Abstract
Background
Identifying modifiable factors associated with well-being is of increased interest for public policy guidance. Developments in record linkage make it possible to identify what contributes to well-being from a myriad of factors. To this end, we link two large-scale data resources; the Geoscience and Health Cohort Consortium, a collection of geo-data, and the Netherlands Twin Register, which holds population-based well-being data.
Objective
We perform an Environment-Wide Association Study (EnWAS), where we examine 139 neighbourhood-level environmental exposures in relation to well-being.
Methods
First, we performed a generalized estimation equation regression (Nā=ā11,975) to test for the effects of environmental exposures on well-being. Second, to account for multicollinearity amongst exposures, we performed principal component regression. Finally, using a genetically informative design, we examined whether environmental exposure is driven by genetic predisposition for well-being.
Results
We identified 21 environmental factors that were associated with well-being in the domains: housing stock, income, core neighbourhood characteristics, livability, and socioeconomic status. Of these associations, socioeconomic status and safety are indicated as the most important factors to explain differences in well-being. No evidence of gene-environment correlation was found.
Significance
These observed associations, especially neighbourhood safety, could be informative for policy makers and provide public policy guidance to improve well-being. Our results show that linking databases is a fruitful exercise to identify determinants of mental health that would remain unknown by a more unilateral approach.
Introduction
Demographic factors are widely recognized as important for peopleās functioning and mental health. For example, urbanization, i.e., the movement of population from rural to more urbanized areas, is accompanied by both beneficial and detrimental effects on mental health. Urbanization is often associated with economic growth and prosperity [1, 2], and comes with better infrastructure and better access to health care services [3]. Mental disorders, though, are more prevalent in more urbanized areas [4, 5] for example due to less access to green space [6], increased social stress [7], and less (perceived) neighbourhood safety [8, 9]. Moreover, genetic factors influence where people prefer to live and how their environment impacts them. For instance, research into urbanization and schizophrenia showed that individuals with a higher genetic predisposition to schizophrenia tend to live in urbanized areas. For instance, research into urbanization and schizophrenia showed that individuals with increased genetic predisposition for schizophrenia tend to live in more urbanized areas. While it was previously assumed that the higher schizophrenia prevalence was explained by increased environmental stress in urbanized areas, this study revealed that part of why schizophrenia is more prevalent in cities is because of an increased genetic predisposition [10].
Recent developments in data sharing and linkage are transforming the way we approach mental health topics and its possible correlates. One of the developments that makes it possible to identify what contributes to mental health and human functioning from a myriad of factors is record linkage. By linking large data resources that contain different types of information, novel, otherwise invisible patterns can be uncovered. A well-known example in is the UK Biobank. By linking genetic (and biological, phenotypic) data to existing health records, great advances have been made in identifying risk factors for disorders such as schizophrenia and depression [11,12,13]. Record linkage is becoming increasingly accessible for researchers across different disciplines and countries. For example, in the Netherlands, data on households, job benefits, education, crime, and more is available on a population-based scale [14]. This type of data can, under certain conditions and strict privacy regulations, be linked to patient data [15], environmental data [16], and other data resources in the country [17, 18].
In this paper, we illustrate the potential of record linkage to better understand complex human traits to inform prevention, intervention, and policy by investigating environmental factors that potentially influence well-being. Most existing research on environmental effects for well-being to date follows a pick and choose approach e.g., [19,20,21], which could result in selective reporting or overestimation of effects. To overcome these limitations we propose a data-driven design, an Environment-Wide Association Study [22] (EnWAS). This approach is based on the principles of a genome-wide association study (GWAS), where each genetic marker in the genome is systematically tested for association with the phenotype of interest. Instead of genetic markers, EnWAS systematically associates environmental variables while reducing the chance of spurious findings by accounting for multiple testing. This data-driven approach is of particular interest given the lack of theoretical inclusion models and was recently successfully applied to examine behavioural patterns, psychosocial factors, mental and physical health conditions, access to and utilization of health care, and anthropometrics with physical, mental and social well-being [23]. From a broad range of psychosocial factors, 3 factors were associated with well-being: depressive symptoms, life satisfaction, and happiness. While this study provides us with valuable information of psychosocial associations with well-being, it did not explore physical environmental factors such as neighbourhood characteristics, in relation to well-being. Given that many governmental decisions and prevention and intervention policies are enroled at a neighbourhood level it is very important to get an indication of the effect of neighbourhood-level characteristics on person-level well- being.
