Skip to main content

Dopamine: The Currency of Desire

Rethinking the “pleasure molecule” could help scientists better understand addiction, Parkinson’s disease and motivation 

Guyco

When Stephanie,

* a 34-year-old middle manager from the Midwest, began to overeat, it was not typical behavior for her. She had never before had trouble with her weight. But in 2009 she started to feel an overwhelming hunger—particularly for sugar—and was soon rapidly piling on pounds.

Then just as abruptly, Stephanie developed an obsession with gambling. She would play games of chance online. “I'd never bought a lottery ticket in my life,” she recalls. Yet she began to do so compulsively: “I would go from gas station to gas station. I had places that in my mind I thought were better places to win.”


On supporting science journalism

If you're enjoying this article, consider supporting our award-winning journalism by subscribing. By purchasing a subscription you are helping to ensure the future of impactful stories about the discoveries and ideas shaping our world today.


Stephanie did not realize it until years later, but her behaviors could all be explained as side effects from a medication called Mirapex (pramipexole). Her doctor had prescribed it to treat Parkinson's disease, with which she had been diagnosed at the unusually young age of 29. Parkinson's is a neurodegenerative condition that causes tremor and movement difficulties. Although much about it remains mysterious, scientists know that the disease gradually destroys brain cells that produce the neurotransmitter dopamine. Mirapex acts, in a sense, as an artificial version of this molecule, activating the same receptors in the brain, which can help explain Stephanie's problems.

Although few of us spend time contemplating the molecular messengers at work in our brain, we owe a tremendous amount to them—and to dopamine in particular. It plays a part in movement, motivation, mood and memory. And as Stephanie's tale hints, it also has a dark side. The neurotransmitter is implicated in addiction, schizophrenia, hallucinations and paranoia. Yet dopamine is best known for its role in pleasure. In the popular press, dopamine is delight; the brain's code word for bliss; the stuff that makes psychoactive drugs dope. Articles and documentaries describe dopamine as what makes life worth living, the chemical that permits every enjoyable moment to be savored, the “hit” everyone is chasing whether through social media, psychoactive substances, sports, food, sex or status.

But it may be time to rethink these ideas. Nora Volkow, director of the National Institute on Drug Abuse and a longtime researcher of this neurotransmitter, would like to set the record straight: “Dopamine is not the pleasure molecule in the simple, direct way it is typically portrayed in the media,” she says. “Its function is much more nuanced.”

Today the precise nature of dopamine is a matter of much controversy. Some researchers argue that dopamine, when acting within what has become known as the brain's reward system, signals desire. Others claim that it helps the brain predict rewards and direct behavior accordingly. A third group splits the difference, saying both explanations can be valid. Ironically, if there is anything scientists now agree on about this neurotransmitter it is that dopamine does not neurologically define joy. Instead this little molecule may unlock the intricate mystery of what drives us.

Missing Motivation

In 1978 Roy Wise, then at Concordia University in Quebec, published a seminal paper on dopamine. He depleted levels of the neurotransmitter in rats with antipsychotic medications and found that the rats would stop working to receive yummy foods or desirable drugs such as amphetamine. The animals could still make the movements needed to obtain what they should have craved, which suggested that their behavior changed because the experience was no longer rewarding. Dopamine, at least when acting in a circuit located near the middle of the brain, seemed to be necessary for anything to feel good.

None

The Dope on Dopamine: Despite a long-standing association with pleasure, this neurotransmitter is linked to many other facets of experience.

Over the next decade, data in support of Wise's idea only grew. So when neuroscientist Kent C. Berridge began researching dopamine at the University of Michigan around that time, he believed, like most of his colleagues, that it was a “pleasure” signal. Berridge's own work was focused on facial expressions of pleasure, which are surprisingly congruent among mammals. Even rats will avidly lick their lips when they receive sweet food and open their mouth in disgust after encountering a bitter taste—as will human babies. Typically mammalian expressions of satisfaction intensify when, for example, a hungry rat receives an especially tasty treat or a thirsty rat finally drinks water. Berridge thought that studying and measuring these responses could further confirm the idea that dopamine means pleasure to the brain.

