Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Letter
  • Published:

Seeing motion behind occluders

Abstract

THE visual system has no difficulty maintaining the identity of an object as it disappears and reappears behind stationary occluders. In the natural world, a moving object may differ from occluders by many characteristics (colour, depth, shape and so on). Scene segmentation based on these characteristics is thought to happen early in visual processing, and to influence how objects, including moving objects, are identified1–5. What happens if the only characteristic distinguishing an object is its direction of motion? Experiments with random dot displays show that one dot moving in a constant trajectory is readily detected among identical dots in brownian motion6. Detection declines sharply if the trajectory is intermittently broken, but improves if occluders obscure the breaks in the trajectory. It is not sufficient that these occluders be perceived as segmented from the rest of the display (such as by colour or depth). Rather, it is critical that the occluders do not contain motion that is similar in direction to that of the target trajectory. We conclude that detection of the trajectory is due to the integration of information within a network of low-level motion detectors and is not dependent on segmentation processes7.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Nakayama, K. & Shimojo, S. Vision Res. 30, 1811–1825 (1990).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Shimojo, S., Silverman, G. H. & Nakayama, K. Nature 333, 265–268 (1988).

    Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. He, Z. L. & Nakayama, K. Nature 367, 173–175 (1994).

    Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. He, Z. L. & Nakayama, K. Nature 359, 231–233 (1992).

    Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Braddick, O. J. Nature 333, 803–804 (1988).

    Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Watamaniuk, S. N. J., McKee, S. P. & Grzywacz, N. M. Vision Res. 35, 65–77 (1995).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Yuille, A. L. & Grzywacz, N. M. Nature 333, 71–74 (1988).

    Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Grzywacz, N. M., Smith, J. A. & Yuille, A. L. in The Proceedings of the IEEE Workshop on Visual Motion 148–155 (IEEE Computer Society, 1989).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  9. Grzywacz, N. M., Watamaniuk, S. N. J. & McKee, S. P. Vision Res. 325 (in the press).

  10. Shipley, T. F. & Kellman, P. J. Spatial Vis. 7, 323–339 (1993).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Bowne, S. F., McKee, S. P. & Glaser, D. A. J. opt. Soc. Am. A 6, 1112–1121 (1989).

    Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Watamaniuk, S., McKee, S. Seeing motion behind occluders. Nature 377, 729–730 (1995). https://doi.org/10.1038/377729a0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/377729a0

This article is cited by

Comments

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing