Abstract
Public acceptance is critical for sharing of genomic data at scale. This paper examines how acceptance of data sharing pertains to the perceived similarities and differences between DNA and other forms of personal data. It explores the perceptions of representative publics from the USA, Canada, the UK and Australia (n = 8967) towards the donation of DNA and health data. Fifty-two percent of this public held ‘exceptionalist’ views about genetics (i.e., believed DNA is different or ‘special’ compared to other types of medical information). This group was more likely to be familiar with or have had personal experience with genomics and to perceive DNA information as having personal as well as clinical and scientific value. Those with personal experience with genetics and genetic exceptionalist views were nearly six times more likely to be willing to donate their anonymous DNA and medical information for research than other respondents. Perceived harms from re-identification did not appear to dissuade publics from being willing to participate in research. The interplay between exceptionalist views about genetics and the personal, scientific and clinical value attributed to data would be a valuable focus for future research.
Introduction
Genomic medicine is being integrated into healthcare in many countries as an emerging basis of clinical care, public health and disease prediction [1]. The collection and sharing of genomic data is fundamental to this goal, driving research and clinical applications, underpinning the ability to do accurate variant interpretation [2]. Researchers have called for global genomic data sharing, enabling databases to be connected internationally across geographical, legal and policy borders, for databases to be more easily accessible and to ensure that they better represent the populations having genomic testing [1, 3, 4]. The collection of genomic information at scale is thus increasing, and major projects are underway to link genetic and health data of millions of citizens [1, 5].
Data sharing presents a number of challenges, notably related to data confidentiality, risk of discrimination, and the need for appropriate governance structures [6]. However, one important consideration in discussions of data sharing pertains to the perceived similarities and differences between DNA and other forms of personal data. In this paper, we consider the relationship between perceptions of ‘genetic exceptionalism’ and the willingness to donate and share DNA information.
The concept of ‘genetic exceptionalism’ suggests that genetic information has different properties than other types of medical information, i.e., it is ‘special’ because it is uniquely identifying, directly links us to our relatives or can provide information about our past, present and future health [7]. Such considerations have been emphasised by some commentators and inform policy such as the USA’s Genetic Information Nondiscrimination Act, which regards genetic data as unique and meriting increased protection [7, 8]. Others contend that features of genetic information are shared with characteristics such as socio-economic status, HIV status or family history [9]. They suggest that DNA can be seen as distinct and as warranting a special respect for privacy, without legal or regulatory protection beyond that accorded to other sensitive information [10].
Genomic and health data come from health services, biobanks, research projects and donated blood [11, 12]. They originate from individuals who have a right to have a say in how this data will be used; at the very least, they should have consented for their de-identified data to be shared with researchers and clinicians [13]. Furthermore, even if an individual has not yet encountered an opportunity to donate their data they are increasingly likely to be genetically related to someone who has. The decisions that one person makes thus have the potential to be relevant to relatives. To protect the rights of patients and research participants and to respect their expectations and values related to data use, it is important to incorporate the views of broader publics as stakeholders in decision making. It is thus timely to explore global public perceptions of issues surrounding the use of de-identified DNA information within the contexts of genomic research and clinical practice [14].
Our research examined whether ‘genetic exceptionalism’, defined as the belief that DNA information is different from other forms of medical information, is associated with the willingness or unwillingness to donate data. We further examined whether it is associated with different perceptions of potential harms arising from sharing DNA information. Research has identified the importance of concerns about discrimination and the privacy of DNA information in shaping decisions about research participation [15,16,17]. What has yet to be studied is the extent to which willingness to donate and share DNA information and perception of harms among the general public is shaped by ‘exceptionalist’ perceptions of genetic information. While exceptionalism might increase concerns about privacy, it also suggests the potentially distinctive value of genetic information.
In this paper, we describe the perceptions of English-speaking respondents from the UK, USA, Australia and Canada to the ‘Your DNA, Your Say’ (YDYS) study. YDYS aims to examine public perceptions of genomic data sharing across international and language borders and involve, where practically possible, representative samples of publics. Although important empirical work has already been conducted in this space, particularly in the context of biobanking, there remains limited published literature on global public attitudes towards personal donation of DNA information [16, 18]. The survey is a global project that has been translated into multiple languages. Once global recruitment is complete, we will publish separately on a between-country meta-analysis of attitudes.
