Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Original Communication
  • Published:

Salivary cortisol and heart rate in stunted and nonstunted Nepalese school children

Abstract

Objective: To test the hypothesis that stunted Nepalese children have an altered stress response system when compared with matched nonstunted children in response to a battery of psychological tests.

Design: Case–control study.

Setting: Poor urban areas of Kathmandu, Nepal.

Subjects: A total of 64 stunted (less than –2 s.d. height-for-age) children compared with 64 nonstunted (> −1s.d. height-for-age) schoolchildren between 8 and 10 y old matched for school and sex.

Methods: A psychological test session was administered, which included mental arithmetic and two tests of working memory. Salivary cortisol samples were obtained at five points during testing, and heart rate was measured during testing and also at baseline. Salivary cortisol samples were also obtained once early in the morning. Hemoglobin was assessed at the testing session, and extensive data were obtained on the social background of the children's families.

Results: Stunted Nepalese children showed a blunted physiologic response (salivary cortisol and heart rate) to psychological stressors (P<0.05) when compared with nonstunted children, but were not different from the nonstunted children in baseline measures, when controlling for social background. The two groups were not different in terms of social background.

Conclusions: These findings suggest that childhood growth retardation may be associated with changes in physiological arousal, and that stunting could be associated with hyporesponsivity in response to psychological stress.

Sponsorship: DFID (The Department for International Development, UK).

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1
Figure 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Armitage P & Berry G (1994): Statistical Methods in Medical Research. Oxford: Blackwell Science Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyce WT, Adams S, Tschann JM, Cohen F, Wara D & Gunnar MR (1995): Adrenocortical and behavioral predictors of immune responses to starting school. Pediatr. Res. 38, 1009–1017.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brandtstadter J, Baltes-Gotz B, Kirschbaum C & Hellhammer D (1991): Developmental and personality correlates of adrenocortical activity as indexed by salivary cortisol: observations in the age range of 35–65 years. J. Psychosom. Res. 35, 173–185.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Clark PM (1998): Programming of the hypothalamo-pituitary–adrenal axis and fetal origins of adult disease hypothesis. Eur. J. Pediatr. 157 (suppl 1), S7–S10.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Desautes C, Bidanel JP & Mormede P (1997): Genetic study of behavioral and pituitary–adrenocortical reactivity in response to an environmental challenge in pigs. Physiol. Behav. 62, 337–345.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Feitosa MF, Rice T, Rosmond R, Borecki IB, An P, Gagnon J, Leon AS, Skinner JS, Wilmore JH, Bouchard C & Rao DC (2002): A genetic study of cortisol measured before and after endurance training: the HERITAGE Family Study. Metabolism 51, 360–365.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fernald LC & Grantham McGregor SM (2002): Growth retardation is associated with changes in the stress response system and behavior in school-aged Jamaican children. J. Nutr. 32, 3674–3679.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fernald LC & Grantham-McGregor SM (1998): Stress response in children who have experienced childhood growth retardation. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 68, 691–698.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Flinn MV & England BG (1997): Social economics of childhood glucocorticoid stress response and health. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 102, 33–53.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein IB & Shapiro D (1995): The cardiovascular response to postural change as a function of race. Biol. Psychol. 39, 173–186.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Grantham McGregor S & Fernald LC (1999): Effects of health and nutrition on cognitive and behavioral development in children in the first three years of life. Part 1: Small for gestational age, breastfeeding and protein-energy malnutrition. Food Nutr. Bull. 20, 53–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gunnar MR, Morison SJ, Chisholm K & Schuder M (2001): Salivary cortisol levels in children adopted from Romanian orphanages. Dev. Psychopathol. 13, 611–628.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gunnar MR & Vasquez DM (2001): Low cortisol and a flattening of expected daytime rhythm: potential indices of risk in human development. Dev. Psychopathol. 13, 515–538.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gunnar MR & Nelson CA (1994): Event-related potentials in year-old infants: relations with emotionality and cortisol. Child Dev. 65: 80–94.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hamill PV, Drizd TA, Johnson CL, Reed RB & Roche AF (1977): NCHS growth curves for children birth–18 years. United States. Vital Health Stat. 11, i-iv, 1–74.

  • Heim C, Ehlert U & Hellhammer DH (2000): The potential role of hypocortisolism in the pathophysiology of stress-related bodily disorders. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 25, 1–35.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kagan JJ, Reznick S & Snidman N (1988): Biological bases of childhood shyness. Science 240, 167–171.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman AS & Lichtenberger EO (2000): Essentials of WISC-II and WPPSI-R assessment. Psychol Assessment Resources.