In order to examine environmental variables associated with well-being, we applied EnWAS by linking well-being data from the population based Netherlands Twin Register (NTR) [17] to environmental data from the Geoscience and Health Cohort Consortium (GECCO) [16]. We examine 139 environmental variables that cover most aspects of peopleās living environments e.g., land use in terms of build area or green space, and neighbourhood characteristics, such as safety and livability. In addition, given that it is widely accepted that people do not randomly choose where they live [24, 25], that differences in well-being are partly accounted for by genetic differences [26, 27], and to overcome possible genetic confounding, we use a genetically informative design. With this design we examine whether environmental exposure is driven by genetic predisposition for well-being. By combining exposome, phenome, and genome data, we aim to extend the limits of traditional approaches to get more comprehensive insight in how well-being can be placed in a broader context [28].
Materials and methods
Sample
This study used well-being data from the Adult sample of the NTR [17, 29]. For the current project, we made use of data collected in the 6th wave of data collection (2002/2003), and the 8th wave of data collection (2009/2010). These waves were chosen based on the fact that we collected satisfaction with life data at both these time-points. This resulted in a dataset of 9951 individuals for 2002/2003 and 11,975 individuals for 2009/2010. Sample characteristics can be found in TableĀ 1. Depending on the missing-ness of environmental data per GECCO dataset, the number of individuals per analysis varies slightly across analyses.
Well-being data
To quantify well-being, we used the satisfaction with life (SWL) scale [30, 31]. The SWL scale consists of five items measuring satisfaction with life. Each item required a judgement of a given statement pertaining to SWL on a response scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree), summed to create individual sum scores ranging from 7 to 35.
Environmental exposure data
Environmental data were extracted from the Geoscience and Health Cohort Consortium (GECCO) [16] database. The GECCO database is a centralized collection of longitudinal geo-data on different geospatial levels in the Netherlands. As the GECCO data were collected in different time frames, we matched the GECCO data to the NTR data available in 2002/2003 and 2009/2010. In total, 1330 postal-code level variables are available within 34 predefined domains in the GECCO database (see Supplementary TablesĀ S1 and S2). The data sources from which GECCO extracted the variables are frequently used government/census data resources, more information on which can be found in Supplementary TableĀ S2. The environmental exposure data that are available in the GECCO database encompass a wide range of environmental domains, including social, physical and demographic variables. We selected variables representative of neighbourhood characteristics, regardless of which environmental domain they encompass. We pre-selected 168 variables based on two criteria: (1) availability at the same time-points as the NTR well-being assessment, and (2) we chose the most representative variables per domain to prevent inclusion of duplicate variables/ variables that were, without inspection of the data, expected not to vary across the Netherlands. Supplementary TableĀ S3 provides an overview of these preregistered variables. Ultimately, quality control led to the inclusion of 133 variables grouped in 22 domains (see Supplementary TableĀ S4). Importantly, some GECCO variables were assessed in both 2002/2003 and 2009/2010, and some exclusively at one of the time-points. More specifically, 80 variables were measured exclusively in 2002/2003, 23 variables were measured exclusively in 2009/2010, and 15 variables were measured on both occasions. Four-digit postal codes were used to link the environmental data to individual level well-being. FigureĀ 1 and TableĀ 2 describe the included domains, and Supplementary TableĀ S5 provides descriptive statistics on these exposure variables. Since we also included educational attainment variables, we provide a schematic overview of the Dutch educational system in Supplementary Fig. S1.
Genetic data
Genotyping was performed on different SNP micro-arrays that were cross-platform imputed using the Genome of the Netherlands (GoNL) reference set [32]. Quality control procedures are described in theĀ Supplementary Methods. Principal component analysis (PCA) was performed to create genomic PCs reflecting ancestry and genotyping batch effects (for details see Abdellaoui et al. 2013). In total, genetic data and well-being scores were available for 7527 individuals (see TableĀ 1).
Analyses
This project was pre-registered at the open science framework (OSF) (https://osf.io/xehkc). Non-pre-registered follow-up analyses are indicated as such throughout the paper.
Pre-registered
Regression analyses
We pre-registered multilevel models to account for potential within-postal code well-being similarity of participants. Supplementary TableĀ S6 summarizes the number of participants per postal code. However, after accessing the data, the intra-class correlation (ICC) for well-being showed that the dependency of the observations within postal code is neglible (0.02 for 2002/2003 and 0.002 for 2009/2010). Therefore, we proceeded our analyses with generalized estimating equation (GEE) models, instead of multilevel models. GEE corrects for correlated observations, allowing us to include the full sample (instead of only genetically unrelated individuals). Regression analyses were performed for each environmental predictor, with sex, age, and age-squared as covariates. Familial relatedness was accounted for using an exchangeable conditional covariance matrix based on sandwich-corrected standard errors [33], as implemented in the GEE package in R. Statistical significance was assessed using a Bonferroni-corrected significance threshold of 3.6āĆā10ā4 (0.05/139). Power to detect associations with different potential effect sizes can be found in the pre-registration.