His colleague at the University of Michigan, neuroscientist Terry Robinson, had been using a neurotoxin to destroy dopamine neurons and create rats that modeled severe symptoms of Parkinson's. Berridge decided to give sweet foods to these rodents and see if they appeared pleased. He expected that their lack of dopamine would deny them this response. Because they were so dopamine-depleted, Robinson's rats rarely moved if left alone. They did not seek food and had to be fed artificially. “They wouldn't want to eat or drink anything,” Berridge says. Unexpectedly, however, their facial reactions were completely normal—they continued to lick their lips in response to something sweet and grimace at a bitter meal.

They tried again and again, but Berridge and his colleagues got the same results. When they conducted an experiment that basically created the opposite conditions—by ramping up dopamine levels in rats using electrodes implanted in appropriate regions—the rats did not lick their chops more eagerly when eating, as the “dopamine is pleasure” theory predicted. Indeed, sometimes the animals actually seemed less pleased when they scarfed down their sweets. Nevertheless, they kept eating far more voraciously than normal.

The researchers were puzzled. Instead of producing pleasure, dopamine seemed to drive desire. Desire itself can be enjoyable in small doses—but in the long run, if it is not satisfied, it is just the opposite. Eventually Berridge and Robinson realized that the pleasure involved in seeking a reward and that of actually obtaining it must be distinct. They labeled the drive that dopamine seemed to induce as “wanting” and called the joy of being satiated, which did not seem to be connected with dopamine, “liking.”

This dissociation fit with studies of Parkinson's patients. They are still, after all, able to enjoy life's ups but often have problems with motivation. Perhaps the most vivid example of this occurred in the early 20th century, when an epidemic of encephalitis lethargica left thousands of people with an especially severe parkinsonian condition. Their brains were so depleted of dopamine that they were unable to initiate movement and were essentially “frozen in place” like living statues. (The film Awakenings, which starred Robin Williams as neurologist Oliver Sacks, was based on the doctor's 1973 memoir of treating such patients.) But a sufficiently strong external stimulus could spark action for people with this condition. In one case cited by Sacks, a man who typically sat motionless in his wheelchair on the beach saw someone drowning. He jumped up, rescued the swimmer and then returned to his prior rigidly fixed position. One of Sacks's own patients would sit silent and still unless thrown several oranges, which she would then catch and juggle.

Mirapex, the drug that Stephanie took, is one of a class of medications known as dopamine agonists used to re-enable movement and motivation. Dopamine receptors perceive these drugs as the real thing and react accordingly. Consequently, the medications can offer excellent relief from tremors, rigidity and other movement problems. But the drugs can also have some destructive and distressing side effects.

Stephanie went from essentially not having enough motivation to having too much—or, at least, the motivation that the drug ignited was misdirected. In addition to overeating, her problems on dopamine agonists included gambling, an obsession with her iPhone, compulsive online shopping and intrusive sexual desires. Subjectively, the experiences she described were nearly identical to those reported by people with more typically caused addictions. In the case of lottery-ticket buying, it was like being programmed into an infinite loop. Whether she won or lost, there was always a need to do it again. As Arnie Wexler, a former compulsive gambler and author of a 2014 book on the subject called All Bets Are Off, puts it, “The difference between normal people and compulsive gamblers is we chase wins and we chase losses, and there's never enough to win and never enough to lose.”

Many people with addictions experience an escalation in desire that, similarly, is not accompanied by a similar increase in enjoyment. “The want, the desire, was so much that I never really thought about, ‘Did I end up liking it at the end?’ because it didn't matter,” Stephanie says. That experience beautifully describes what Berridge and Robinson call the “incentive sensitization” theory of dopamine action, which they introduced in 1993 and which has been bolstered by more recent studies.

None

Humans, orangutans and rats all express their delight in a sweet snack by licking their lips. This common code enabled researchers to assess animals' joy in dopamine experiments. Credit: © iStock.com (baby); Gerard Lacz Age Fotostock (orangutan); Oleg Kozlov Alamy (rat)

In 2005, for example, a research team at the Medical College of Wisconsin and the National Institute on Drug Abuse tracked the brain activity of eight people with an addiction to cocaine as they pushed a button to self-administer the substance. In line with Berridge's “wanting” theory, activity along dopamine pathways peaked just before button pushing.

What dopamine is “really doing,” Berridge says, “is taking things you encounter, little cues, things you smell and hear, and if they have a motivational significance, [it] can magnify that significance,” raising the incentive to pursue them. Placing dopamine directly into the nucleus accumbens of rats, he notes, will make them work two to three times harder to get what they crave, but it will not amplify the pleasurable experience of rewards once they are obtained.

Prediction Engines

More recently, other researchers have focused on a different function for dopamine in the brain's motivational systems. They say that the brain uses dopamine in these regions not so much as a way to spur behavior through wanting but as a signal that predicts which actions or objects will reliably provide a reward. “It encodes the difference between what you're getting and what you have expected,” says neuroscientist Wolfram Schultz, now at the University of Cambridge, who published a seminal paper on what is now known as the “reward prediction error” theory of dopamine in 1997.

In a series of experiments begun in the 1980s, Schultz and his colleagues showed that when monkeys first get something pleasant—in this case, fruit juice—their dopamine neurons fire most intensely when they drink the liquid. But once they learn that a cue like a light or a sound predicts the delivery of delicious stuff, the neurons fire when the cue is perceived, not when the reward is received. This response changes when the value of the reward shifts. If a reward is bigger or better than expected, the dopamine neurons fire more in response to this happy surprise; if it is nonexistent or smaller than anticipated, dopamine levels crash.

In a 2016 study, Schultz and his colleagues asked 27 participants undergoing magnetic resonance imaging to look at a computer screen with a series of rectangles, each representing a “range” of money (for example, £0 to £100), without specific values indicated. A crosshair landed somewhere along a rectangle to indicate a cash prize. In several trials, people would guess at and then (virtually) receive the corresponding amount. Meanwhile the researchers tracked activity in select dopamine hotspots. They found that activity in the substantia nigra and ventral tegmental area, neighboring regions in the midbrain, was linked to people's prediction errors—whether they were pleasantly surprised or disappointed by the prize. In addition, the activity in this area over the course of the experiment related to how well participants adapted their estimates as they gained insight from past mistakes. Schultz therefore sees dopamine as playing a role in how we learn what to seek and what to avoid.

In this view, dopamine does not signify how pleasant an experience will be but how much value it has to the organism at that particular moment. Schultz notes that dopamine neurons do not distinguish among different types of reward. “They're only interested in the value,” he says. “They don't care whether it's food reward or liquid reward or money. They're specific about the prediction error, but they don't care what the reward is.”

Schultz suggests dopamine serves as a common currency system for desire. For example, when the brain receives a signal that the body needs water, the value of water for that individual at that time should rise. Because this makes a cold drink more attractive, quenching thirst will be prioritized, avoiding dehydration. Yet, Schultz explains, “if I fall in love, then all my other rewards become relatively less valuable.” A glass of water will pale in comparison to a chance to be with the beloved.

From this perspective, it is easy to see why dopamine would be critical to addiction. If drugs or other compelling pleasures alter the way the reward system determines what is valuable, the addictive behavior will be given top priority and motivation will shift accordingly. Seeing dopamine this way can also explain a number of psychological phenomena. Consider how people typically prefer a smaller reward now to a bigger one later, what economists term “delay discounting.” This shift occurs because as rewards recede into the distant future, they are far less salient than those that are just about to be received—and are represented by progressively lower amounts of dopamine.

Moreover, if dopamine codes reward prediction error, it could also account for the so-called hedonic treadmill, that sadly universal experience in which what initially makes us ache with desire, over time becomes less alluring, requiring a greater intensity of experience, new degree of novelty or higher dose to achieve the same joy. (You buy a new car, but driving it soon becomes routine and you start to crave a fancier one.) According to reward prediction error theory, when there is no prediction error—when something is just as pleasant as expected, no more or less—dopamine levels do not budge. But your current pleasure may increase expectations for the next experience, Shultz says, “so the prediction error is less high and your reaction is less strong.” (This logic would also confirm the 1965 hypothesis by Mick Jagger et al. regarding the low probability of getting long-term satisfaction.)