Materials and methods
Details of the study design, methodology, recruitment and data collection are published separately, as is a review of the context and background of this project [19,20,21].
Sample
Using the market research company Dynata to invite public audiences to participate, we collected surveys from publics in the USA, Canada, UK and Australia (n = 8967). Participants were paid a small financial reward (<£1) for participating. Owing to the nature of recruitment, there are no details on non-response rate. Dynata invited represenative public from each country to participate and we demonstrate how these are matched in terms of age and gender.
Measures
Our cross-sectional, exploratory online survey can be accessed at YourDNAYourSay.org. It contains 29 questions and piloting showed it took 15–20 min to complete.
Sociodemographic information
Information about age was collected in 10-year categories from age 16 onwards; due to the low number of responses in younger and older age categories these were collapsed to ‘30 years and under’ and ‘60 years and older’. Whether participants had children was determined by the question ‘Do you have children?’ Participants were asked to answer ‘Yes’ or ‘No’ and were not asked to specify whether they were their biological children. Relationship status was collected as ‘Divorced’, ‘Separated’, ‘Single’, ‘Widowed’, ‘Married/civil partnership/living together’; all categories apart from the latter were collapsed for analyses.
We piloted how to collect ethnicity data, starting with the categories provided in the UK Census survey and adapting these based on feedback from pilot participants. The resultant ethnicity question in the final survey asked participants to self-identify as: White; Afro-European/African American, Black; Hispanic; South Asian, Indian, Pakistani; East Asian Chinese, Japanese; Arabic, Central Asian; Other. Participants could choose not to answer this question. Owing to the low number of participants who self-identified as a member of a group other than ‘White’ (<10% of the sample for each country), these were collapsed into a single ‘Non-White’ category for analysis.
Education level was categorised as ‘Tertiary’, ‘Secondary’, ‘Primary’ or ‘Other’ based on structured and free text descriptions of educational qualifications and collapsed to a binary indicator of tertiary education for multivariable analyses. Religiosity was determined by response to the question ‘Independent of whether you attend religious services or not, would you say you are…?’ with options ‘A religious person’ or ‘Not a religious person’.
Genetics experience
Genetics experience was derived from two questions: ‘Are you familiar with DNA, genetics or genomics?’ and ‘I’m familiar through my work, personal interests or family/medical history’. Participants were categorised as having ‘Personal’ experience of genetics if they said they were familiar and that familiarity was due to either having a genetic condition in their family, or through their work (e.g., genetic health professional or genetic researcher). Participants without this experience were categorised as ‘Familiar’ or ‘Unfamiliar’ based on their response to the first question.
View of genetic information
Participants were asked if they believed DNA information was the same as other sorts of medical information. The question used was:
Some people think DNA information is the same as any other medical information, like blood pressure or blood sugar levels. Others think DNA information is special, for example, because it tells us how we are related to other people. What do you think?
For me, DNA information is different to other medical information.
For me, DNA information is the same as other medical information.
I’m not sure.
Those who answered that DNA information was different were categorised as having ‘genetic exceptionalist views’. As we were primarily interested in people who had clear exceptionalist views, responses indicating that the participant was unsure or did not think DNA information is the same were combined.
Potential for harm
Participants were asked a single question regarding harms associated with linking personally identifying information to their DNA data: ‘If someone linked your name, address and phone number to it, do you think you could be harmed in any way from this?’. Response options were ‘Yes’, ‘No’, ‘I’m not sure’ with the latter two categories collapsed for analysis. As we were primarily interested in whether people had a clear understanding of the harms, the ‘No’ and ‘Unsure’ categories were collapsed for analysis.
Concerns about specific harms
Participants were presented with a list of theoretical harms that could occur in relation to DNA information and asked to indicate which were the three that concerned them most. The list of theoretical harms presented to participants was:
My friends potentially knowing something about me that I hadn’t chosen to tell them.
My family potentially knowing something about me that I hadn’t chosen to tell them.
My government potentially knowing something about me that I hadn’t chosen to tell them.
Police potentially knowing something about me that I hadn’t chosen to tell them.
Marketing companies targeting me to sell me products
Being stigmatised and labelled in some way online.
Being cloned.
My DNA being copied and then planted at the scene of a crime.
Health or life insurance companies using the information to discriminate against me.
Employers using the information to discriminate against me.
Upsetting my genetic relatives.
Ethnic identification and racial discrimination.
Donating DNA and medical information
Throughout the survey, participants were asked whether they would donate their ‘anonymous’ DNA and medical information for use by others in research [1]Footnote 1. Participants were asked whether they would donate DNA and medical information for use by (a) medical doctors; (b) non-profit researchers; (c) for-profit researchers. Participants were classified as willing to donate if they answered yes to at least one of these questions, and as unwilling if they answered no to all three. In terms of influences on donating, participants were asked ‘Would being offered a DNA readout influence your decision to donate?’ and ‘Would you be more comfortable donating your DNA and/or medical information if you knew there was a law in place to protect against being exploited?’. Finally, participants were asked ‘Would you allow someone else, such as an ethics committee or custodian, to decide on your behalf which researchers and studies could use your DNA and/or medical information?’.
Statistical analysis
Sample characteristics were summarised using standard descriptive statistics, with differences between those with and without exceptionalist views evaluated using chi-squared tests. Multivariable analyses were conducted using multi-level binary logistic regression models to allow for clustering of participants within countries, and estimation of the variability in the outcome variables based on country of residence. The models provide odds ratio (OR) estimates for the association between perspective on DNA information and either perspective on harms or willingness to donate, holding country of residence constant [22, 23].
Multivariable analyses
The multivariable analysis investigated the association between perceptions of DNA information and (i) perceptions of harms associated with linking DNA to other personal information; (ii) willingness to donate DNA for research. Familiarity with genetics, age, gender, ethnicity, marital status, having children, education level, and religiosity have previously been associated with perceptions of genetics and were included as covariates [24,25,26].
Models included a random country-level intercept to allow for between-country variation in perspectives on harms associated with linking DNA, and willingness to donate DNA for research. The effect of country of residence was quantified using the intraclass correlation and median odds ratio [27]. Perspective on DNA information was initially modelled as a fixed effect; random slope models were then fitted to evaluate whether the associations varied by country of residence. Familiarity with genetics, age, gender, ethnicity, marital status, having children, education level, and religiosity were included as fixed effects. As we anticipated that the relationship between exceptionalism and the outcome variables would be influenced by familiarity with genetics, an interaction term between perspective on DNA information and familiarity with genetics was also included.
Models were fitted via maximum likelihood with difference in model fit evaluated using likelihood ratio chi-squared tests and akaike information criterion (AIC) where appropriate [28]. As the alternative hypotheses regarding variances are technically one-sided, having the p-value for these tests has been suggested; we report the standard p-values but consider this modification when interpreting results [29]. Complete-case analyses were conducted in R version 3.3.1 [30] using the lme4 package for multi-level models, with 95% confidence intervals (CI) for the final model parameter estimates obtained using bootstrapping with 1000 replicates per model [31].
Results
Sample description
Comparison with most recent census data from each country shows the sample is representative for gender, but with a slight over-sampling of younger age groups and under-sampling of those over 60 years of age (Table S1). There was some variation between those who did and did not have genetic exceptionalist views for all sociodemographic variables with the exception of gender and ethnicity (Table 1); however, there were minimal between-country differences for this variable.
Participants with genetic exceptionalist views (~52% of survey respondents) were more likely to report: familiarity with genetics (35.2% versus 26.7%); personal experience with genetics (17.2% versus 8.7%); having children (60.2% versus 53.6%); tertiary-level education (60.5% versus 54.8%); and being religious (41.4% versus 33.1%). We adjusted for all potential confounding effects in the multivariable modelling.
