Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Review Article
  • Published:

'Noisy patients'—can signal detection theory help?

Abstract

Signal detection theory tests an observer's ability to discriminate between signal and noise. Deciding whether or not a patient's symptoms warrant further investigation or treatment is an example of this task in the clinical setting. Noise can exist within the observer—for example, in the brain of a tired or inexperienced doctor—or can arise from an external source such as the patient. Patients can produce external noise by giving numerous unrelated presenting complaints, providing overly detailed accounts of their symptoms, or simply talking too quickly. The more noise that is present, the harder the signal (such as a new disease or a notable change in an old condition) is to detect. Patients in the neurology clinic seem to be 'noisier' than average, perhaps owing to the long duration of their condition in many cases and the relatively high proportion of patients with medically unexplained symptoms. The ability to interpret such 'noisy' histories often underpins the neurological diagnosis. This Review aims to promote the relevance of signal detection theory to the overworked neurologist on the ward or in the clinic and explores strategies to reduce the noise generated both within the brain of the doctor and by patients.

Key Points

  • Signal detection theory offers a powerful model to explain decision-making processes in the laboratory, and it can also be applied to the setting of clinical consultation

  • Signal becomes harder to distinguish from noise when we are tired or trying to hold too much information in our working memory

  • The way in which patients present information might make important symptoms harder to distinguish from less clinically relevant ones

  • Patients in neurology might be 'noisier' than average, owing to the often long-standing duration of their condition and the greater proportion that have medically unexplained symptoms

  • Strategies to reduce noise include writing down key facts as they emerge, getting patients to self-filter their symptoms, and asking the patients to focus on one presenting complaint at a time

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: Calculation of d-prime.
Figure 2: The criterion.
Figure 3: The effect of shifts in the criterion.
Figure 4: The receiver operating characteristic curve for different values of d-prime.
Figure 5: The merging of criteria in a multi-stimulus environment.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Heeger D (1997) Signal detection theory [http://www.cns.nyu.edu/~david/sdt/sdt.html]

  2. Chavanet P et al. (2006) Performance of a predictive rule to distinguish bacterial and viral meningitis. J Infect 54: 328–336

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. McFall RM and Treat TA (1999) Quantifying the information value of clinical assessments with signal detection theory. Annu Rev Psychol 50: 215–241

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Green M and Swets J (1966) Signal Detection and Psychophysics. New York: John Wiley & Sons

    Google Scholar 

  5. Théberge I et al. (2005) Volume of screening mammography and performance in the Quebec population-based Breast Cancer Screening Program. CMAJ 172: 195–199

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Schriger D et al. (1998) Cranial computed tomography interpretation in acute stroke. JAMA 279: 1293–1296

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Chimowitz MI et al. (1990) The accuracy of bedside neurological diagnoses. Ann Neurol 28: 78–85

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Morton C and Mackie R (1998) Clinical accuracy of the diagnosis of cutaneous malignant melanoma. Br J Dermatol 138: 283–287

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Weideman RA et al. (1999) Pharmacist recognition of potential drug interactions. Am J Health Syst Pharm 56: 1524–1529

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. See JE et al. (1995) Meta-analysis of the sensitivity decrement in vigilance. Psychol Bull 117: 230–249

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Tucker P et al. (2003) Rest breaks and accident risk. Lancet 361: 680

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Parasuranam R and Davies D (1977) A taxonomic analysis of vigilance performance. In Vigilance: Theory, Operational Performance, and Psychological Correlates, 559–574 (Ed Mackie RR) New York: Plenium

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  13. Koelega H et al. (1989) Processing demands, effort, and individual differences in four different vigilance tasks. Hum Factors 31: 45–62

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Garg A et al. (2005) Effects of computerized clinical decision support systems on practitioner performance and patient outcomes—a systematic review. JAMA 293: 1223–1238

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Kawamoto K et al. (2005) Improving clinical practice using clinical decision support systems: a systematic review of trials to identify features critical to success. BMJ 330: 765–772

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Ji D and Wilson MA (2007) Coordinated memory replay in the visual cortex and hippocampus during sleep. Nat Neurosci 10: 100–107

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Walker MP and Stickgold R (2004) Sleep-dependent learning and memory consolidation. Neuron 44: 121–133

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Friedman RC et al. (1971) The intern and sleep loss. N Engl J Med 285: 201–203

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Horne JA et al. (1983) Effects of sleep deprivation on signal detection measures of vigilance: implications for sleep function. Sleep 6: 347–358

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Blagrove M et al. (1995) The effects of chronic sleep reduction on the performance of cognitive tasks sensitive to sleep deprivation. Appl Cogn Psychol 9: 21–40

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Frey R et al. (2002) Effect of rest on physicians' performance in an emergency department, objectified by electroencephalographic analyses and psychometric tests. Crit Care Med 30: 2322–2329

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Philibert I (2005) Sleep loss and performance in residents and nonphysicians: a meta-analytic examination. Sleep 28: 1351–1353

