-p+ and PD-p−, analysis of covariance was performed with age and cognitive status (MMSE score) as covariates. The threshold for statistical significance was set to an FDR-p < 0.05 (two-sided).
Next, based on our hypothesis that altered attentional modulation to the FFA may cause pareidolia, we carried out ROI-to-voxel (seed-based) connectivity analysis using bilateral TFusC (including the FFA) as a seed. For the seed-based connectivity analysis between the PD-p+ and PD-p− groups, we performed an analysis of covariance using age and cognitive status (MMSE score) as covariates. The thresholds for statistical significance were set to an uncorrected p < 0.001 at the initial voxel level and an FDR-p < 0.05 (two-sided) at the cluster level.
To investigate the relationship between the severity of pareidolia and impaired functional connectivity, we performed Pearson’s correlation analysis between the pareidolia score and Z-score for functional connectivity of clusters detected in the seed-based analysis with the threshold for statistical significance set to p < 0.05. We also performed ANOVA for seed-based functional connectivity among the PD-p + , PD-p−, and HC groups, with post hoc Tukey–Kramer test. For multiple comparison, we used FDR adjustment with the statistical threshold of p < 0.05 (two-sided). We also calculated Cohen’s d as a measure of the effect size of the intergroup difference in functional connectivity. As our results showed left a dominant decrease in frontotemporal functional connectivity in the PD-p+ group, we performed ad hoc analysis comparing this connectivity between right- and left-dominant patients to investigate the effect of symptom laterality on the network alteration related with pareidolia.
In addition, since there were significant differences in cognitive status (MMSE scores) between the PD-p+ and PD-p− groups, we additionally conducted a post hoc analysis to distinguish the network associated with pareidolia from those associated with cognitive status. Using rsfMRI data, we performed a regression functional connectivity analysis for each pareidolia score and MMSE score. The threshold for statistical significance was set to an FDR-p < 0.05 (two-sided).
Reporting Summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Data availability
The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
References
Ascherio, A. & Schwarzschild, M. A. The epidemiology of Parkinson’s disease: risk factors and prevention. Lancet Neurol. 15, 1257–1272 (2016).
Chaudhuri, K. R., Healy, D. G., Schapira, A. H. & Anthony, H. V. S. Non-motor symptoms of Parkinson’s disease: diagnosis and management. Lancet Neurol. 5, 235–245 (2006).
Getz, S. J. & Levin, B. Cognitive and neuropsychiatric features of early Parkinson’s disease. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 32, 769–785 (2017).
Nishio, Y. et al. Deconstructing psychosis and misperception symptoms in Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 88, 722–729 (2017).
Sasai-Sakuma, T., Nishio, Y., Yokoi, K., Mori, E. & Inoue, Y. Pareidolias in REM sleep behavior disorder: a possible predictive marker of Lewy body dseases? Sleep. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/zsw045 (2017).
Uchiyama, M. et al. Pareidolias: complex visual illusions in dementia with Lewy bodies. Brain 135, 2458–2469 (2012).
Puce, A., Allison, T., Asgari, M., Gore, J. C. & McCarthy, G. Differential sensitivity of human visual cortex to faces, letterstrings, and textures: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Neurosci. 16, 5205–5215 (1996).
Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J. & Chun, M. M. The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. J. Neurosci. 17, 4302–4311 (1997).
Grill-Spector, K. The neural basis of object perception. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 13, 159–166 (2003).
Haxby, J. V., Hoffman, E. A. & Gobbini, M. I. The distributed human neural system for face perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 223–233 (2000).
Ullman, S. Sequence seeking and counter streams: a computational model for bidirectional information flow in the visual cortex. Cereb. Cortex. 5, 1–11 (1995).
Deco, G. & Rolls, E. T. A neurodynamical cortical model of visual attention and invariant object recognition. Vis. Res. 44, 621–642 (2004).
Ishai, A. Let’s face it: it’s a cortical network. Neuroimage 40, 415–419 (2008).
Liu, J. et al. Seeing Jesus in toast: neural and behavioral correlates of face pareidolia. Cortex 53, 60–77 (2014).