Polygenic risk score analysis
To assess the role of genetic factors in the associations obtained in the GEE analyses, we performed polygenic score (PGS) prediction analyses. A PGS reflects an individualās genetic liability for a trait of interest, calculated from the effect sizes from GWA summary statistics. The PGSs were computed for the well-being spectrum in NTR participants using the GWA summary statistics (recomputed excluding NTR) from Baselmans et al. [34]. The summary statistics were recomputed using LDpred [35]. These recomputed summary statistics were turned into PGSs using allelic scoring function in PLINK [36]. This function aggregates the number of effect alleles weighted by their effect estimates in each individual to create scores reflecting an individualās genetic liability for a trait. GEE was used to test the association of the well-being spectrum PGSs (independent variable) with significant environmental correlates (dependent variables) from the EnWAS. Age, age-squared, sex, and the first ten genomic PCs were included as covariates.
In addition, we used the well-being spectrum PGSs to split the sample into septiles to evaluate the potential of stratifying individuals based on a PGS for well-being. The first septile contains participants with the lowest genetic susceptibility for well-being, and the seventh septile contains those with the highest. We calculated the mean well-being and environmental value per septile and compared whether these means differed significantly by examining overlap in confidence intervals.
Non pre-registered
Multicollinearity follow-up
In the univariate analyses the covariates were considered one at a time, thus ignoring the possible correlation between these variables. To illustrate the overlap between the different variables that significantly predict well-being, we visualized the correlations in chord diagrams using the circlize package in R [37]. We plotted the associations separately for the variables from 2002/2003 and 2009/2010, and made separate plots for: (a) correlations stronger than 0.8, and (c) correlations stronger than 0.4.
Next, to accommodate the relative strong correlations between the environmental factors (see Supplementary TableĀ S7), we ran a principal component analysis (PCA) of the standardized environmental exposures using the prcomp function from the stats package in R. We aimed to extract independent principal components (PCs) that explained at least 90% of the environmental data. Next, these uncorrelated PCs were used as independent predictors to predict well-being in an unrelated sample (after the effects of age, age [2], and sex were regressed out). Based on this analysis, we examined how much variance in well-being can be explained by the combined environmental factors.
Socioeconomic status correction
In the exploratory, data-driven approach of our initial pre-registered analyses, we did not correct for socioeconomic status (SES). However, outcomes of the GEE and the principal component analyses suggested a potential role of SES in the associations. Therefore, as none-preregistered follow-up, we repeated the GEE analyses while correcting for SES using two strategies: (1) including the individualās educational attainment to approximate individual SES, and (2) including the GECCO variable āstatus score of the neighbourhoodā as a measurement of neighbourhood SES (seeĀ Supplementary Methods for more information).
Results
Regression analyses
In the GEE analyses, 21 of the 139 environmental variables passed the Bonferroni-corrected threshold and thus were found to be associated with well-being (Figs.Ā 2ā3, and TableĀ 3). These variables were included in the domains: housing stock, income, core neighbourhood characteristics, livability, and SES scores. An overview of all associations can be found in Supplementary TableĀ S8.
Polygenic risk score analysis
The well-being spectrum polygenic score predicted well-being in our sample (R2ā=ā0.007, Pā=ā5.11āĆā10ā12), but it did not predict any of the environmental correlates (TableĀ 4). In addition, no mean difference between polygenic septiles was observed for any of the variables (see Supplementary TableĀ S9).
Multicollinearity follow-up
Strong correlations (ranging between ā1 and 0.87) were observed between the significant variables from the GEE analyses (Supplementary TableĀ S7). For both time-points, we plotted the variables that were correlated 0.8 or stronger (Supplementary Figs. S2A and S3A), and .4 or stronger (Supplementary Figs. S2B and S3B) using chord plots. These plots display all associations (above our defined thresholds) between the included variables. The variables are presented in a circle, and whenever a line connects two variables, it indicates they are associated. For both time-points, when we defined the threshold as correlations >0.4, we see that all variables are connected to all other variables, creating a densely connected plot. However, when we increased the threshold to 0.8, the plots become more organized with only few connections remaining. For the 2002/2003 data, this resulted in a plot with three clusters: (1) a housing cluster with housing score, housing stock owner-occupied, housing stock: rental, and housing stock: social rent, (2) a livability cluster of livability scores, population composition scores, and safety scores, and (3) another livability cluster with status scores and rank order of the neighbourhoods. For the 2009/2010 data, we see two clusters: (1) an SES cluster including two income variables, mean house value, and the status score and rank order of the neighbourhood, and (2) a livability cluster including LBM scores, population composition, and safety.