Associations between potential harms, willingness to donate and genetic exceptionalism
Participants holding genetic exceptionalist views were substantially more likely to think that linking personally identifying information to their DNA information could potentially harm them in some way (49.5% compared to 35.2%; see Table 2). However, concerns about specific harms did not vary significantly between the two groups. Of the 11 potential harms participants were asked to rate, the most frequently identified by both those with and without genetic exceptionalist views was that related to ‘My DNA being copied and then planted at the scene of a crime’ (included in the top three concerns by 45.2% of the sample). The next most frequently identified were ‘Health or life insurance companies using the information to discriminate against me’ (37.2%) and ‘Marketing companies targeting me to sell me products’ (35%) (Fig. 1 shows these results broken down by views on genetic exceptionalism).
Despite being more likely to identify the possibility of harm, participants with genetic exceptionalist views were substantially more likely to accept that their ‘anonymous’ DNA and/or medical information should be donated for research purposes (65.3% versus 47.1%, see Table 2). Their decision on whether to contribute their data to research was also more likely to be influenced by the prospect of receiving a readout of their own DNA upon sharing their DNA (50.6% versus 30.2%) and by knowing there are legal protections in place to prevent exploitation (63.9% versus 47.2%). Irrespective of participants’ view of genetic exceptionalism, only a minority (17.4%) were comfortable with allowing a research ethics committee to make decisions on their behalf regarding research use of their DNA and/or medical information. However, those who had genetic exceptionalist views were more likely to be comfortable with this (22.7% versus 11.7%).
Multivariable associations with perceived harm of linking DNA information
We first fitted ‘empty’ multi-level models to investigate how much variability in participant views of whether linking DNA and personal information could harm them could be explained by participant country of residence [32]. The likelihood ratio test comparing fixed effects and random intercept models indicated that there was significant variance explained by the between-country effect on perceived harm (\(\chi_{1}^{2}\) = 54.3; p < 0.0001). However, the intraclass correlations and median ORs were small; the percentage of variance in perceived harm explained by country of residence was only 0.8% and the median OR was 1.09.
Allowing the effect of participants’ views of the difference between DNA and other medical information to vary by country of residence did not significantly improve model fit (\(\chi_{2}^{2}\) = 0.49; p = 0.78), indicating the strength of relationship between exceptionalism and perceived harm was similar across countries. Consequently this variable was included as a fixed effect in the full multivariable model. We tested the importance of the interaction between perception of DNA information and familiarity of genetics, and found moderate evidence to support it for this outcome variable (\(\chi_{2}^{2}\) = 5.64; p = 0.06; AIC 11,464 versus 11,463). Results from the full model are shown in Table 3.
Based on the full multivariable model (Tables 3 and 4), participants who had genetic exceptionalist views had greater odds of thinking that linking their DNA information to identifying personal details could potentially cause them harm, compared to others with the same level of genetics familiarity [OR 1.76 (95% CI: 1.56–1.98), OR 1.40 (1.19–1.65), OR 1.76 (1.35–2.31)] for ‘unfamiliar’, ‘familiar’, and ‘personal’ strata, respectively). Those who had genetic exceptionalist views and were familiar with genetics had greater odds of thinking this could potentially cause them harm compared to those who did not view DNA as different and were unfamiliar with genetics (OR 2.73, 95% CI 2.38–3.13). However, there was minimal difference between those who were familiar with genetics and those who had personal experience through work or a family history of an inherited condition (OR 2.81, 95% CI 2.36–3.35 for the latter).
Multivariable associations with donating DNA information for research
The likelihood ratio test comparing fixed effects and random intercept models did not indicate that substantial variance was explained by the between-country effect on willingness to donate DNA and/or medical information for research (\(\chi_{1}^{2}\) = 3.18; p = 0.075) and thus a standard logistic regression model could feasibly be fitted. We retained the multi-level model specification to ensure we adequately accounted for between-country differences. Allowing the effect of participant view of the difference between DNA and other medical information to vary by country of residence did not significantly improve model fit (\(\chi_{2}^{2}\) = 2.69; p = 0.26) and this was, therefore, included as a fixed effect in the full multivariable model. We tested the interaction between willingness to donate DNA and/or medical information and familiarity with genetics and found evidence to support it for this outcome variable (\(\chi_{2}^{2}\) = 6.65; p = 0.036; AIC 11,171 versus 11,168). Results are shown in Table 3.
In the full multivariable model (Tables 3 and 4), participants who had genetic exceptionalist views had greater odds of being willing to donate their DNA compared to others with the same level of genetics familiarity [OR 1.98 (95% CI: 1.78–2.24), OR 1.55 (1.32–1.83), OR 1.58 (1.18–2.12) for ‘unfamiliar’, ‘familiar’, and ‘personal’ strata, respectively]. Those who had personal experience of genetics and genetic exceptionalist views had the greatest odds of being willing to donate, compared to those who were unfamiliar with genetics and did not have a genetic exceptionalist view (OR 5.66, 95% CI 4.64–6.91). Those with personal experience of genetics also had greater odds of being willing to donate, even if they did not have genetic exceptionalist views (OR 3.58, 95% CI 2.81–4.65). However, those who were familiar with genetics and had genetic exceptionalist views had similar odds of being willing to donate (OR 3.05, 95% CI 2.66–3.49).
Discussion
In this age and gender-matched sample from the USA, Canada, the UK and Australia, participants who held genetic exceptionalist views were more likely to be familiar with or have had personal experience with genomics. They were also the most likely to say they were willing to donate their ‘anonymous’ DNA and medical information to research, despite also being the most likely to understand that linking DNA information to personal information had the potential to cause harm. Thus, while they identified the possibility of harm if they were re-identified from their data, they would still donate, presumably because they accepted the benefits outweighed the perceived harms. This is concordant with the findings of previous research on biobanking and the donation of DNA information for research [33]. In other words, those interested in data donation appear aware of both what genetic information is and its value for research and genomic medicine.
Participants with genetic exceptionalist views and self-reported familiarity with genomics had the greatest odds (nearly 6 times greater) of being willing to donate their DNA information (compared to those who were unfamiliar with genomics and did not have exceptionalist views). Such willingness among those with personal familiarity has been seen in patient groups who want their data to be put to good use for future disease prevention, or to help future generations within their own family [34, 35]. Many of those most willing to donate were also professionals working in the genomics field. This is in agreement with our previous research on the liberal attitudes of scientists to participate in genomic research [36].
The perceived harms that participants associated with being identified from one’s DNA information reflect the influence of popular culture. After secondary education, most non-experts are thought to absorb science information and knowledge from the media [37]. For example, TV shows such as CSI (American television programme, made 2000–2015) with 30–50 million viewers worldwide function as a resource for quasi-scientific information about genetics [38]. Media representations play an important role in shaping public familiarity with science, including framing or priming conversations about DNA [39]. The identification by participants of concerns about their DNA being copied and then planted at the scene of a crime fits with this picture. What we are not able to conclude is whether participants have a genuine concern about this, or whether it is made salient and plausible by exposure to popular media.
Risks associated with data sharing may be seen as less concrete and more future-oriented than benefits [18]. However, the relevance of perceived ‘harms’ as actual ‘risks’ needs to be understood in relation to their probability of occurring [40]. Public engagement that discusses the potential risks or harms of linking DNA information to personal information, however small or unlikely, may therefore not adversely affect the willingness of people to donate their data. Indeed, engagement activities that explain the distinctive features of DNA data may in fact support engagement with research.
Those with exceptionalist views were more likely to make decisions about donation based on whether they could obtain a copy of their own raw data (a ‘DNA readout’ or raw sequence data) in return and if they were aware of any clear legal sanctions in place to protect against exploitation. This reflects the perceived personal and economic, as well as clinical and scientific, value of DNA information. This group was more likely to allow research ethics committees (RECs) to make decisions about data sharing on their behalf. These findings suggest the potential value of returning results to this population, but also the corollary, that receiving sequence data may be less appealing to those who do not regard it as something special or distinctive. They also emphasise the importance of transparent processes for decision making around data sharing, and communication about the sanctions consequent upon data misuse.
Limitations
Exploratory online surveys have important limitations in that they capture perceptions about intended behaviour at a single time point. While intentions are one potential predictor of behaviour, further work is needed to document what people actually do when faced with opportunities to donate data [41]. Generic limitations of the study and online survey design have been published separately [19]. Our findings should not be extrapolated to indicate views of all people from the USA, Canada, the UK and Australia, particularly the older population who may differ in respect of their willingness to donate and beliefs about the uniqueness of genetic information.
Conclusion
Big data and genomics now go hand in hand, and it is time to bring broader publics into conversation about their willingness to donate their data to be accessed and shared to enable the potential of genomic medicine to be fully realised. To support this, we have explored ‘representative’ English-speaking public perceptions of genomic data donation and some of the characteristics of those reportedly willing to donate their data for use in the research endeavour.
There appear obvious factors that may support and encourage data donation and sharing, such as offering to return results in some form, providing clear information about legal protections, and engagement that addresses the distinctive characteristics of genetic information. The potential of these approaches should be explored and evaluated. However, it is not sufficient to ‘educate’ people about genomics—familiarity does not necessarily equate factual knowledge. Rather, we must work to understand what people need to know and how to make the subject resonate, so that genomics becomes a social and sociable concept and that citizens can feel comfortable having a basic conversation about its benefits and limitations.
Finally, we found that despite believing that there were potential specific risks from re-identification, these alone did not appear to dissuade publics from being willing to participate in research. Thus, for some, it appears that DNA has sufficient value to warrant donation for research. Exploring the interplay between exceptionalist views and the personal, scientific and clinical value attributed to data would be a fruitful focus for future research.
Notes
Within the survey glossary we explained ‘anonymous’ in more detail: ‘Anonymous: removal of personal information such as name and date of birth. It is questionable as to whether DNA information can ever be truly anonymous as our DNA code is unique to us and thus, in itself, could be used to identify us. However, in the circumstances we are exploring here, by making DNA and medical information ‘anonymous’, we mean detaching personal identifiers from it.’ What we are actually meaning here is ‘de-identified’ but within the pilot work for the survey we discovered that public participants did not naturally understand this term and ‘anonymous’ was more easily understood, thus we added the glossary definition within the survey itself, to explain this in more detail.
References
Stark Z, Dolman L, Manolio TA, Ozenberger B, Hill SL, Caulfield MJ. et al. Integrating genomics into healthcare: a global responsibility. Am J Hum Genet. 2019;104:13–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajhg.2018.11.014.
Aronson SJ, Rehm HL. Building the foundation for genomics in precision medicine. Nature. 2015;526:336. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature15816.
Bustamante CD, Burchard EG, De la Vega FM. Genomics for the world. Nature. 2011;475:163–5. https://doi.org/10.1038/475163a.
Popejoy AB, Fullerton SM. Genomics is failing on diversity. Nature. 2016;538:161–4. https://doi.org/10.1038/538161a.
Collins FS, Varmus H. A new initiative on precision medicine. N. Engl J Med. 2015;372:793–5. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMp1500523.
Knoppers BM. Framework for responsible sharing of genomic and health-related. data. Hugo J. 2014;8:3. https://doi.org/10.1186/s11568-014-0003-1.
Roche PA, Annas GJ. Protecting genetic privacy. Nat Rev Genet. 2001;2:392–6. https://doi.org/10.1038/35072029.
McGuire AL, Fisher R, Cusenza P, Hudson K, Rothstein MA, McGraw D. et al. Confidentiality, privacy, and security of genetic and genomic test information in electronic health records: points to consider. Genet Med. 2008;10:495–9. https://doi.org/10.1097/GIM.0b013e31817a8aaa.
Evans JP, Burke W. Genetic exceptionalism. Too much of a good thing?. Genet Med.2008;10:500–1. https://doi.org/10.1097/GIM.0b013e31817f280a.
Sulmasy DP. Naked bodies, naked genomes: the special (but not exceptional) nature of genomic information. Genet Med. 2015;17:331–6. https://doi.org/10.1038/gim.2014.111.
Wright CF, FitzPatrick DR, Firth HV. Paediatric genomics: diagnosing rare disease in children. Nat Rev Genet.2018;19:253–68. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrg.2017.116.
Auffray C, Balling R, Barroso I, Bencze L, Benson M, Bergeron J. et al. Making sense of big data in health research: towards an EU action plan. Genome Med. 2016;8:71. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13073-016-0323-y.
Nuffield Council on Bioethics. The collection, linking and use of data in biomedical research and health care: ethical issues. London; 2015. Accessed 27 November 2019. https://nuffieldbioethics.org/wp-content/uploads/Biodata-a-guide-to-the-report-PDF.pdf.
Kaye J, Terry SF, Juengst E, Coy S, Harris JR, Chalmers D. et al. Including all voices in international data-sharing governance. Hum Genomics. 2018;12:13. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40246-018-0143-9.
Aitken M, de St. Jorre J, Pagliari C, Jepson R, Cunningham-Burley S. Public responses to the sharing and linkage of health data for research purposes: a systematic review and thematic synthesis of qualitative studies. BMC Med Ethics. 2016;17:73. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12910-016-0153-x.
Lemke AAA, Wolf WAA, Hebert-Beirne J, Smith MEE. Public and biobank participant attitudes toward genetic research participation and data sharing. Public Health Genomics. 2010;13:368–77. https://doi.org/10.1159/000276767.
Trinidad SB, Fullerton SM, Bares JM, Jarvik GP, Larson EB, Burke W. Genomic research and wide data sharing: views of prospective participants. Genet Med. 2010;12:486–95. https://doi.org/10.1097/GIM.0b013e3181e38f9e.
Oliver JM, Slashinski MJ, Wang T, Kelly PA, Hilsenbeck SG, McGuire AL. Balancing the risks and benefits of genomic data sharing: genome research participants’ perspectives. Public Health Genomics. 2012;15:106–14. https://doi.org/10.1159/000334718.
Middleton A, Niemiec E, Prainsack B, Bobe J, Farley L, Steed C. et al. ‘Your DNA, Your Say’: global survey gathering attitudes toward genomics: design, delivery and methods. Per Med. 2018;15:311–8. https://doi.org/10.2217/pme-2018-0032.
Middleton A. Your DNA, your say. N. Bioeth. 2017;23:74–80. https://doi.org/10.1080/20502877.2017.1314890.
Middleton A. Society and personal genome data. Hum Mol Genet. 2018;27(R1):R8–13. https://doi.org/10.1093/hmg/ddy084.
Gibbons RD, Hedeker D, DuToit S. Advances in analysis of longitudinal data. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2010;6:79–107. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.032408.153550.
Hubbard AE, Ahern J, Fleischer NL, Van der Laan M, Lippman SA, Jewell N. et al. To GEE or not to GEE: comparing population average and mixed models for estimating the associations between neighborhood risk factors and health. Epidemiology. 2010;21:467–74. https://doi.org/10.1097/EDE.0b013e3181caeb90.
Condit CM. Public attitudes and beliefs about genetics. Annu Rev Genomics Hum Genet. 2010;11:339–59. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-genom-082509-141740.
Allum N, Sibley E, Sturgis P, Stoneman P. Religious beliefs, knowledge about science and attitudes towards medical genetics. Public Underst Sci. 2014;23:833–49. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963662513492485.
Haga SB, Barry WT, Mills R, Ginsburg GS, Svetkey L, Sullivan J. et al. Public knowledge of and attitudes toward genetics and genetic testing. Genet Test Mol Biomark. 2013;17:327–35. https://doi.org/10.1089/gtmb.2012.0350.
Merlo J, Chaix B, Ohlsson H, Beckman A, Johnell K, Hjerpe P. et al. A brief conceptual tutorial of multilevel analysis in social epidemiology: using measures of clustering in multilevel logistic regression to investigate contextual phenomena. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2006;60:290–7. https://doi.org/10.1136/jech.2004.029454.
Shmueli G. To explain or to predict?. Stat Sci. 2010;25:289–310. https://doi.org/10.1214/10-STS330.