Palermo, R. & Rhodes, G. Are you always on my mind? A review of how face perception and attention interact. Neuropsychologia 45, 75–92 (2007).
Hadjikhani, N., Kveraga, K., Naik, P. & Ahlfors, S. P. Early (N170) activation of face-specific cortex by face-like objects. Neuroreport 20, 403–407 (2009).
Zimmermann, K. M., Stratil, A. S., Thome, I., Sommer, J. & Jansen, A. Illusory face detection in pure noise images: The role of interindividual variability in fMRI activation patterns. PLoS ONE 14, e0209310 (2019).
Hou, Y. et al. Resting-state network connectivity in cognitively unimpaired drug-naïve patients with rigidity-dominant Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol. Sci. 395, 147–152 (2018).
Kim, J. et al. Abnormal intrinsic brain functional network dynamics in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 140, 2955–2967 (2017).
Wang, H. et al. Altered resting-state voxel-level whole-brain functional connectivity in depressed Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 50, 74–80 (2018).
Wen, M. C. et al. Neural substrates of excessive daytime sleepiness in early drug naïve Parkinson’s disease: A resting state functional MRI study. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 24, 63–68 (2016).
Filippi, M. et al. Longitudinal brain connectivity changes and clinical evolution in Parkinson’s disease. Mol. Psychiatry. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-020-0770-0. (2020).
Revankar, G. S. et al. Ocular fixations and presaccadic potentials to explain pareidolias in Parkinson’s disease. Brain Commun. 2, fcaa073 (2020).
Goetz, C. G. et al. Movement Disorder Society Task Force report on the Hoehn and Yahr staging scale: status and recommendations. Mov. Disord. 19, 1020–1028 (2004).
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E. & McHugh, P. R. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198 (1975).
Summerfield, C. et al. Predictive codes for forthcoming perception in the frontal cortex. Science 314, 1311–1314 (2006).
Gazzaley, A., Cooney, J. W., McEvoy, K., Knight, R. T. & D’Esposito, M. Top-down enhancement and suppression of the magnitude and speed of neural activity. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 507–517 (2005).
Zhang, H. et al. Detecting faces in pure noise images: a functional MRI study on top-down perception. Neuroreport 19, 229–233 (2008).
Uchiyama, M. et al. Pareidolia in Parkinson’s disease without dementia: A positron emission tomography study. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 21, 603–609 (2015).
Gama, R. L. et al. Risk factors for visual hallucinations in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Neurol. Res. 37, 112–116 (2015).
Shine, J. M., Halliday, G. M., Naismith, S. L. & Lewis, S. J. Visual misperceptions and hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease: dysfunction of attentional control networks? Mov. Disord. 26, 2154–2159 (2011).
Shine, J. M. et al. Abnormal connectivity between the default mode and the visual system underlies the manifestation of visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease: a task-based fMRI study. NPJ Parkinsons. Dis. 1, 15003 (2015).
Yao, N. et al. Resting activity in visual and corticostriatal pathways in Parkinson’s disease with hallucinations. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 21, 131–137 (2015).
Montse, A., Pere, V., Carme, J., Francesc, V. & Eduardo, T. Visuospatial deficits in Parkinson’s disease assessed by judgment of line orientation test: error analyses and practice effects. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 23, 592–598 (2001).
Garcia-Diaz, A. I. et al. Cortical thinning correlates of changes in visuospatial and visuoperceptual performance in Parkinson’s disease: A 4-year follow-up. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 46, 62–68 (2018).
Frassle, S. et al. Mechanisms of hemispheric lateralization: Asymmetric interhemispheric recruitment in the face perception network. Neuroimage 124, 977–988 (2016).
Meng, M., Cherian, T., Singal, G. & Sinha, P. Lateralization of face processing in the human brain. Proc. Biol. Sci. 279, 2052–2061 (2012).
Nestor, A., Vettel, J. M. & Tarr, M. J. Internal representations for face detection: an application of noise-based image classification to BOLD responses. Hum. Brain. Mapp. 34, 3101–3115 (2013).