The PCA extracted 95 and 38 independent PCs for 2002/2003 and 2009/2010, respectively. The first 43 PCs cumulatively explained 90.5% of the 95 environmental variables in the 2002/2003 data, and the first 16 PCs explained 90.7% of the 38 environmental variables in the 2009/2010 data (see Supplementary TableĀ S10). Combined in one linear regression model, these 43 PCs explained 1.45% of the variance in well-being in the 2002/2003 data. After correcting for the number of PCs included, this decreased to 0.69% (adjusted R2). One PC (PC3: βā=āā0.029, SEā=ā0.006, Pā=ā2.73āĆā10ā7) significantly predicted well-being after correcting for multiple testing. For the 2009/2010 data, the 16 PCs explained 1.11% of the variance in well-being, which decreased to 0.79% after correcting for the number of PCs (adjusted R2). Two PCs significantly predicted well-being (PC1: βā=ā0.0185, SEā=ā0.005, Pā=ā0.0001, PC2: βā=āā0.0240, SEā=ā0.006, Pā=ā3.4āĆā10ā5). Supplementary TableĀ S11 lists the environmental variables with loadings higher than 0.1 with the significant PCs. For the 2002/2003 data, the PC that significantly negatively predicted well-being was represented by four variables reflecting low-income neighbourhoods. For the 2009/2010 data, one of the PCs (PC1) was indicative of high income and livability, while the other PC (PC2) was indicative of low income and lower livability.
Analyses with socioeconomic status
Correcting for individual EA had a small effect on the observed associations. After also including the SES of the neighbourhood, only neighbourhood safety and the percentage of land devoted to greenhouse horticulture remained significantĀ (see SupplementaryĀ Table S8).
A summary of all analyses and their results can be found in Fig.Ā 2.
Discussion
The present study linked two large data-resources in the Netherlands in order to examine potential associations between well-being and a range of environmental factors. Using this environment-wide association approach, we identified 21 environmental factors that were associated with well-being. These factors cluster in the following domains: housing stock, income, core neighbourhood characteristics, livability, and SES. A common theme that emerged is that the identified correlates can be classified as socioeconomic indicators.
An examination of the correlations between these variables reveals that they are not independent. When correcting for individual and neighbourhood SES, only safety and % of land devoted to greenhouse horticulture were significantly associated with well-being, with safer neighbourhoods and neighbourhoods with more greenhouse horticulture showing higher average levels of well-being. A closer examination of the distribution of these two environmental variables in the Netherlands (Supplementary Figs. S4ā6) revealed that greenhouse horticulture did not show a lot of variation across the country, especially compared to the other associated variables (SES and safety). Therefore, this association should be interpreted with caution. Safety, on the other hand varies widely across the different postal codes. Earlier studies also found associations between psychological health and neighbourhood safety [38,39,40]. It is furthermore in line with previous research where well-being was linked to neighbourhood-level SES indicators [41, 42]. Moreover, similar results have been found for depression using GECCO data [43]. Importantly, what should be kept in mind when examining the results of this study is that we are examining associations, and not causal effects. For the identified associations, this means two things should be considered. First, there might be some third, mediating factor that explains the associations. Most of the factors assessed in the first round of EnWAS disappeared when we corrected for SES, already suggesting that SES was driving these associations. Secondly, even if there are potential causal associations, we cannot make any statement regarding the direction of the effect.
No effects of genetic differences were observed, indicated by the absence of significant genetic prediction. This indicates that either the genetic predisposition for well-being does not cause individuals to pick certain environments or that we suffer from a lack of power. Indeed, a post-hoc power analysis (Supplementary Fig.Ā 7) indicates that with our current sample size and alpha, we could have detected associations between the well-being polygenic scores and environmental exposures with effect sizes greater than R2ā=ā0.002. Thus, associations between the current PRS and the environmental exposures assessed here are likely extremely small. The well-being spectrum polygenic score explains less than a percentage of the variance in well-being itself, and there was no difference in mean well-being between different genetic susceptibility groups. This raises the question of whether a stronger PGS would lead to different results than presented here. Therefore, while any statement on this genetic component is speculative at this moment, we cannot write off the potential role that genetics play in these associations, and encourage future investigations in this area.
From the existing literature, we already knew that the effect of individual genetic variants on well-being is small [44]: 12-18% of the variance in well-being is explained by ~600k genome-wide measured SNPs for complex traits, with GWA study SNP-based heritability estimates around ~5% [27]. Here, we report small environmental effects on well-being. The significant environmental predictors from the EnWAS individually explain only 0.2% to 0.5% of the variance in well-being. In addition, the PCA showed that the combined effect of the EnWAS variables explains only around ~1% of the variance in well-being. Important to keep in mind while interpreting these effect sizes is the fact that we examined environmental exposures at the postal code level. It is likely that the well-being exposome varies over different geographical levels (e.g., cities, municipalities) [45], measures of well-being, and is differently associated with subjective indicators of the environment [46, 47]. Take as an example SES: studies examining the effect of individual-level SES on well-being find estimates as large as 6% explained variance [48, 49], which is much larger than our current finding for neighbourhood SES indicators.
Moreover, we did not, despite our large sample, find any evidence for many previously suggested indicators, such as the presence of green space [50] or air pollution [51]. Different reasons might explain this discrepancy: e.g., the level and country of examination (postal code level in the Netherlands), the use of objective indicators of the environment (instead of subjective experiences), and the measure of well-being we used. Therefore, our findings should be interpreted in the context of this study. Important, though, is that our study investigates the association between wellbeing and postal code linked variables, e.g., the amount of greenspace in the postal code area. That is a different approach than studying wellbeing in relation to frequency of visiting or enjoying greenspace. In order to develop a full picture of the well-being exposome, it is necessary to take these different aspects into account. Mapping the well-being exposome will also require investigations on different time-points or, optimally, longitudinal investigations tracking the dynamic interplay and direction of causality between environmental factors, biological factors and well-being [52]. For consistency, we decided to assess each variable on the same geospatial scale (PC-4 level). However, this level is likely not the most relevant level for each assessed exposure variable. The methodology used in this project can easily be applied to different levels of analysis (e.g. individual level objective data, individual level subjective data, street level). In this way, we can compare EnWAS results on different levels, offering a replicable means of mapping the well-being exposome. What should additionally be kept in mind is that many studies focus their efforts on one or a few exposures at a time, limiting the potential to study such an exposure in a broader context. This study demonstrates the importance of large, data-driven explorations to get a more adequate image of these intertwined environmental associations.
In the genetics field, small effects are common and combined in polygenic scores that are used for more in-depth analyses. An interesting approach would be to combine environmental effects in āpoly-environmentalā scores. In this way, small environmental effects can be combined and used to predict well-being. An obstacle that needs to be overcome in order to construct these scores is that we need a better understanding of the correlational structure between different environmental factors. In case of polygenic scores, we can correct for correlations between genetic variants based on our knowledge of recombination patterns and linkage disequilibrium [35]. For poly-environmental scores, however, the association between different environmental factors is much more complex and dynamic. By combining small effects in poly-environmental scores, complemented by polygenic scores, it might in the future be possible to develop personalized prevention and intervention strategies for well-being. However, in addition to acquiring better knowledge of the correlational structure of the environment, this will also require more insight into the potential direction of causality of current findings. Another interesting direction for future research that aims to combine genetic and environmental effects is to compare the well-being of monozygotic twins that are exposed to different living environments. Since monozygotic twins are 100% genetically identical, a difference in well-being between the twins can only be caused by unique environmental experiences. Therefore, by associating monozygotic intra-pair difference scores for well-being with intra-pair difference scores for environmental exposure, it becomes possible to examine the extent to which an association between well-being and an environmental exposure exists independent from genetic and shared environmental factors. In our sample, there was a relatively low number of complete monozygotic twin pairs for which we could compute difference scores for both well-being and the environmental exposures (Npairs 2002/2003ā=ā389, Npairs 2009/2010ā=ā270). As a result, no evidence was found for an association between well-being intra-pair difference scores and any of the environmental exposure intra-pair difference scores (see Supplementary TableĀ S12). Therefore, we encourage other cohorts with larger samples to perform these analyses in order to get a grasp of the potential genetic effects.
To conclude, in this study we combined the strengths of record linkage to understand individual differences in well-being. Taken together, our analyses suggest that, at the postal-code level, the most important predictors of well-being are socioeconomic factors and safety. Moreover, we find that environmental effects are typically small and context dependent, emphasizing the need for large scale linkage efforts and data-driven designs.
Data availability
The Netherlands Twin Register cohort data may be accessed through the Netherlands Twin Register (ntr.fgb@vu.nl) upon approval of the data access committee. The environmental exposure data may be accessed through the Geoscience and Health Cohort Consortium (gecco@vumc.nl) upon approval of the data access committee.