None

Longing in the Brain: Signals that originate in dopamine-rich structures in the midbrain, such as the nucleus accumbens, and travel to the prefrontal cortex have long been seen as “pleasure pathways.” More recent work, however, suggests that dopamine stimulates desire or wanting rather than delight. Credit: © iStock.com

Other researchers have begun putting Schultz's ideas to the test. In 2016 in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, neuroscientist Read Montague of Virginia Tech and his colleagues published findings involving 17 people with Parkinson's who had brain implants that could measure changes in dopamine in the striatum, another midbrain area linked to rewarding experiences. They found that dopamine signaling might be even more nuanced than making a simple calculation that compares experience with expectations.

In the experiment, the patients played a game that involved betting on a simulated market. While playing, they considered the possible outcomes of various choices and later evaluated their decisions based on what had actually occurred. Here the dopamine signals that were recorded did not track a simple reward prediction error. Instead they varied by how the bets came in compared with how the investment would have fared if they had chosen differently. In other words, if someone won more than she expected but could have won even more if she had made a different choice, she had less dopamine release than if she had not known there was a way she could do even better.

In addition, if someone lost a few bucks but could have lost a lot more if he had made a different choice, dopamine would rise somewhat. This finding explains why knowing that “it could have been worse” can make what would otherwise feel awful into a positive—or at least less dire—experience.

Putting It All Together

Although some scientists view the reward prediction error theory and Robinson and Berridge's incentive sensitization theory as incompatible, they do not directly falsify each other. Indeed, many experts think that each captures some element of the truth. Dopamine might signal wanting in some neurons or circuits and could signify reward prediction error in others.

Alternatively, these functions may operate on different timescales—as suggested by a 2016 study in rats conducted by scientists at the University of Michigan (colleagues of Berridge and Robinson). The study, published in Nature Neuroscience, found that changes in dopamine levels from second to second were congruent with dopamine as an indicator of value, which supports the reward prediction error hypothesis. Longer-term changes, over the course of minutes, however, were linked with changes in motivation, which bolsters the incentive sensitization theory.

While taking dopamine agonists, Stephanie was certainly aware that her priorities had shifted in uncomfortable ways—but at the time she accepted this because her addictive behavior seemed, as she put it, like “the right thing to do.” Our feelings, in a sense, are decision-making algorithms that evolved to guide behavior toward what was historically most likely to promote survival and reproduction. Pleasure can cue us to repeat activities such as eating and sex; fear drives us away from potential harm. But if the brain regions that determine what you value go askew, it can be extremely difficult to change your behavior because these areas will make you “want” to continue and will also make the addictive behavior “feel” right.

When Stephanie stopped taking Mirapex, all her addictive desires and behavior vanished overnight. In fact, not only did she no longer find her iPhone or buying lottery tickets attractive, they seemed at best of little interest or even actively repellent. She also lost the weight she had gained without much effort. Indeed, Daniel Weintraub, a psychiatrist at the University of Pennsylvania, says that her experience is typical of Parkinson's patients who develop impulse-control disorders while taking dopamine agonists, which is roughly 8 to 17 percent of people taking such drugs. The fact that stopping these drugs can end addictive behavior so abruptly and decisively shows how critical dopamine is in driving it.

A few years later, however, Stephanie's Parkinson's symptoms became worse, and she agreed to try another dopamine agonist. Not long after, she found herself spending hours online, looking at Web sites for people who wanted to have affairs or casual sex. She resisted the urges. “I couldn't do that to my husband,” she explains. “Part of my brain still knew that was wrong.”

Her prior experience with Mirapex also made a difference, she says: “I knew what to look for, which I didn't the first time around,” so she stopped taking the drug. Although dopamine can modulate our drives, it is not the only determinant of what we do and what matters to us—as Stephanie's decisions demonstrate. Ultimately what we humans seek and value is a little more complicated than our fleeting desires.

*Not her real name.