Nakano, T., Higashida, N. & Kitazawa, S. Facilitation of face recognition through the retino-tectal pathway. Neuropsychologia 51, 2043–2049 (2013).
Ortuño-Lizarán, I. et al. Dopaminergic retinal cell loss and visual dysfunction in Parkinson disease. Ann. Neurol. 88, 893–906 (2020).
Hughes, A. J., Daniel, S. E., Kilford, L. & Lees, A. J. Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson’s disease: a clinico-pathological study of 100 cases. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 55, 181–184 (1992).
Koshiyama, D., et al. Neuroimaging studies within Cognitive Genetics Collaborative Research Organization aiming to replicate and extend works of ENIGMA. Hum Brain Mapp. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25040 (2020).
Goetz, C. G. et al. Movement Disorder Society-sponsored revision of the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (MDS-UPDRS): scale presentation and clinimetric testing results. Mov. Disord. 23, 2129–2170 (2008).
Dubois, B., Slachevsky, A., Litvan, I. & Pillon, B. The FAB: a Frontal Assessment Battery at bedside. Neurology 55, 1621–1626 (2000).
Nasreddine, Z. S. et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 53, 695–699 (2005).
Benton, A. L., Varney, N. R. & Hamsher, K. D. Visuospatial judgment. A clinical test. Arch. Neurol. 35, 364–367 (1978).
Venderploeg, R. D., LaLone, L. V., Greblo, P. & Schinka, J. A. Odd-even short forms of the Judgment of Line Orientation Test. Appl. Neuropsychol. 4, 244–246 (1997).
Tomlinson, C. L. et al. Systematic review of levodopa dose equivalency reporting in Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 25, 2649–2653 (2010).
Yokoi, K. et al. Hallucinators find meaning in noises: pareidolic illusions in dementia with Lewy bodies. Neuropsychologia 56, 245–254 (2014).
Otomune, H. et al. Involvement of cortical dysfunction in frequent falls in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat. Dirord. 64, 169–174 (2019).
Good, C. D. et al. A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. Neuroimage 14, 21–36 (2001).
Ashburner, J. A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage 38, 95–113 (2007).
Whitfield-Gabrieli, S. & Nieto-Castanon, A. Conn: a functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks. Brain Connect. 2, 125–141 (2012).
Makris, N. et al. Decreased volume of left and total anterior insular lobule in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 83, 155–171 (2006).
Frazier, J. A. et al. Structural brain magnetic resonance imaging of limbic and thalamic volumes in pediatric bipolar disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 162, 1256–1265 (2005).
Desikan, R. S. et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage 31, 968–980 (2006).
Goldstein, J. M. et al. Hypothalamic abnormalities in schizophrenia: sex effects and genetic vulnerability. Biol. Psychiatry 61, 935–945 (2007).
Keller, C. J. et al. Intrinsic functional architecture predicts electrically evoked responses in the human brain. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 108, 10308–10313 (2011).
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank Editage (www.editage.com) for English language editing. This research was funded by Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research C (16K01453) from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS), Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research B (20H04044) from JSPS and The Brain Mapping by Integrated Neuroethologies for Disease Studies (JP21dm0207070) from the Japan Agency for Medical Research and Development (AMED).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
Y.K. Clinical and imaging data collection, clinical and imaging data analysis, interpretation of data, and writing of the initial draft of the manuscript. N.H. Imaging data analysis, interpretation of data, writing of the initial draft of the manuscript, and revision of the manuscript. T.N. Clinical data collection, clinical data analysis, and revision of the manuscript. G.S.R., Clinical data analysis, interpretation of data, and revision of the manuscript. H.O. Clinical data collection, clinical data analysis, and revision of the manuscript. R.H. Imaging data collection and revision of the manuscript. E.M. Critical review for design of the work, Interpretation of data, and revision of the manuscript. M.I. Critical review of the study design, Interpretation of data, and revision of the manuscript. M.M. Conception and design of the work, Clinical data collection, interpretation of data, writing of the initial draft of the manuscript, and revision of the manuscript. H.M. Conception of the work, Interpretation of data, and revision of the manuscript.