  • Kiess W, Meidert A, Dressendorfer RA, Schriever K, Kessler U, Konig A, Schwarz HP & Strasburger CJ (1995): Salivary cortisol levels throughout childhood and adolescence: relation with age, pubertal stage, and weight. Pediatr. Res. 37, 502–506.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kirschbaum C & Hellhammer DH (1994): Salivary cortisol in psychoneuroendocrine research: recent developments and applications. Psychoneuroendocrinology 19, 313–333.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kirschbaum C, Strasburger CJ, Jammers W & Hellhammer DH (1989): Cortisol and behavior: 1. Adaptation of a radioimmunoassay kit for reliable and inexpensive salivary cortisol determination. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 34, 747–751.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kirschbaum C, Wust S & Hellhammer D (1992): Consistent sex differences in cortisol responses to psychological stress. Psychosom. Med. 54, 648–657.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lohman TG, Roche AF & Martorell R (1989): Anthropometric Standardization Reference Manual. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lupien SJ, King S, Meaney MJ & McEwen BS (2000): Child's stress hormone levels correlate with mother's socioeconomic status and depressive state. Biol. Psychiatry. 48, 976–980.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lupien SJ, King S, Meaney MJ & McEwen BS (2001): Can poverty get under your skin? Basal cortisol levels and cognitive function in children from low and high socioeconomic status. Dev. Psychopathol. 13, 653–676.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meaney MJ, Tannebaum B, Francis D, Bhatnagar S, Shanks N, Viau V, O'Donnell D, Plotsky PM . et al (1994): Early environmental programming of hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal responses to stress. Semin. Neurosci. 6, 247–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mendez M & Adair L (1999): Severity and timing of stunting in the first two years of life affect performance on cognitive tests in late childhood. J. Nutr. 129, 1555–1562.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy JK, Alpert BS, Walker SS & Willey ES (1991): Children's cardiovascular reactivity: stability of racial differences and relation to subsequent blood pressure over a one-year period. Psychophysiology 28, 447–457.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Musante L, Treiber FA, Davis H, Levy M & Strong WB (1995): Temporal stability of children's cardiovascular (CV) reactivity: role of ethnicity, gender and family history of myocardial infarction. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 19, 281–286.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Panter-Brick C, Todd A, Baker R & Worthman C (1996): Comparative study of flex heart rate in three samples of Nepali boys. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 8, 653–660.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips DIW (2001): Fetal growth and programming of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis. Clin. Exp. Pharmacol. Physiol. 28, 967–970.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pruessner JC, Wolf OT, Hellhammer DH, Buske Kirschbaum A, von Auer K, Jobst S, Kaspers F & Kirschbaum C (1997): Free cortisol levels after awakening: a reliable biological marker for the assessment of adrenocortical activity. Life Sci. 61, 2539–2549.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sanchéz MM, Aguado F, Sánchez-Toscano F & Saphier D (1998): Neuroendocrine and immunocytochemical demonstrations of decreased hypothalamo–pituitary–adrenal axis responsiveness to restraint stress after long-term isolation. Endocrinology 139, 579–587.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanchéz MM, Ladd CO & Plotsky PM (2001): Early adverse experience as a developmental risk factor for later psychopathology: evidence from rodent and primate models. Dev. Psychopathol. 13, 419–449.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smart JL, Whatson TS & Dobbing J (1975): Thresholds of response to electric shock in previously undernourished rats. Br. J. Nutr. 34, 511–516.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Smith A (1973): Symbol Digit Modalities Test Manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strupp BJ & Levitsky DA (1995): Enduring cognitive effects of early malnutrition: a theoretical reappraisal. J. Nutr. 125, 2221S–2232S.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • UNICEF (2002): The State of the World's Children 2002. New York: Oxford University Press.

  • Worthman C & Panter-Brick C (1996): Street versus home, urban versus rural, disadvantaged versus secure: contrasting lifestyles and well-being status of Nepali children. Paper Presented at the Symposium on Child Stress in Natural Settings: Psychobiological Studies of Children in Diverse Cultures, Biannual Meetings of the ISSBD, Quebec City, August 15, 1996.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to Purna Shrestha and Bimala Manadhar at the Mother Infant Research Activities in Kathmandu, Nepal, for their support and assistance, as well as Kamala Manadhar, Ganga Ranjitkar, Sharmila Karki and Sheela Sainju who tested all of the children. Thanks are also due to David Osrin, Wendy Morris, Suzanne Filteau, Andrew Tomkins and Juana Willumsen, in addition to the anonymous reviewers of this journal, and to all of the children and families for their participation in this research. The work was undertaken by the Institute of Child Health and Great Ormond Street Hospital for Sick Children NHS Trust, who receive a proportion of their funding from the NHS Executive.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to L C Fernald.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Fernald, L., Grantham-McGregor, S., Manandhar, D. et al. Salivary cortisol and heart rate in stunted and nonstunted Nepalese school children. Eur J Clin Nutr 57, 1458–1465 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.ejcn.1601710

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.ejcn.1601710

Keywords

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links