References
Fan P, Ouyang Z, Nguyen DD, Nguyen TTH, Park H, Chen J. Urbanization, economic development, environmental and social changes in transitional economies: Vietnam after Doimoi. Landsc Urban Plan. 2019;187:145ā55.
Liang W, Yang M. Urbanization, economic growth and environmental pollution: evidence from China. Sustain Comput Inform Syst. 2019;21:1ā9.
Gamm L, Stone S, Pittman S. Mental health and mental disordersāA rural challenge: a literature review. Rural Heal people. 2010;2:97ā113.
Srivastava K. Urbanization and mental health. Ind Psychiatry J. 2009;18:75ā76.
Peen J, Dekker J, Schoevers RA, ten Have M, Graaf R, Beekman AT. Is the prevalence of psychiatric disorders associated with urbanization? Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2007;42:984ā9.
Alcock I, White MP, Wheeler BW, Fleming LE, Depledge MH. Longitudinal effects on mental health of moving to greener and less green urban areas. Environ Sci Technol. 2014;48:1247ā55.
Lederbogen F, Kirsch P, Haddad L, Streit F, Tost H, Schuch P, et al. City living and urban upbringing affect neural social stress processing in humans. Nature. 2011;474:498ā501.
Chen J, Chen S. Mental health effects of perceived living environment and neighborhood safety in urbanizing China. Habitat Int. 2015;46:101ā10.
Whitley R, Prince M. Fear of crime, mobility and mental health in inner-city London, UK. Soc Sci Med. 2005;61:1678ā88.
Colodro-Conde L, Couvy-Duchesne B, Whitfield JB, Streit F, Gordon S, Kemper KE et al. Association between population density and genetic risk for schizophrenia. In: JAMA Psychiatry. American Medical Association, 2018, pp 901-10.
Sudlow C, Gallacher J, Allen N, Beral V, Burton P, Danesh J, et al. UK biobank: an open access resource for identifying the causes of a wide range of complex diseases of middle and old age. PLOS Med. 2015;12:e1001779.
Howard DM, Adams MJ, Shirali M, Clarke TK, Marioni RE, Davies G, et al. Genome-wide association study of depression phenotypes in UK Biobank identifies variants in excitatory synaptic pathways. Nat Commun. 2018;9:1470.
Shen X, Reus LM, Cox SR, Adams MJ, Liewald DC, Bastin ME, et al. Subcortical volume and white matter integrity abnormalities in major depressive disorder: findings from UK Biobank imaging data. Sci Rep. 2017;7:1ā10.
Bakker BFM, Van Rooijen J, Van Toor L. The system of social statistical datasets of statistics netherlands: an integral approach to the production of register-based social statistics. Stat J IAOS. 2014;30:411ā24.
De Bruin A, Kardaun J, Gast F, De Bruin E, Van Sijl M, Verweij G. Record linkage of hospital discharge register with population register: experiences at Statistics Netherlands. Stat J UN Econ Comm Eur. 2004;21:23ā32.
Timmermans EJ, Lakerveld J, Beulens JWJ, Boomsma DI, Kramer SE, Oosterman M, et al. Cohort profile: the Geoscience and Health Cohort Consortium (GECCO) in the Netherlands. BMJ Open. 2018;8:e021597.
Ligthart L, van Beijsterveldt CEM, Kevenaar ST, de Zeeuw E, van Bergen E, Bruins S et al. The Netherlands twin register: longitudinal research based on twin and twin-family designs. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2019;22:623ā36.
Penninx BWJH, Beekman ATF, Smit JH, Zitman FG, Nolen WA, Spinhoven P, et al. The Netherlands study of depression and anxiety (NESDA): rationale, objectives and methods. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2008;17:121ā40.
Pinquart M, Sƶrensen S. Influences of socioeconomic status, social network, and competence on subjective well-being in later life: a meta-analysis. Psychol Aging. 2000;15:187ā224.
Dolan P, Peasgood T, White M. Do we really know what makes us happy? A review of the economic literature on the factors associated with subjective well-being. J Econ Psychol. 2008;29:94ā122.
Helliwell JF, Putnam RD. The social context of well-being. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2004;359:1435ā46.
Patel CJ, Ioannidis JPA. Studying the elusive environment in large scale. JAMA - J Am Med Assoc 2014;311:2173ā4.
Ni MY, Yao XI, Chueng F, Wu JT, Schooling CM, Pang H, et al. Determinants of physical, mental and social well-being: a longitudinal environment-wide association study. Int J Epidemiol. 2020;49:380ā9.
Abdellaoui A, Hugh-Jones D, Yengo L, Kemper KE, Nivard MG, Veul L, et al. Genetic correlates of social stratification in Great Britain. Nat Hum Behav. 2019;3:1332ā42.
Abdellaoui A, Hottenga J-J, de Knijff P, Nivard MG, Xiao X, Scheet P, et al. Population structure, migration, and diversifying selection in the Netherlands. Eur J Hum Genet. 2013;21:1277ā85.
Bartels M. Genetics of wellbeing and its components satisfaction with life, happiness, and quality of life: a review and meta-analysis of heritability studies. Behav Genet. 2015;45:137ā56.
Okbay A, Baselmans BML, De Neve J-E, Turley P, Nivard MG, Fontana MA, et al. Genetic variants associated with subjective well-being, depressive symptoms and neuroticism identified through genome-wide analyses. Nat Genet. 2016;48:624ā33.
Ioannidis JPA, Loy EY, Poulton R, Chia KS Researching genetic versus nongenetic determinants of disease: A comparison and proposed unification. Sci Transl Med. 2009; 1. https://doi.org/10.1126/scitranslmed.3000247.
Willemsen G, Vink JM, Abdellaoui A, den Braber A, van Beek JHDA, Draisma HHM, et al. The adult Netherlands twin register: twenty-five years of survey and biological data collection. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2013;16:271ā81.
Diener E, Emmons RA, Larsen RJ, Griffin S. The satisfaction with life scale. J Pers Assess. 1985;49:71ā75.
Delle Fave A, Brdar I, Freire T, Vella-Brodrick D, Wissing MP. The eudaimonic and hedonic components of happiness: qualitative and quantitative findings. Soc Indic Res. 2011;100:185ā207.
Consortium TG of the N, Francioli LC, Menelaou A, Pulit SL, Dijk Fvan, Palamara PF, et al. Whole-genome sequence variation, population structure and demographic history of the Dutch population. Nat Genet. 2014;46:818ā25.
MinicÉ CC, Boomsma DI, Vink JM, Dolan CV. MZ twin pairs or MZ singletons in population family-based GWAS? More power in pairs. Mol Psychiatry 2014;19:1154ā5.
Baselmans BML, Jansen R, Ip HF, van Dongen J, Abdellaoui A, van de Weijer MP, et al. Multivariate genome-wide analyses of the well-being spectrum. Nat Genet. 2019;51:445ā51.
VilhjĆ”lmsson BJ, Yang J, Finucane HK, Gusev A, Lindstrƶm S, Ripke S, et al. Modeling linkage disequilibrium increases accuracy of polygenic risk scores. Am J Hum Genet. 2015;97:576ā92.
Purcell S, Neale B, Todd-Brown K, Thomas L, Ferreira MAR, Bender D, et al. PLINK: a tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses. Am J Hum Genet. 2007;81:559ā75.
Gu Z, Gu L, Eils R, Schlesner M, Brors B. Circlize implements and enhances circular visualization in R | Bioinformatics | Oxford Academic. Bioinformatics. 2014;30:2811ā2.
Choi YJ, Matz-Costa C. Perceived neighborhood safety, social cohesion, and psychological health of older adults. Gerontologist. 2018;58:196ā206.
Wilson-Genderson M, Pruchno R. Effects of neighborhood violence and perceptions of neighborhood safety on depressive symptoms of older adults. Soc Sci Med. 2013;85:43ā49.
Cheng Z, Smyth R. Crime victimization, neighborhood safety and happiness in China. Econ Model. 2015;51:424ā35.
Ludwig J, Duncan GJ, Gennetian LA, Katz LF, Kessler RC, Kling JR, et al. Neighborhood effects on the long-term well-being of low-income adults. Science. 2012;337:1505ā10.
Dittmann J, Goebel J. Your house, your car, your education: the socioeconomic situation of the neighborhood and its impact on life satisfaction in Germany. Soc Indic Res. 2010;96:497ā513.
Generaal E, Timmermans EJ, Dekkers JEC, Smit JH, Penninx BWJH. Not urbanization level but socioeconomic, physical and social neighbourhood characteristics are associated with presence and severity of depressive and anxiety disorders. Psychol Med. 2019;49:149ā61.
Rietveld CA, Cesarini D, Benjamin DJ, Koellinger PD, De Neve J-E, Tiemeier H, et al. Molecular genetics and subjective well-being. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2013;110:9692ā7.
van Kamp I, Leidelmeijer K, Marsman G, de Hollander A. Urban environmental quality and human well-being: towards a conceptual framework and demarcation of concepts; a literature study. Landsc Urban Plan. 2003;65:5ā18.