Snijders TAB, Bosker RJ. Multilevel analysis: an introduction to basic and advanced multilevel modeling. Thousand Oaks California; London: SAGE; 1999.
R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Core Team; 2016. Accessed 27 November 2019. https://www.r-project.org/.
Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker BM, Walker SC. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Softw. 2015;67:1–48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01.
Merlo J, Chaix B, Yang M, Lynch J, Råstam L. A brief conceptual tutorial of multilevel analysis in social epidemiology: linking the statistical concept of clustering to the idea of contextual phenomenon. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2005;59:443–9. https://doi.org/10.1136/jech.2004.023473.
Gaskell G, Gottweis H, Starkbaum J, Gerber MM, Broerse J, Gottweis U. et al. Publics and biobanks: Pan-European diversity and the challenge of responsible innovation. Eur J Hum Genet. 2013;21:14–20. https://doi.org/10.1038/ejhg.2012.104.
McCormack P, Kole A, Gainotti S, Mascalzoni D, Molster C, Lochmüller H. et al. ‘You should at least ask’. The expectations, hopes and fears of rare disease patients on large-scale data and biomaterial sharing for genomics research. Eur J Hum Genet. 2016;24:1403–8. https://doi.org/10.1038/ejhg.2016.30.
Darquy S, Moutel G, Lapointe AS, D’Audiffret D, Champagnat J, Guerroui S. et al. Patient/family views on data sharing in rare diseases: study in the European LeukoTreat project. Eur J Hum Genet. 2016;24:338–43. https://doi.org/10.1038/ejhg.2015.115.
Middleton A, Morley KI, Bragin E, Firth HV, Hurles ME, Wright CF. et al. Attitudes of nearly 7000 health professionals, genomic researchers and publics toward the return of incidental results from sequencing research. Eur J Hum Genet. 2016;24:21–9. https://doi.org/10.1038/ejhg.2015.58.
National Science Board. Science and Engineering Indicators 2012. Washington DC: OmniStudio, Inc; 2012. https://nsf.gov/statistics/seind12/pdf/seind12.pdf.
Ley BL, Jankowski N, Brewer PR. Investigating CSI: portrayals of DNA testing on a forensic crime show and their potential effects. Public Underst Sci. 2012;21:51–67. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963662510367571.
Brewer PR, Ley BL. Media use and public perceptions of DNA evidence. Sci Commun. 2009;32:93–117. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547009340343.
Sjoberg L. Factors in risk perception. Risk Anal. 2000;20:1–11. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10841699
Glasman LR, Albarracín D. Forming attitudes that predict future behavior: a meta-analysis of the attitude–behavior relation. Psychol Bull. 2006;132:778–822. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.5.778.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by Wellcome grant [206194] to the Society and Ethics Research Group, Connecting Science, Wellcome Genome Campus, Cambridge, UK. We would like to thank the following people from GA4GH for their encouragement and infrastructure support: Peter Goodhand, Julia Wilson, Bartha Knoppers. This work was also supported by Global Alliance for Genomics and Health, with their funding delivered via Wellcome (GA4GH grant, with thanks to Audrey Duncansen). DV acknowledges the infrastructure funding received from the Victorian State Government through the Operational Infrastructure Support (OIS) Programme.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Consortia
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Middleton, A., Milne, R., Howard, H. et al. Members of the public in the USA, UK, Canada and Australia expressing genetic exceptionalism say they are more willing to donate genomic data. Eur J Hum Genet 28, 424–434 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41431-019-0550-y
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41431-019-0550-y
This article is cited by
-
Context matters in genomic data sharing: a qualitative investigation into responses from the Australian public
BMC Medical Genomics (2023)
-
Motives for withdrawal of participation in biobanking and participants’ willingness to allow linkages of their data
European Journal of Human Genetics (2022)
-
Sharing genomic data from clinical testing with researchers: public survey of expectations of clinical genomic data management in Queensland, Australia
BMC Medical Ethics (2020)
-
Willingness to donate genomic and other medical data: results from Germany
European Journal of Human Genetics (2020)