Corresponding authors
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
Y.K. was supported by JSPS. N.H. reported honoraria from INSIGHTEC Japan K.K. and was supported by the Endowed Research Department of Clinical Neuroengineering and grants from AMED Translational Research Program; Strategic Promotion for practical application of Innovative Medical Technology (TR-SPRINT). T.N. has nothing to disclose. G.R. was supported by the Interdisciplinary Program for Biomedical Sciences, Program for Leading Graduate School, Osaka University Grants-in-Aid for Education and Research, Japan. H.O. has nothing to disclose. R. H. reported honoraria from Dainippon Sumitomo Pharma Co., Ltd. and Mochida Pharmaceutical Co., Ltd., and was supported by grants from Takeda Pharmaceutical Company Limited, Otsuka Pharmaceutical Co., Ltd., and Japan Tobacco, Inc. E.M. reported grants and personal fees from Heptares, grants and personal fees from Mentis Cura, grants and personal fees from Sumitomo Dainippon Pharma, grants and personal fees from Eisai, and personal fees from FP Pharmaceutical, outside the submitted work. M.I. made efforts for the advisory boards of Eisai Co., Ltd., Eli Lilly Japan, and Chugai Pharmaceutical Co., Ltd., reported honoraria from Eisai Co., Ltd., Daiichi Sankyo Co., Ltd., Dainippon Sumitomo Pharma Co., Ltd., Novartis, Fujifilm RI Pharma Co., Ltd., Nihon Medi-Physics Co., Ltd., Takeda Pharmaceutical Company Limited, MSD K.K., a subsidiary of Merck & Co., Inc., Otsuka Pharmaceutical Co., Ltd., Janssen Pharmaceutical K.K., and was supported by grants from Eisai Co., Ltd., Dainippon Sumitomo Pharma Co., Ltd., and Otsuka Pharmaceutical Co., Ltd. M.M. was supported by grants from AMED, the JSPS, Japan Blood Products Organization, Kyowa Kirin Co., Ltd., Daiichi Sankyo, Sumitomo Dainippon Pharma, Shimadzu Corp., and honoraria were obtained from Otsuka Pharmaceutical Co., Ltd., Novartis, Chugai Pharmaceutical Co., Ltd., Sanofi, Takeda Pharmaceutical Co., Ltd., Ono Pharmaceutical Co., Ltd., Sumitomo Dainippon Pharma, and Shimadzu Corp. H.M. reported honoraria from Kyowa Kirin Co., Ltd., Sumitomo Dainippon Pharma, Mitsubishi Tanabe Pharma Corporation, Takeda Pharmaceutical Co., Ltd., Ono Pharmaceutical Co., Ltd., Eisai Co., Ltd., IQVIA Solutions Japan K.K., AbbVie Inc., INSIGHTEC Japan K.K.; made efforts for the advisory boards of Kissei Pharmaceutical Co., Ltd., Kyowa Kirin Co., Ltd., T-PEC corporation, FP Pharmaceutical corporation, Ono Pharmaceutical Co., Ltd., and was supported by grants from AMED, grants from Health and Labour Sciences Research Grants, grants from Strategic Basic Research Programs: Core Research for Evolutional Science and Technology, Jansen Pharmaceutical K.K., Bayer Yakuhin Ltd., Kyowa Kirin Co., Ltd., Teijin Pharma Limited, Daiichi Sankyo Company, Limited, Eisai Co., Ltd., Astellas Pharma Inc., Shionogi Pharma Co., Ltd., Sumitomo Dainippon Pharma, Nippon Boehringer Ingelheim Co., Ltd., and Takeda Pharmaceutical Co., Ltd.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Kajiyama, Y., Hattori, N., Nakano, T. et al. Decreased frontotemporal connectivity in patients with parkinson’s disease experiencing face pareidolia. npj Parkinsons Dis. 7, 90 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41531-021-00237-z
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41531-021-00237-z