Liao PS, Shaw D, Lin YM. Environmental quality and life satisfaction: subjective versus objective measures of air quality. Soc Indic Res. 2015;124:599ā616.
Zeidner M, Shechter M. Psychological responses to air pollution: some personality and demographic correlates. J Environ Psychol. 1988;8:191ā208.
Barger SD, Donoho CJ, Wayment HA. The relative contributions of race/ethnicity, socioeconomic status, health, and social relationships to life satisfaction in the United States. Qual Life Res. 2009;18:179ā89.
Navarro-Carrillo G, Alonso-Ferres M, Moya M, Valor-Segura I. Socioeconomic status and psychological well-being: revisiting the role of subjective socioeconomic status. Front Psychol. 2020;11:1303.
McMahan EA, Estes D. The effect of contact with natural environments on positive and negative affect: a meta-analysis. J Posit Psychol. 2015;10:507ā19.
Welsch H. Environment and happiness: valuation of air pollution using life satisfaction data. Ecol Econ. 2006;58:801ā13.
Dennis KK, Auerbach SS, Balshaw DM, Cui Y, Fallin MD, Smith MT, et al. The importance of the biological impact of exposure to the concept of the exposome. Environ Health Perspect 2016;124:1504ā10.
Acknowledgements
GECCO, Geoscience and Health Cohort Consortium; is acknowledged for gathering and combining existing data into the GECCO repository, and maintaining the infrastructure necessary for these data. The following people contributed to the GECCO consortium. The views presented here may not reflect the opinions of the individuals listed below, except for those who are listed as co-author (G.W). Amsterdam UMC: Jeroen Lakerveld, Brenda Penninx, Joline Beulens, Erik Timmermans, Martijn Huisman, Alfred Wagtendonk, Sophia Kramer. VU: Dorret Boomsma, Gonneke Willemsen, Carlo Schuengel, Mirjam Oosterman. AMC: Karien Stronks. Global Geo Health Data Centre: Derek Karssenberg, Ilonca Vaartjes, Roel Vermeulen. Maastricht University: Annemarie Koster, Coen Stehouwer. Sanquin: Katja van den Hurk. Spatial Information Laboratory VU Amsterdam: Eric Koomen. Leiden University Medical Centre: RenƩe de Mutsert. Trimbos Institute: Margreet ten Have. The Netherlands Institute for Public Health and Environment: Monique Verschuren, Susan Picavet. Erasmus University Amsterdam: Frank van Lenthe, Mariƫlle Beenackers, Arfan Ikram, Oscar Franco, Vincent Jaddoe. University Medical Centre Groningen: Tineke Oldehinkel. Lifelines: Trynke de Jong, Saakje Mulder & Aafje Dotinga.
Funding
This work was supported by the European Research Council (ERC-COG WELL-BEING [grant number 771057 to MB], NWO Middelgroot [911-09-032], NWO-Groot [480- 15-001/674], the Avera Institute for Human Genetics, Sioux Falls, South Dakota (USA) and the National Institutes of Health (NIH, [R01D0042157ā01A]), ZonMw Rubicon [grant number 45219101 to BMLB]. Part of the genotyping was funded by the Genetic Association Information Network (GAIN) of the Foundation for the US National Institutes of Health (NIMH, MH081802) and by the Grand Opportunity grants [1RC2MH089951ā01] and 1RC2 [MH089995ā01] from the NIMH. Survey 6: This study was supported by The Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO-MW [904-61-193], NWO [575-25- 006]) Survey 8: Supported by the Addiction programme of ZonMW [31160008]: Genetic determinants of risk behaviour in relation to alcohol use and alcohol use disorder. 1. The Geoscience and Health Cohort Consortium (GECCO) was financially supported by the EMGO Institute for Health and Care Research of Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam and VU University Medical Centre in Amsterdam, the Netherlands.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Ethics approval statement
All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. This article does not contain any studies with animals performed by any of the authors. Ethical approval was provided by (1) the Ethical Review Board (VCWE) of the Faculty of Behaviour and Movement Sciences of the VU University Medical Centre, Amsterdam and, (2) The European Research Council Executive Agency Screening Ethics Panel. Written informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.
Additional information
Publisherās note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the articleās Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the articleās Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
van de Weijer, M.P., Baselmans, B.M.L., Hottenga, JJ. et al. Expanding the environmental scope: an environment-wide association study for mental well-being. J Expo Sci Environ Epidemiol 32, 195ā204 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41370-021-00346-0
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41370-021-00346-0
Keywords
- Well-being, Environment, Environment-wide association, Polygenic score, Safety, Socioeconomic status