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Firth-Cozens J and Cording H (2004) What matters more in patient care? Giving doctors shorter hours of work or a good night's sleep? Qual Saf Health Care 13: 165–166

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Fletcher KE et al. (2004) Systematic review: effects of resident work hours on patient safety. Ann Intern Med 141: 851–857

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Benowitz NL (1990) Clinical pharmacology of caffeine. Annu Rev Med 41: 277–288

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. Battig K and Buzzi R (1986) Effect of coffee on the speed of subject-paced information processing. Neuropsychobiology 16: 126–130

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Frewer LJ and Lader M (1991) The effects of caffeine on two computerized tests of attention and vigilance. Hum Psychopharmacol 6: 119–128

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  28. MayoClinic.com: tools for healthier lives: how much caffeine is in your daily habit? [https://www.mayoclinic.com/health/caffeine/AN01211]

  29. Angelucci ME et al. (1999) The effect of caffeine in animal models of learning and memory. Eur J Pharmacol 373: 135–140

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. Angelucci ME et al. (2002) Effects of caffeine on learning and memory in rats tested in the Morris water maze. Braz J Med Biol Res 35: 1201–1208

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. Foulds J et al. (1996) Cognitive performance effects of subcutaneous nicotine in smokers and never-smokers. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 127: 31–38

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Pashler HE (1998) The Psychology of Attention. Cambridge: MIT Press

    Google Scholar 

  33. Gorea A and Sagi D (2000) Failure to handle more than one internal representation in visual detection tasks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 97: 12380–12384

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Gorea A and Sagi D (2002) Natural extinction: a criterion shift phenomenon. Vis Cogn 9: 913–936

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Speier C et al. (1997) The effects of task interruption and information presentation on individual decision making. In Proceedings of the 18th International Conference on Information Systems: 1997 December 14–17; Atlanta, 21–35 (Eds. DeGross JI and Kumar K) New York: Association for Computing Machinery

    Google Scholar 

  36. Seymour J et al. (1995) Response of headaches to nonnarcotic analgesics resulting in missed intracranial hemorrhage. Am J Emerg Med 13: 43–45

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  37. Reuber M et al. (2004) Failure to recognize psychogenic nonepileptic seizures may cause death. Neurology 62: 834–835

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  38. Gander P et al. (2007) Work patterns and fatigue related-risk among junior doctors. Occup Environ Med 64: 733–738

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Smit AS et al. (2004) Mental effort causes vigilance decrease due to resource depletion. Acta Psychol (Amst) 115: 35–42

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Neuchterlein KH et al. (1983) Visual sustained attention: image degradation produces rapid sensitivity decrement over time. Science 220: 327–329

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Sarter M et al. (2001) The cognitive neuroscience of sustained attention: where top-down meets bottom-up. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 35: 146–160

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  42. Jones JW et al. (1988) Stress and medical malpractice: organizational risk assessment and intervention. J Appl Psychol 4: 727–735

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Firth-Cozens J (2001) Interventions to improve physicians' well-being and patient care. Soc Sci Med 52: 215–222

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  44. Schon F et al. (2002) Is clinical neurology really so difficult? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 72: 557–559

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  45. Dacre J et al. (2003) MRCP(UK) Part 2 Clinical Examination (PACES): a review of the first four examination sessions (June 2001–July 2002). Clin Med 3: 452–459

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Nahm FKD (2001) Neurology, technology, and the diagnostic imperative. Perspect Biol Med 44: 99–107

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  47. Caplan L and Hollander J (2001) The Effective Clinical Neurologist, edn 2. Oxford: Butterworth-Heinemann

    Google Scholar 

  48. Dubos F et al. (2006) Clinical decision rules to distinguish between bacterial and aseptic meningitis. Arch Dis Child 91: 647–650

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  49. Swets JA et al. (1979) Assessment of diagnostic technologies. Science 205: 753–759

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  50. Reuber M et al. (2005) Functional symptoms in neurology: questions and answers. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 76: 307–314

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  51. Nimnuan C et al. (2001) Medically unexplained symptoms: an epidemiological study in seven specialities. J Psychosom Res 51: 361–367

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  52. Scrag A et al. (2004) Reliability of self-reported diagnoses in patients with neurologically unexplained symptoms. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 75: 608–611

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Ridsdale L et al. Royal College of General Practitioners Curriculum Statement 15.7: Neurological Problems [http://www.rcgp-curriculum.org.uk/PDF/curr_15_7_Neurological_problems.pdf] (accessed 29 February 2008)

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rupert Oliver.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Box 1

The ability of neurologists, radiologists and emergency physicians to detect infarction or hemorrhage on cranial CT. (DOC 55 kb)

Supplementary Box 2

The ability of clinicians to detect malignant melanoma. (DOC 24 kb)

Supplementary Box 3

Signal detection: clinical observation versus radiological investigation. (DOC 26 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Oliver, R., Bjoertomt, O., Greenwood, R. et al. 'Noisy patients'—can signal detection theory help?. Nat Rev Neurol 4, 306–316 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1038/ncpneuro0794

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/ncpneuro0794

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing